Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
1.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 37(5): 866-895, 2023 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36594199

ABSTRACT

Objective: Japanese-Americans are the sixth largest Asian ethnicity in the United States and represent a highly heterogeneous population. Despite representing a large and diverse group, relatively little attention has been paid to clinical best practices for working with this population in the West, particularly for Japanese speakers and issei (first-generation in the United States). This paper offers guidance for providing competent neuropsychological services to Japanese-Americans. Method: Pertinent facets of Japanese culture are presented within the context of the ECLECTIC framework (education and literacy, culture and acculturation, language, economics, communication, testing situation: comfort and motivation, intelligence conceptualization, and context of immigration; Fujii, 2018). The available literature on clinical neuropsychological tests that are translated into Japanese and normed with Japanese samples was reviewed. Results: Specific recommendations for clinicians providing neuropsychological services to Japanese-Americans are presented with an aim of maximizing test fairness by addressing the following issues: comfort with the testing situation, test biases, accessibility, and validity (American Educational Research Association et al., 2014). Additional recommendations for the use of teleneuropsychology; working with geriatric, pediatric, and multiracial populations; and providing useful recommendations and feedback from clinical assessment are provided. Measures that are appropriately translated and/or adapted for use with Japanese populations are presented by cognitive domain to assist clinicians with test selection. Conclusions: This paper provides concrete recommendations for Western neuropsychologists working with patients of Japanese descent in order to address the current gap in cultural competence among clinicians when working with this heterogeneous population.


Subject(s)
Ethnicity , Neuropsychology , United States , Humans , Child , Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Language , Linguistics
2.
Appl Neuropsychol Adult ; : 1-9, 2022 Jun 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35696557

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: The present study examined the efficacy of a CogSMART-based program in improving cognitive and emotional functioning in a clinic-based sample of Veterans presenting with cognitive concerns and history of mental health diagnoses. METHOD: Forty Veterans (Mage = 61.2 years, 85% male) completed a weekly CogSMART-based group program as well as a battery of neuropsychological and psychological measures at both pre- and post-group evaluations. Participants met DSM-5 criteria for at least one mental health diagnosis. RESULTS: Significant improvements on global cognition as well as measures of learning/memory and attention were observed from pre- to post-group (p < .05, cohen's d range = .48-1.01). As many as 33.3% of participants showed significant improvement, depending on the cognitive domain. Significant overall improvements were observed in depression symptoms and life satisfaction (p < .01, cohen's d = .67 and .59, respectively). Over one-third of the sample demonstrated a reliable improvement in depressive symptoms, 25% in anxiety symptoms, and 18% in life satisfaction. CONCLUSIONS: Among individuals with mental health diagnosis but without major neurocognitive disorders, CogSMART-based interventions may be an effective treatment for improving aspects of cognition, depression, and life satisfaction.

3.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry ; 30(6): 587-94, 2015 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25176562

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The term "chemobrain" is sometimes used to denote deficits in neuropsychological functioning that may occur as a result of cancer treatment. As breast cancer survivors now commonly reach late life, it is not known whether previous exposure to chemotherapy may affect long-term risk for cognitive impairment. To help address this concern, this study tested whether successfully surviving chemotherapy earlier in life was associated with later differences in brain metabolic function as an older adult compared to controls. This question was examined using positron emission tomography measures of brain glucose metabolism in elderly women cancer survivors. METHODS: Breast cancer survivors (N = 10), currently free of recurrent cancer and without a diagnosis of a cognitive disorder, were compared to matched healthy controls (N = 10). All subjects were imaged at rest with [(18)F]fluorodeoxyglucose. Images were analyzed semi-quantitatively using the Alzheimer's Discrimination Tool and a volume of interest-based approach derived from co-registered magnetic resonance imaging. RESULTS: Relative [(18)F]fluorodeoxyglucose uptake (normalized to global) was significantly lower in the survivors compared with control subjects in bilateral orbital frontal regions, consistent with differences between the groups in cognition and executive function (i.e., Trail Making Test, Part B and mini-mental state examination) and despite no significant differences with respect to age, education, intelligence, or working memory. None of the survivors and only one control manifested a global positron emission tomography score consistent with an Alzheimer's disease metabolic pattern. CONCLUSION: Breast cancer survivors treated with chemotherapy may manifest long-term changes in brain glucose metabolism indicative of subtle frontal hypometabolism, a finding consistent with results from neuropsychological testing and other imaging modalities.


