Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 16 de 16
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
2.
Nano Lett ; 10(8): 3090-5, 2010 Aug 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20698622

ABSTRACT

The optical properties of a nanoparticle dimer bridged by a conductive junction depend strongly on the junction conductivity. As the conductivity increases, the bonding dimer plasmon blueshifts and broadens. For large conductance, a low energy charge transfer plasmon also appears in the spectra with a line width that decreases with increasing conductance. A simple physical model for the understanding of the spectral feature is presented. Our finding of a strong influence of junction conductivity on the optical spectrum suggests that plasmonic cavities might serve as probes of molecular conductance at elevated frequencies not accessible through electrical measurements.

4.
Phys Rev Lett ; 91(14): 143902, 2003 Oct 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14611524

ABSTRACT

Electron energy-loss spectroscopy (EELS) is shown to be an excellent source of information both on photonic crystal bands and on radiation modes of complex nanostructures. Good agreement is reported between measurements and parameter-free calculations of EELS in porous alumina films, where Cherenkov radiation is scattered by the pores to yield a strong 8.3-eV (7-eV) feature for 120-keV (200-keV) electrons. The latter is related to the bands of two-dimensional photonic crystals formed by air cylinders in an alumina matrix with similar near-range ordering. Finally, the band structure is proved to be directly mapped by angle-resolved EELS.

5.
Scanning Microsc ; 9(4): 927-36; discussion 937-8, 1995.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8819881

ABSTRACT

In the frame of the Self-Energy formalism, we study the interaction between STEM electrons and small particles in the range of the valence electron excitations. We first calculate the energy loss probability for an isolated sphere and study the loss spectrum dependence on the size of the particle and on the relative impact parameter. Then we analyze the loss spectra in more realistic situations: (a) the effect of the coupling between the particle and supporting surface is studied in a simple geometrical model; and (b) we analyze the dependence of the losses on the geometrical shape of the target by considering hemispherical particle. Our results are in a good qualitative (and in simple cases, quantitative too) agreement with several experimental results which show anomalous excitations. We restate the suitability of the dielectric theory to study the surface excitations of these systems.


Subject(s)
Microscopy, Electron, Scanning Transmission
6.
Phys Rev B Condens Matter ; 48(19): 14534-14542, 1993 Nov 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10007876
8.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav ; 43(1): 205-14, 1992 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1409806

ABSTRACT

We followed the titer of free amino acids in nervous ganglia and hemolymph of the cricket Pteronemobius sp. at different times during and after a shock avoidance training that included one experimental group and three controls. The results showed that Tau, urea, Thr, His, GABA, and an unidentified compound (Q) increased their titer in ganglia and hemolymph during training, whereas Ala, Arg, Val, Glu, Ser, and one or all of the group formed by Cys, Phe, Ile, Leu, and Trp decreased theirs concomitantly to memory consolidation. The difference in the rate of experimental insects and their yoked slaves to consolidate the learned task was reflected in the changes of the titers of the amino acids mentioned above. The data add to the evidence for a direct involvement of these amino acids in modulating the memory consolidation process.


Subject(s)
Amino Acids/metabolism , Avoidance Learning/physiology , Central Nervous System/metabolism , Gryllidae/physiology , Memory/physiology , Animals , Electroshock , Female , Hemolymph/metabolism , Physical Stimulation
9.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav ; 40(4): 887-91, 1991 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1816576

