Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 38
Filter
1.
J Nutr Health Aging ; 26(6): 637-651, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35718874

ABSTRACT

Sarcopenia and frailty are highly prevalent conditions in older hospitalized patients, which are associated with a myriad of adverse clinical outcomes. This paper, prepared by a multidisciplinary expert working group from the Australian and New Zealand Society for Sarcopenia and Frailty Research (ANZSSFR), provides an up-to-date overview of current evidence and recommendations based on a narrative review of the literature for the screening, diagnosis, and management of sarcopenia and frailty in older patients within the hospital setting. It also includes suggestions on potential pathways to implement change to encourage widespread adoption of these evidence-informed recommendations within hospital settings. The expert working group concluded there was insufficient evidence to support any specific screening tool for sarcopenia and recommends an assessment of probable sarcopenia/sarcopenia using established criteria for all older (≥65 years) hospitalized patients or in younger patients with conditions (e.g., comorbidities) that may increase their risk of sarcopenia. Diagnosis of probable sarcopenia should be based on an assessment of low muscle strength (grip strength or five times sit-to-stand) with sarcopenia diagnosis including low muscle mass quantified from dual energy X-ray absorptiometry, bioelectrical impedance analysis or in the absence of diagnostic devices, calf circumference as a proxy measure. Severe sarcopenia is represented by the addition of impaired physical performance (slow gait speed). All patients with probable sarcopenia or sarcopenia should be investigated for causes (e.g., chronic/acute disease or malnutrition), and treated accordingly. For frailty, it is recommended that all hospitalized patients aged 70 years and older be screened using a validated tool [Clinical Frailty Scale (CFS), Hospital Frailty Risk Score, the FRAIL scale or the Frailty Index]. Patients screened as positive for frailty should undergo further clinical assessment using the Frailty Phenotype, Frailty Index or information collected from a Comprehensive Geriatric Assessment (CGA). All patients identified as frail should receive follow up by a health practitioner(s) for an individualized care plan. To treat older hospitalized patients with probable sarcopenia, sarcopenia, or frailty, it is recommended that a structured and supervised multi-component exercise program incorporating elements of resistance (muscle strengthening), challenging balance, and functional mobility training be prescribed as early as possible combined with nutritional support to optimize energy and protein intake and correct any deficiencies. There is insufficient evidence to recommend pharmacological agents for the treatment of sarcopenia or frailty. Finally, to facilitate integration of these recommendations into hospital settings organization-wide approaches are needed, with the Spread and Sustain framework recommended to facilitate organizational culture change, with the help of 'champions' to drive these changes. A multidisciplinary team approach incorporating awareness and education initiatives for healthcare professionals is recommended to ensure that screening, diagnosis and management approaches for sarcopenia and frailty are embedded and sustained within hospital settings. Finally, patients and caregivers' education should be integrated into the care pathway to facilitate adherence to prescribed management approaches for sarcopenia and frailty.


Subject(s)
Frailty , Sarcopenia , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Australia , Frail Elderly , Frailty/diagnosis , Frailty/therapy , Geriatric Assessment , Hand Strength/physiology , Humans , New Zealand , Sarcopenia/diagnosis , Sarcopenia/therapy
2.
J Nutr Health Aging ; 23(2): 145-150, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30697623

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Beta-hydroxy-beta-methylbutyrate (HMB) has been shown to be effective and superior to other types of protein supplements to attenuate loss of muscle mass, strength and function, however, its benefits in sarcopenic and frail older people remain unclear. OBJECTIVE: We seek to determine the effect of HMB on muscle mass, strength and function in older people with sarcopenia or frailty by reviewing results from available randomized controlled trials (RCTs). DESIGN: This review was registered at PROSPERO (University of York) with registration number CRD42018088462 and conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines. Using a pre-determined e-search strategy, we searched PubMed, Medline, EMBASE, CINAHL, LILACS, Web of Science, Cochrane and Scopus databases. Our inclusion criteria were RCTs that assessed the effect of HMB on muscle mass, strength and function in older people with sarcopenia and frailty aged ≥60 years. The main outcomes were lean body mass, handgrip, leg press strength, and Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB) score. RESULTS: Three studies matched our eligibility criteria which enrolled 203 subjects through a variety of definitions of sarcopenia or frailty. Lean body mass increased and muscle strength and function were preserved following HMB supplementation. CONCLUSION: HMB improves lean muscle mass and preserves muscle strength and function in older people with sarcopenia or frailty.