Subject(s)
Antineoplastic Agents/adverse effects , Breast Neoplasms/drug therapy , Frontal Lobe/metabolism , Glucose/metabolism , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cognition/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Female , Fluorodeoxyglucose F18 , Frontal Lobe/drug effects , Humans , Neuropsychological Tests , Pilot Projects , Positron-Emission Tomography/methods , Radiopharmaceuticals , Survivors
4.
J Psychosoc Oncol ; 32(3): 342-58, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24611890

ABSTRACT

Posttraumatic growth has been demonstrated to occur following the diagnosis and treatment of cancer. Still unknown is whether patients expect such growth, how growth is perceived at early points in time that follow the cancer experience, and whether patient reports of growth are corroborated by others. Participants were 87 patients and 55 collaterals who reported their anticipation of growth pretreatment and their perceived growth at a 9-month follow-up. Patients' expectations for their own growth were significantly higher than collaterals' expectations for theirs. When anticipated growth was compared to later reported growth, patients overanticipated growth across all domains and collaterals underanticipated growth.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Anticipation, Psychological , Attitude to Health , Neoplasms/psychology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Adult , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Friends/psychology , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Middle Aged , Neoplasms/therapy , Parents/psychology , Patients/psychology , Patients/statistics & numerical data , Qualitative Research , Siblings/psychology , Spouses/psychology , Spouses/statistics & numerical data
5.
Psychooncology ; 22(4): 862-8, 2013 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22585465

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The present study examined the long-term cognitive implications of cancer treatment among breast cancer survivors aged 65 years and older to better understand the long term implications of cancer treatment. METHODS: Fifty-seven women survivors were compared with 30 healthy older female adult comparisons, matched in terms of age and education, with no history of cancer. Cancer survivors were also compared on the basis of treatment intervention, involving chemotherapy (n = 27) versus local therapy through surgery and radiation (n = 30). RESULTS: As a group, the breast cancer survivors scored lower on measures of general cognitive function, working memory, psychomotor speed, and executive function when compared with the normal comparisons. Among the cancer survivors, those who received local therapy scored lower than the other survivors and normal comparisons on measures of verbal learning, visual perception and construction, as well as visual attention and short-term retention. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings suggest that cognitive outcomes may involve greater age-related deficits among older cancer survivors compared with matched healthy subjects.


Subject(s)
Breast Neoplasms/psychology , Breast Neoplasms/therapy , Cognition , Survivors/psychology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Analysis of Variance , Antineoplastic Agents/adverse effects , Attention , Case-Control Studies , Disease-Free Survival , Executive Function , Female , Humans , Mastectomy , Memory, Short-Term , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychomotor Performance , Radiotherapy/adverse effects , Time Factors , Treatment Outcome , Verbal Learning
6.
J Cogn Psychol (Hove) ; 23(6): 748-759, 2011 Sep 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23493494

ABSTRACT

The Ultimatum Game (UG) is a widely used and well-studied laboratory model of economic decision-making. Here, we studied 129 healthy adults and compared demographic (i.e., age, gender, education), cognitive (i.e., intelligence, attention/working memory, speed, language, visuospatial, memory, executive functions), and personality (i.e., "Big Five", positive affect, negative affect) variables between those with a "rational" versus an "irrational" response pattern on the UG. Our data indicated that participants with "rational" UG performance (accepting any offer, no matter the fairness) endorsed higher levels of trust, or the belief in the sincerity and good intentions of others, while participants with "irrational" UG performance (rejecting unfair offers) endorsed higher levels of negative affect, such as anger and contempt. These personality variables were the only ones that differentiated the two response patterns-demographic and cognitive factors did not differ between rational and irrational players. The results indicate that the examination of personality and affect is crucial to our understanding of the individual differences that underlie decision-making.

7.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci ; 22(1): 48-54, 2010.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20160209

ABSTRACT

The authors examined the long-term cognitive implications of cancer treatment among breast cancer survivors over 65 years old who received treatment during midlife. Thirty women survivors were matched with 30 noncancer, healthy older adults in terms of age, education, and IQ. The cancer survivors scored significantly lower in the cognitive domains of executive functioning, working memory, and divided attention, reflecting potential dysfunction in frontal-subcortical brain regions. Our findings suggest that among breast cancer survivors who remain disease-free for more than a decade, the previous cancer treatment may further augment cognitive dysfunction associated with age-related brain changes.


Subject(s)
Antineoplastic Combined Chemotherapy Protocols/therapeutic use , Breast Neoplasms/drug therapy , Breast Neoplasms/epidemiology , Cognition Disorders/epidemiology , Age Factors , Aged , Breast Neoplasms/pathology , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Disease-Free Survival , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neoplasm Staging , Neuropsychological Tests , Severity of Illness Index
8.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 1121: 480-98, 2007 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17872394

ABSTRACT

The present series of three studies aims at investigating the hypothesis that some seemingly normal older persons have deficits in reasoning and decision making due to dysfunction in a neural system which includes the ventromedial prefrontal cortices. This hypothesis is relevant to the comprehensive study of aging, and also addresses the question of why so many older adults fall prey to fraud. To our knowledge, this work represents the first of its kind to begin to identify, from an individual-differences perspective, the behavioral, psychophysiological, and consumer correlates of defective decision making among healthy older adults. Our findings, in a cross-sectional sample of community-dwelling participants, demonstrate that a sizeable subset of older adults (approximately 35-40%) perform disadvantageously on a laboratory measure of decision making that closely mimics everyday life, by the manner in which it factors in reward, punishment, risk, and ambiguity. These same poor decision makers display defective autonomic responses (or somatic markers), reminiscent of that previously established in patients with acquired prefrontal lesions. Finally, we present data demonstrating that poor decision makers are more likely to fall prey to deceptive advertising, suggesting compromise of real-world judgment and decision-making abilities.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Decision Making , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Behavior , Brain Diseases/physiopathology , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...