Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 10 de 10
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Biol Psychiatry ; 48(11): 1081-7, 2000 Dec 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11094141

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Nightmares are rare in the sleep laboratory, even in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder for whom nightmare complaints are diagnostic. Nevertheless, it is possible that laboratory conditions do not preclude the observation of telltales-nightmare-related modifications of tonic sleep-given sufficiently large samples. METHODS: Sixty-three unmedicated, nonapneic Vietnam combat veterans undergoing inpatient treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder underwent polysomnographic testing and assessment of nightmare complaint. RESULTS: Trauma-related nightmare complaint, but not non-trauma-related complaint, was associated with increased wake-after-sleep-onset in the sleep laboratory. No relationships between nightmare complaint and rapid eye movement sleep architecture were observed. CONCLUSIONS: Increased wake-after-sleep-onset was specifically associated with trauma-related nightmare complaint, confirming data from other quarters suggesting they are both phenomenologically and functionally distinct from normal dreaming.


Subject(s)
Combat Disorders/physiopathology , Combat Disorders/psychology , Dreams , Sleep Wake Disorders/physiopathology , Sleep Wake Disorders/psychology , Adult , Hospitals, Veterans , Humans , Inpatients , MMPI , Male , Middle Aged , Polysomnography , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/physiopathology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Vietnam , Warfare
2.
Physiol Behav ; 70(1-2): 197-203, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10978496

ABSTRACT

Posttraumatic stress disorder is widely understood to include "persistent symptoms of increased arousal." This presumption has rarely been tested under conditions in which effects of anticipatory anxiety could be ruled out. In this study, heart rate and electroencephalogram spectral power were assessed during sleep, a state free of most sources of artifact contaminating indices of tonic arousal. Fifty-six unmedicated nonapneic Vietnam combat-related inpatients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and 14 controls spent 3 or more nights in the sleep laboratory during which their electrocardiograms and electroencephalograms were continuously recorded. Heart rate and electroencephalogram spectral power were quantified continuously off-line and averaged by sleep stage over all postadaptational nights. Sleep heart rate exhibited no group differences and no covariation with the severity of subjective hyperarousal reported by PTSD patients. PTSD patients exhibited a trend toward reduced low-frequency electroencephalogram spectral power during nonrapid-eye-movement (NREM) sleep. This reduction was significant during slow-wave sleep in those subjects producing scoreable slow-wave sleep. The relationship of rapid-eye-movement (REM) beta-band power to NREM beta-band power was different in PTSD patients and controls, with the patients exhibiting more beta in REM versus NREM sleep than controls. In patients, NREM sleep sigma-band electroencephalogram spectral power exhibited a positive correlation with subjective hyperarousal. Finally, a novel and surprisingly strong inverse correlation between REM-NREM sleep heart rate difference and REM percent of sleep was observed in PTSD patients only. In summary, peripheral and central measures of tonic arousal during sleep demonstrated contrastive relations to PTSD diagnostic and symptom status. The data suggest that more consideration should be directed to mechanisms of central arousal in PTSD.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Sleep/physiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Adult , Electroencephalography , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Polysomnography , Psychometrics , Sleep, REM/physiology , Veterans
3.
Integr Physiol Behav Sci ; 32(1): 19-30, 1997.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9105911

ABSTRACT

The objective of this study was to assess, via heart rate, the arousal levels of participants in group trauma reexposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder, and so to better understand this common mode of treatment, particularly in regards to its presumed curative factor, extinction. Six Vietnam combat-related PTSD inpatients participated twice weekly in group trauma reexposure therapy during which their electrocardiograms were recorded. Heart rate was quantified continuously off-line. Heart rates of participants not directly engaged in imaginal reexposure to their personal combat traumas consistently exhibited mild linear declines from the beginnings to the ends of the approximately 2.5 hour sessions. Participants actively engaged in personal combat trauma reexposure exhibited higher whole-session heart rates. Most also exhibited more specific elevation extending over the later portions of therapy sessions during which intensive reexposure usually occurred. Surprisingly, no patients exhibited focal increases in heart rate concurrent with periods of most intensive traumatic incident review as judged from videotape. Administering imaginal reexposure in a group context does not preclude substantial physiological (sympathetic) arousal, as is preconditional for extinction. Under conditions in which the actively engaged reexposure patient is reliably identified, group trauma reexposure therapy may not provide an opportunity for "vicarious" flooding in nonengaged participants.