Subject(s)
Frailty/drug therapy , Hand Strength/physiology , Sarcopenia/drug therapy , Sarcopenia/physiopathology , Valerates/therapeutic use , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Body Composition , Dietary Supplements , Female , Humans , Male , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic
3.
J Nutr Health Aging ; 23(1): 105-110, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30569078

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Globally there are several operational definitions for sarcopenia, complicating clinical and research applications. OBJECTIVE: The objective of the Australian and New Zealand Society for Sarcopenia and Frailty Research (ANZSSFR) Task Force on Diagnostic Criteria for Sarcopenia was to reach consensus on the operational definition of sarcopenia for regional use by clinicians and researchers. METHOD: A four-Phase modified Delphi process was undertaken in which 24 individuals with expertise or a recognised interest in sarcopenia from different fields across Australia and New Zealand were invited to be Task Force members. An initial face-to-face meeting was held in Adelaide, South Australia, in November 2017, followed by two subsequent online Phases conducted by electronic surveys. A final Phase was used to approve the final statements. Responses were analysed using a pre-specified strategy. The level of agreement required for consensus was 80%. RESULTS: In Phase 2, 94.1% of Task Force respondents voted in favour of adopting an existing operational definition of sarcopenia. In Phase 3, 94.4% of respondents voted in favour of adopting the European Working Group on Sarcopenia in Older People (EWGSOP) definition as the operational definition for sarcopenia in Australia and New Zealand. CONCLUSION: With consensus achieved, the ANZSSFR will adopt, promote and validate the EWGSOP operational definition of sarcopenia for use by clinicians and researchers in Australia and New Zealand.


Subject(s)
Sarcopenia/diagnosis , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Australia , Consensus , Female , Humans , Male , New Zealand , Surveys and Questionnaires
4.
Network ; 16(4): 357-76, 2005 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16611590

ABSTRACT

The movement of an observer generates a characteristic field of velocity vectors on the retina (Gibson 1950). Because such optic flow-fields are useful for navigation, many theoretical, psychophysical and physiological studies have addressed the question how ego-motion parameters such as direction of heading can be estimated from optic flow. Little is known, however, about the structure of optic flow under natural conditions. To address this issue, we recorded sequences of panoramic images along accurately defined paths in a variety of outdoor locations and used these sequences as input to a two-dimensional array of correlation-based motion detectors (2DMD). We find that (a) motion signal distributions are sparse and noisy with respect to local motion directions; (b) motion signal distributions contain patches (motion streaks) which are systematically oriented along the principal flow-field directions; (c) motion signal distributions show a distinct, dorso-ventral topography, reflecting the distance anisotropy of terrestrial environments; (d) the spatiotemporal tuning of the local motion detector we used has little influence on the structure of motion signal distributions, at least for the range of conditions we tested; and (e) environmental motion is locally noisy throughout the visual field, with little spatial or temporal correlation; it can therefore be removed by temporal averaging and is largely over-ridden by image motion caused by observer movement. Our results suggest that spatial or temporal integration is important to retrieve reliable information on the local direction and size of motion vectors, because the structure of optic flow is clearly detectable in the temporal average of motion signal distributions. Ego-motion parameters can be reliably retrieved from such averaged distributions under a range of environmental conditions. These observations raise a number of questions about the role of specific environmental and computational constraints in the processing of natural optic flow.