ABSTRACT

The escape reaction time (ERT) of the cricket Pteronemobius sp. from the heated box begins at 48 degrees and increases with temperature until 56 degrees C, beyond which there is no further increase. The ERT (2.2 +/- 1.39 s) from the hot box at 54 degrees C is used as a model for studying the analgesic effects of opiates. Results of the present paper show that the ERT did not change after injecting the insect in the abdominal haemocoel with 0.9% saline solution, but ERT increased when 0.32, 0.52 or 0.69 mg/g of morphine is injected in the same place. The maximum ERT increase is reached at 90 min after drug injection, and the drug effect disappears 3 h after the injection. At 90 min after drug injections, the dose of 0.50 mg/g of morphine produces 50% of ERT increase, and it is referred to as the median analgesic dose (D50). 1.05 mg/g of morphine produces an ERT longer than 30 s that results in an irreversible damage to the insect. Sixty-four micrograms/g of naloxone given in addition to D50 of morphine fully blocked the effect of morphine during its 3-h action. However, more than 64 micrograms/g of naloxone alone also increase the ERT in the cricket, similar to what has been described for vertebrates. Four daily morphine injections of D50 decreased ERT in such a way that, at the fourth day, the ERT is similar to the ERT produced by saline solution; i.e., tolerance is shown. The suppression of daily morphine injections of D50 during the fifth day produced a hyperresponse to vibration (big jumps) not shown in the case of the injections of saline solution; i.e., addiction is shown.


Subject(s)
Analgesia , Gryllidae , Morphine/pharmacology , Animals , Drug Tolerance , Escape Reaction/drug effects , Hot Temperature , Naloxone/pharmacology , Nociceptors/drug effects , Substance-Related Disorders
10.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav ; 35(1): 127-31, 1990 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2315350

ABSTRACT

The involvement of certain amino acids in the memory consolidation process was investigated in the cricket Pteronemobius sp. Thirsty crickets were trained to constantly turn to one side of a symmetrical Y-shaped maze using reinforcement with water. Controls were trained to turn to both sides of the maze according to a random program. Animals were sacrificed immediately after training and free amino acid fractions were isolated from whole brain, subesophagic, prothoracic, mesothoracic and metathoracic ganglia homogenates and analyzed by high pressure liquid chromatography. A complex pattern of variation in the titer of amino acids emerged after learning, where the changes differed among the various ganglia. The most conspicuous change was an increase in the levels of urea and an amino acid-like compound related to the urea cycle, in all ganglia except the subesophagic one, if compared to controls. Arginine increased in the subesophagic ganglion, but decreased significantly in the metathoracic ganglion. The variation of ganglionic amino acid levels and its possible relation to mnemonic processes is discussed.


Subject(s)
Amino Acids/metabolism , Conditioning, Operant/physiology , Ganglia/metabolism , Gryllidae/metabolism , Memory/physiology , Orthoptera/metabolism , Animals , Female , Gryllidae/physiology
11.
Acta Cient Venez ; 41(3): 186-90, 1990.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2152377

ABSTRACT

Learning and memory consolidation was investigated in the mantis Stagmatoptera biocellata, by two successive trainings: a) the attack (A) avoidance training in which animals were not allowed to catch a mobile star, followed by the deimatic reaction (DR) avoidance training of not displaying this defensive response, or b) DR-avoidance training followed by A-avoidance training. The results showed that the presence and the 1 of A-avoidance learning and DR-avoidance learning did not affect the learning of the other training. Total retention of A-avoidance memory was demonstrated when A-avoidance training was given first, whereas DR-avoidance memory was significantly lost (716%) when the DR-avoidance training was given first. The retroactive forgetting of DR-avoidance memory is considered as retrograde amnesia rather than retroactive memory inhibition because it depends on the activity during A-avoidance training (number of attacks) and not the quantity of A-avoidance learning. The magnitude of the amnesic effect due to this distracting activity in the A-avoidance training appears to be remarkably similar to that reported in a previous work using nitrogen-induced anoxia as the amnesic factor.