Subject(s)
Heart Rate/physiology , Psychotherapy, Group , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/physiopathology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/therapy , Adult , Electrocardiography , Extinction, Psychological , Humans , Male , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Veterans
4.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol ; 99(3): 235-46, 1996 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8862113

ABSTRACT

The status of semantic priming in Alzheimer's disease (AD) was examined using the speech elicited N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Speech was naturally paced, with 1 s of silence before the final word. In the semantic task, subjects attended to the meaning of the sentences for a subsequent memory test. In the phonemic monitoring task, they counted the words beginning with the letter 'p'. The effects of age were assessed by comparing young and elderly, and the effects of disease by comparing elderly and AD subjects. In healthy young and elderly subjects, N400s were large to semantically unprimed words and small to semantically primed words. In AD subjects, N400s were large to primed words, reflecting a failure of the sentence stem to prime the final word, and probably an impairment in semantic knowledge. The N400 priming effect was not smaller during the phonemic than semantic task in any group, suggesting that the semantic qualities of speech are processed even when subjects are attending to phonemic qualities. N400 latency was delayed with age and further delayed with dementia.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Evoked Potentials , Speech Perception/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Memory/physiology , Middle Aged , Scalp/physiopathology , Semantics
5.
Sleep ; 19(4): 312-7, 1996 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8776789

ABSTRACT

Recent data have suggested that first night effects are attenuated in inpatient depressive subjects. We examined first night effects in 80 inpatients hospitalized for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) as well as non-hospitalized PTSD sufferers and non-ill control subjects. PTSD inpatients exhibited attenuated first night effects compared to non-hospitalized PTSD sufferers and non-trauma-exposed controls. Non-ill combat-exposed subjects also exhibited small first night effects. Within the inpatient sample, severity indices of PTSD, depression and anxiety failed to account for variance in first night effects. These data demonstrate attenuation of first night effects in a new inpatient population and suggest their statistical independence vis-a-vis a range of relevant symptoms. Both the attenuation of first night effects in PTSD inpatients and their accentuation in PTSD outpatients may be indicative of enhanced sensitivity to the sleep environment. Conversely, the trend to small first night effects in non-ill combat-exposed subjects may reflect a dimension of their apparent resistance to traumatic stress.


Subject(s)
Sleep, REM , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Depressive Disorder/rehabilitation , Hospitalization , Humans , Sleep Stages
6.
Biol Psychiatry ; 39(3): 182-92, 1996 Feb 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8837979

ABSTRACT

The sleep of 27 unmedicated Vietnam combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) inpatients was monitored for 3 nights. Depressive comorbidity was considered both as a diagnostic category using DMS-III-R criteria, and as a continuous variable using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). Data collected included sleep architecture features that have discriminated unipolar depressives from controls in many prior studies, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep latency, and slow-wave sleep time, as well as two additional indices that have sometimes discriminated depressives from controls in waking studies-baseline heart rate and facial electromyography. Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-III-R (SCID)-diagnosed PTSD+major depressive disorder (MDD) patients failed to exhibit shorter REM latencies, greater REM percents of sleep, or greater REM densities than PTSD-MDD patients, but did exhibit less slow wave sleep. PTSD+MDD patients also exhibited less facial (mentalis) electromyographic activity. REM densities and baseline heart rates were equivocal. REM density, baseline heart rate, and mentalis electromyography all correlated with the BDI, the former two positively, the last, negatively. In summary, SCID-diagnosed PTSD+MDD patients failed to exhibit the classic REM sleep architectural modifications associated with unipolar depression, despite the fact that several other psychophysiologic indices of dysphoria were detectable in their sleep.