Subject(s)
Models, Neurological , Motion Perception/physiology , Movement/physiology , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Visual Pathways/physiology , Environment , Humans , Motion , Orientation , Psychophysics , Signal Detection, Psychological
5.
Vision Res ; 42(22): 2499-508, 2002 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12445844

ABSTRACT

The spatial grain of the human visual system has always been a central topic for visual sciences, and the optical and physiological basis of perceptual limitations are well described. In particular, we have thorough accounts of spatial hyperacuity, which refers to a precision in the spatial localisation of stimulus contours that is better than the photoreceptor grain that determines spatial resolution. However, although the temporal resolution of the human visual system is comparably well described, we have almost no direct knowledge about the precision of localising visual stimuli in time in the absence of correlated spatial cues. The present study addresses this question by comparing directly the temporal resolution of human observers with their temporal acuity as measured in a temporal bisection task. Despite some improvement with practice, temporal acuity in this task does not fall below 20-30 ms in the best case, which is similar to the temporal resolution limit, and performance does not improve for comparison tasks with multiple stimulus presentations. The absence of visual hyperacuity for purely temporal modulations as tested here contrasts with processing limitations for other types of visual information in comparable tasks, and with other sensory modalities, in particular to those of the auditory system. Such differences can be interpreted in the context of the ecological requirements for organising behaviour, and the functional design of nervous systems.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Observer Variation , Photic Stimulation , Space Perception/physiology
6.
Exp Brain Res ; 141(1): 79-87, 2001 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11685412

ABSTRACT

Whereas it is a well known fact that objects appear to move faster in smaller stimulus fields, the reason for such a misjudgement of speed is still a matter of debate. We present four experiments to characterise the stimulus parameters that are important for the apparent speed increase of dots moving behind small apertures. In these experiments we varied the size and the shape of the aperture and its location in the visual field, as well as the stimulus duration. We report that the field-size effect does not depend on the overall duration of the stimulus, which does influence the typical path length of individual dots in the display. It is, however, affected by the shape of the aperture in such a way that the aperture size along the motion path is crucial for the speed misjudgement. The field-size effect furthermore depends on the location of the stimulus in the visual field. Our combined results are best described as an increase in perceived speed that is consistently elicited when a motion sink, i.e. a boundary of disappearing dots, is located close to the fovea. Such a description of the relevant stimulus parameters is discussed with respect to possible high-level mechanisms, relating back to classic Gestalt psychology explanations of the field-size effect, and with respect to well-known aspects of neuronal processing that may underlie speed perception and motion integration.


Subject(s)
Photic Stimulation/methods , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Motion , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychometrics , Visual Fields/physiology
7.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 18(9): 2321-30, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11551066

ABSTRACT

Motion-defined motion can play a special role in the discussion of whether one or two separate systems are required to process first- and second-order information because, in contrast to other second-order stimuli, such as contrast-modulated contours, motion detection cannot be explained by a simple input nonlinearity but requires preprocessing by motion detectors. Furthermore, the perceptual quality that defines an object (motion on the object surface) is identical to that which is attributed to the object as an emergent feature (motion of the object), raising the question of how these two object properties are linked. The interaction of first- and second-order information in such stimuli has been analyzed previously in a direction-discrimination task, revealing some cooperativity. Because any comprehensive integration of these two types of motion information should be reflected in the most fundamental property of a moving object, i.e., the direction in which it moves, we now investigate how motion direction is estimated in motion-defined objects. Observers had to report the direction of moving objects that were defined by luminance contrast or in random-dot kinematograms by differences in the spatiotemporal properties between the object region and the random-noise background. When the dots were moving coherently with the object (Fourier motion), direction sensitivity resembled that for luminance-defined objects, but performance deteriorated when the dots in the object region were static (drift-balanced motion). When the dots on the object surface were moving diagonally relative to the object direction (theta motion), the general level of accuracy declined further, and the perceived direction was intermediate between the veridical object motion direction and the direction of dot motion, indicating that the first- and second-order velocity vectors are somehow pooled. The inability to separate first- and second-order directional information suggests that the two corresponding subsystems of motion processing are not producing independent percepts and provides clues for possible implementations of the two-layer motion-processing network.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Cues , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Light , Male , Psychophysics/methods
8.
Naturwissenschaften ; 88(3): 129-32, 2001 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11402843

ABSTRACT

The perceived position of a moving object can be misleading because the object has advanced while its previous retinal image has been transmitted through the visual stream, leading to a mismatch between actual location and its neural representation. It has been suggested that the human visual system compensates for neural processing delays to retrieve instantaneous position. However, such a mechanism would require a precise measure of the actual delay in order to provide a reliable position estimate. A novel illusory deformation of moving contours demonstrates that humans misjudge the spatial relationship between parts of coherently moving targets, and therefore do not perfectly account for neural delays. The size of this deformation increases with growing speed. In some subjects this illusion can be reversed by varying the luminance of individual dots; a manipulation that affects the neural delays. Our experiments agree with other evidence that the capacity of the visual system to compensate for processing delays is limited.