Subject(s)
Avoidance Learning/physiology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Memory/physiology , Orthoptera/physiology , Animals
12.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav ; 35(1): 133-6, 1990 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2107553

ABSTRACT

The effect of injections of selected amino acids on memory, given before a maze-learning, was investigated. Thirsty crickets (Pteronemobius sp.) were trained to turn only to one side of a symmetrical Y-shaped maze using reinforcements with water. The insects retained the learned task 24 hr later. N2 anoxia applied immediately after training produced retrograde amnesia. Injections of Ala, Arg, Gln or morphine prior to training blocked the amnesic action of anoxia, whereas those of Cys, Met, Pro, Orn, octopamine or naloxone did not. Naloxone blocked long-term memory formation, but not learning, whereas Pro and Orn blocked both. The antiamnesic effect of morphine and Arg, but not that of Ala, was blocked by naloxone. A hypothesis assigning a neuromodulatory role to some amino acids is put forward.


Subject(s)
Amino Acids/pharmacology , Gryllidae/physiology , Memory/drug effects , Narcotics/pharmacology , Orthoptera/physiology , Animals , Conditioning, Operant/drug effects , Drinking Behavior/drug effects , Female , Morphine/pharmacology , Naloxone/pharmacology , Octopamine/pharmacology , Retention, Psychology/drug effects
13.
Acta cient. venez ; 41(3): 186-90, 1990. tab
Article in English | LILACS | ID: lil-97611

ABSTRACT

Learning and memory consolidation was investigated in the mantis Stagmatoptera biocellata, by two succesive trainings: a) the attak (A) avoidance training in which animals were not allowed to catch a mobile star, fallowed by the deimatic reaction (DR) avoidance training of not displaying this defensive response, or b) DR-avoidance training followed by A-avoidance training. The results showed that the presence and the 1 of A-avoidance learning and DR-avoidance learning did not affect the learning of the other training. Total retention of A-avoidance memory was demostrated when A-avoidance training was given first, whereas DR-avoidance memory was significantly lost (716%) when the DR-avoidance training was given first. The retroactive forgetting of DR-avoidance memory is considered as retrograde amnesia rather than retroactive memory inhibition because it depends on the activity during A-avoidance training (number of attacks) and not the quantity of A-avoidance learning. The magnitude of the amnesia effect due to this distracting activity in the A-avoidance training appears to be remarkably similar to that reported in a previous work using nitrogen-induced anoxia as the amnesia factor


Subject(s)
Animals , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Memory/physiology , Orthoptera/physiology
16.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav ; 20(5): 683-7, 1984 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6330763

ABSTRACT

A praying mantis displays a "frightening reaction" called deimatic reaction (DR), any time that it is faced with a patterned visual stimulus that represents a potential damage for the insect. Results of the present paper show that the DR could be also elicited by an actual noxious (an electrical shock) and that this response is similar to that elicited by a potential nociceptive stimulus (a patterned visual stimulus). The DR elicited by the electric shock was used as a model for studying the analgesic effect of opiates. The mantis was placed in an apparatus that allowed us to give the insect an electrical shock and to measure the strength of its DR. During a first session the voltage threshold necessary to induce a full DR was determined, and then, the insect was injected with a certain solution. The voltage threshold was tested one, two and four hours after injection. Mantises that were injected with only distilled water showed no changes in their voltage threshold during the three tests. Injections of 300, 350 and 400 micrograms/g of morphine-HCl increased the voltage threshold in both a time-dependent and a dose related manner. A dose of 350 micrograms/g of morphine-HCl produced 50% of response inhibition after two hours of injections and is referred to as the median antinoxious dose ( AD50 ). Sixteen micrograms/g of naloxone given in conjunction with an AD50 of morphine, partially blocked the effect of morphine during the first hour and fully blocked it during the second hour. Thirty-two micrograms/g of naloxone fully blocked the morphine effect during the first and the second hour. However, more than 48 micrograms/g of naloxone alone also increased the voltage threshold in insects, similar to those described for vertebrates.


Subject(s)
Morphine/pharmacology , Naloxone/pharmacology , Orthoptera/metabolism , Receptors, Opioid/drug effects , Animals , Behavior, Animal/drug effects , Electric Stimulation , Female , Morphine/antagonists & inhibitors , Time Factors , Vision, Ocular/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...