Subject(s)
Combat Disorders/diagnosis , Depressive Disorder/diagnosis , Polysomnography , Sleep Wake Disorders/diagnosis , Veterans/psychology , Adult , Combat Disorders/psychology , Comorbidity , Depressive Disorder/psychology , Electromyography , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Reaction Time , Sleep Wake Disorders/psychology , Sleep, REM , Vietnam
7.
J Trauma Stress ; 9(1): 137-43, 1996 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8750457

ABSTRACT

Twenty-five Vietnam combat veterans with chronic severe posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) completed a sleep self-report questionnaire on admission to an inpatient treatment program. Between 1 and 2 months later each spent 3 or more nights in the sleep laboratory. When self-report and laboratory findings were compared, significant relationships were observed between sleep schedule items such as time-to-bed/time-out-of-bed and polysomnographic measures of sleep. In contrast, global ratings of sleep quality were generally unrelated to polysomnographic measures. These findings may have implications for survey research assessing sleep quality in traumatized populations.


Subject(s)
Sleep , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/rehabilitation , Veterans/psychology , Chronic Disease , Hospitalization , Humans , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Vietnam , Warfare
8.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol ; 87(5): 306-20, 1993 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7693442

ABSTRACT

Event-related potentials (ERPs) to spoken sentences were recorded from young and elderly subjects. Sentences ended in semantically typical or atypical words. Subjects listened to the sentences during two attention conditions. In a semantic attention task, subjects listened to the meanings of the sentences for a subsequent memory test. In a phonemic attention task, subjects counted words beginning with the letter "p". Following these tasks, the sentence-final words were presented in isolation. A conventional auditory oddball paradigm was also presented. The N4 components elicited by semantically atypical sentence-final words were slightly attenuated and delayed in the older group. N4 was not influenced by the attention manipulation. Young but not older subjects' N4s were attenuated by repetition. When the sentence-final words were presented a third time in isolation, the N4s elicited were similar in amplitude and latency to those elicited in sentence context, but were not attenuated by repetition. In young and older subjects, the peak amplitude of the sentence-elicited N4 difference wave form was highly correlated with the number of correct recognitions of typical words, but not atypical sentence-final words. In young but not older subjects, sentence-elicited N4 amplitude was correlated with the amplitude of the P3 elicited in the auditory oddball paradigm.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Brain/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Attention/physiology , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Memory/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Speech Perception/physiology
9.
Psychophysiology ; 28(6): 609-18, 1991 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1816588

ABSTRACT

An elementary neural model of the P3 is proposed in which the P3 is held to manifest a brief, widely-distributed, inhibitory event. A preliminary and indirect test of the model is described using secondary-task methodology. Manual reaction times were measured to probe clicks delivered during the presumed time-course of an auditory oddball P3. We observed that reaction times to probes presented after oddball stimuli were significantly slowed as compared to reaction times to probes presented after standards. The latency of maximum reaction time slowing corresponded generally to the latency of the P3. The latency of maximum reaction time slowing did not respond to a manipulation varying the latency of the P3. Thus, some of the obtained results were consistent with the P3-inhibition hypothesis, whereas others were not. Secondary-task methodology may provide a valuable new approach to understanding the late event-related potentials.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Loudness Perception/physiology , Pitch Discrimination/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adult , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Female , Humans , Male
10.
Brain Lang ; 38(4): 488-503, 1990 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2375978

ABSTRACT

Data were collected shedding light on the brain electrical activity underlying word recognition. Subjects listened to a list of 48 spoken words in six random orders under two instructional sets: first to "think about the meanings of the words," and second, to learn the list. The scalp EEG associated with hearing and identifying the words was recorded at F3, F4, Cz, P3, P4, Pz, and Oz. Standard within-subjects time-locked averaging across words showed a late negative-positive complex with N2-P3 topography, the negative component peaking around 480 msec, the positive component peaking around 830 msec. Averaging within words across subjects uncovered considerable latency variability in both components. Within-word N2 and P3 component latencies covaried with word durations and with the "recognition points" predicted for the words by the "cohort theory" of word recognition. N2 latencies corresponded closely to the "N400" effect elicited with semantically incongruous sentence-final spoken words. Implications for ERP investigations of language processing are discussed.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Arousal/physiology , Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Electroencephalography , Reaction Time/physiology , Speech Perception/physiology , Adult , Aged , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Phonetics , Reference Values , Semantics , Sound Spectrography
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...