Subject(s)
Illusions , Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Decision Making , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Judgment , Reaction Time
9.
Perception ; 30(11): 1321-36, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11768487

ABSTRACT

In order to study the integration of local motion signals in the human visual system, we measured directional tuning curves for the barber-pole illusion by varying two crucial aspects of the stimulus layout independently across a wide a range in the same experiment. These were the orientation of the grating presented behind the rectangular aperture and the aspect ratio of the aperture, which in combination determine the relative contributions of local motion signals perpendicular to the gratings and parallel to the aperture borders, respectively. The strength of the illusion, ie the tendency to perceive motion along the major axis of the aperture, obviously depends on the spatial layout of the aperture, but also on grating orientation. Subjects were asked which direction they perceived and how compelling their motion percept was, revealing different strategies of the visual system to deal with the barber-pole stimulus. Some individuals respond strongly to the unambiguous motion information at the boundaries, leading to multistable percepts and multimodal distributions of responses. Others tend to report intermediate directions, apparently being less influenced by the actual boundaries. The general pattern of deviations from the motion direction perpendicular to grating orientation--a decrease with aspect ratio approaching unity (ie square-shaped apertures) and with gratings approaching parallel orientation to the shorter aperture boundary--is discussed in the context of simple phenomenological models of motion integration. The best fit between model predictions and experimental data is found for an interaction between two stimulus parameters: (i) cycle ratio, which is the sine-wave gratings equivalent of the terminator ratio for line gratings, describing the effects from the aperture boundaries, and (ii) the grating orientation, responsible for perpendicular motion components, which describes the influence of motion signals from inside the aperture. This suggests that the most simple cycle (terminator) ratio explanation cannot fully account for the quantitative properties of the barber-pole illusion.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Optical Illusions , Adult , Female , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Psychological , Orientation/physiology
10.
J Exp Biol ; 203(Pt 22): 3397-409, 2000 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11044379

ABSTRACT

The Strepsiptera are an enigmatic group of parasitic insects whose phylogenetic relationships are hotly debated. Male Strepsiptera have very unusual compound eyes, in which each of a small number of ommatidia possesses a retina of at least 60 retinula cells. We analysed the optomotor response of Xenos vesparum males to determine whether spatial resolution in these eyes is limited by the interommatidial angle or by the higher resolution potentially provided by the extended array of retinula cells within each ommatidium. We find that the optomotor response in Strepsiptera has a typical bandpass characteristic in the temporal domain, with a temporal frequency optimum at 1-3 Hz. As a function of spatial wavelength, the optomotor response is zero at grating periods below 12 degrees and reaches its maximum strength at grating periods between 60 degrees and 70 degrees. To identify the combination of interommatidial angles and angular sensitivity functions that would generate such a spatial characteristic, we used motion detection theory to model the spatial tuning function of the strepsipteran optomotor response. We found the best correspondence between the measured response profile and theoretical prediction for an irregular array of sampling distances spaced around 9 degrees (half the estimated interommatidial angle) and an angular sensitivity function of approximately 50 degrees, which corresponds to the angular extent of the retina we estimated at the centre of curvature of the lens. Our behavioural data strongly suggest that, at least for the optomotor response, the resolution of the strepsipteran compound eye is limited by the ommatidial sampling array and not by the array of retinula cells within each ommatidium. We discuss the significance of these results in relation to the functional organisation of strepsipteran compound eyes, their evolution and the role of vision in these insects.


Subject(s)
Insecta/physiology , Vision, Ocular , Animals , Eye , Male , Motor Activity , Space Perception
11.
Clin Exp Ophthalmol ; 28(3): 220-2, 2000 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10981804

ABSTRACT

When two identical objects move in opposite directions on the same path and at the same speed, they can appear, after crossing over to continue n their original directions (streaming), or to reverse direction (bouncing). In order to be able to man pulate visibility by adding no se, we used objects defined by contrast, flicker, or motion, and thereby extended previous findings on luminance-defined objects. Two identical rectangles (1.1 x 1.4 degrees) composed of random dot patterns moved toward each other at a speed of 3.5 degrees/s. In experiment I we used backgrounds of a grey field, static random dots, or dynamic noise, and examined the effect of introducing a pause in motion and a visual distractor. In experiment 2 we introduced visual noise at four levels. For all three types of motion display, we found an increase in the proportion of the bouncing percept when either a pause in motion or an attentional distractor was introduced. Experiment 2 showed that neither of these effects depends on the visibility of the moving objects. An increase in the bouncing percept, due to a pause in motion or the distraction of attention, can be observed for all types of object definition, and is not affected by decreas ng the visibility of the motion-defined objects.This finding suggests that the role of attention in determining the perception of bouncing does not lie in the modulation of object visibility.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Attention , Contrast Sensitivity , Humans , Noise , Psychophysics
12.
Eur J Neurosci ; 12(1): 273-82, 2000 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10651882

ABSTRACT

Based on single cell recordings in monkey, it has been suggested that neural activity can be related directly to psychophysically measured threshold behaviour. Here, we investigated in humans whether evoked potentials correlate with behavioural measurements like discrimination thresholds and reaction time. Subjects were asked to report the perceived direction of object motion stimuli which contained variable amounts of coherent motion. Simultaneously, we recorded evoked potentials with a multielectrode array, or measured the reaction time. We show here that motion coherence had a strong influence on both amplitude and latency of the evoked potential. Stronger motion signals evoked stronger and faster cortical responses. The latency reduction of the motion onset response with increasing coherence correlated very well with the concurrent decrease in reaction time. Taken together, these results suggest that temporal integration is an important step in analysing motion signals to generate a reliable behavioural response. We stimulated a two-dimensional array of correlation-type motion detectors with the same motion sequences, and analysed the distribution of local motion signals according to signal detection theory. Performance resembled that of human subjects when the decision strategy was optimized so as to exclude small signals and, in particular, when the ideal observer had some knowledge about a region of interest in which the object was to be expected.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Computer Simulation , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Models, Neurological , Motion Perception/physiology , Humans , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time
13.
Naturwissenschaften ; 86(10): 492-5, 1999 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10541660

ABSTRACT

Human observers achieve a surprising precision in many visual judgements, such as estimating relative position, colinearity and the regularity of shape. We measured the sensitivity in detecting shape deformations by presenting a square simultaneously with a rectangle of variable aspect ratio, or a circle with an ellipsoid. Weber fractions approach 3-5% and improve to approximately 1% when subjects are asked to tell which of the two objects was 'oriented more vertically', instead of identifying the square or circle. Contour position can be judged with a precision of 10-20 arc s, clearly in the hyperacuity range and also beyond the thresholds known for detecting differences in the curvature of comparable line segments. Our results suggest that detecting deformation in rectangles seems to rely on aspect ratio, whereas performance is improved for ellipsoids by a high sensitivity for changes in local curvature.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Adult , Discrimination, Psychological , Humans , Judgment , Middle Aged , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Visual Acuity
14.
Aust N Z J Ophthalmol ; 27(3-4): 254-7, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10484207

ABSTRACT

A procedure is described for generating stimuli to study the detection of noise components in motion signals. By using random dots with intensities distributed according to a Gaussian probability function, a temporally and spatially continuous mixture of signal and noise components can be realized in random dot kinematograms. These stimuli were used in a noise detection task, a signal detection task and a direction discrimination task. Signal-to-noise ratio ('coherence') thresholds for the signal detection and direction discrimination tasks were consistent with previous research. Noise can be detected at levels of approximately 0.5-2.5%, depending on the size of the motion stimulus. We argue that the noise in the motion stimulus becomes detectable when it exceeds the noise intrinsic to the various stages of motion processing. Therefore,the method provides a simple procedure for obtaining measures of equivalent input noise and can be used for estimating internal noise levels of motion processing mechanisms.


Subject(s)
Artifacts , Motion Perception , Signal Detection, Psychological , Adult , Differential Threshold , Discrimination, Psychological , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods
15.
Spat Vis ; 12(3): 287-307, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10442515

ABSTRACT

When a sinewave grating is moving within a cross-shaped aperture, a strongly multi-stable phenomenon is perceived. The percept switches between the coherence of an extended surface moving in a single direction and the segregation of two patterned strips sliding across each other in directions parallel to the branches of the cross. We studied how the balance between these two percepts is affected by the length of the arms and by the shape of their ends. We report here that human observers report the segregation into two surfaces more often when the branches of the cross are extended, and when the small sides of the arms are oriented parallel to the grating. Two kinds of early motion signals interact in the crossed barber-pole stimulus: (a) the signals extracted in the middle of the bars are ambiguous with regard to their direction, and usually would be interpreted as motion normal to the grating orientation; (b) the signals from regions where the grating is intersected by the borders of the aperture convey motion signals in direction of the border. Our results show that the global appearance of our display can be dramatically influenced by the reliability of motion signals located in small regions that may be separated by large distances. To explain this long-range effect, we tentatively propose the existence of a representation level situated between the extraction of low-level local signals and the final global percept. The postulated processing level is concerned with the segmenting of the entire image into surfaces that are likely to belong to the same object, even if they are not contiguous in space. This hypothetical mechanism involves the construction of coarse-scale 'patches' from the local motion signal distributions, each carrying a single velocity associated with a certain degree of reliability. Our experiments indicate that the probability of grouping together similar patches depends on their respective reliabilities.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Observer Variation , Photic Stimulation , Reference Values , Reproducibility of Results
16.
Vision Res ; 39(14): 2411-20, 1999 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10367061

ABSTRACT

Local motion signals have to be combined in space and time, to yield a coherent motion percept as it is involved in a variety of visual tasks. This combination necessarily means to trade-off between loosing spatio-temporal resolution by pooling local signals and maintaining perceptually significant segmentation between them. When signals are pooled to detect the presence of coherent motion in large amounts of random noise, the question raised is how the noise affects the perceived quality, in particular speed, of the coherent motion. Is there an analogy to the well-known reduction in the perceived speed of moving gratings at low contrast? Using a two-interval forced-choice procedure, we have investigated the assessment of speed in random-dot kinematograms containing different proportions of noise. Under the conditions investigated, there is no strong reduction of perceived speed with increasing noise, as long as coherence levels remain well above the thresholds for directional judgements. This basic result, which could suggest considerable but not perfect segregation of signal and noise motion components in the pooling process leading to speed estimation, is discussed in relation to a model that is designed to decode speed from a population of elementary motion detectors (EMDs) of the correlation type. A strategy to estimate speed from a set of EMDs with a variety of spatio-temporal tuning does not only provide a velocity predictor unambiguous with the spatial structure of the stimulus, but also is largely independent of noise.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Adult , Differential Threshold/physiology , Humans , Models, Biological , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychometrics , Time Factors
17.
Vision Res ; 39(7): 1293-304, 1999 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10343843

ABSTRACT

Specific improvements of perceptual capabilities with practise are thought to give some clues about cortical plasticity and the localisation of cortical processing. In the present study, perceptual learning is used as a paradigm to separate mechanisms underlying the perception of different classes of motion stimuli. Primary motion stimuli (phi-motion), are characterised by displacements of the luminance distribution. However, for secondary motion stimuli the movement is not accompanied by a corresponding luminance shift. Instead, moving objects are defined by their temporal frequency composition (mu-motion) or by motion itself (theta-motion). On theoretical grounds, the perception of secondary motion requires a higher degree of nonlinearity in the processing stream than the perception of primary motion but debate continues as to whether there might be a unique mechanism underlying the perception of both motion classes. In a large group of subjects, coherence thresholds for direction discrimination in random dot kinematograms of phi-, mu-, and theta-motion were repeatedly measured in a staircase paradigm. Training effects were found on different timescales, within short sessions containing multiple staircases and over training periods of several months. They were fairly stable over long breaks without testing. When subjects were trained with two different motion stimuli in a sequence, an asymmetry in the transfer of perceptual learning was revealed: sensitivity increases achieved during practise of theta-motion are largely transferred to phi-motion, but theta-motion perception does not profit from prior exposure to phi-motion. This finding supports the view derived from modelling of motion processing that there must be at least partially separate systems. A primary motion detection mechanism falls short of discriminating direction in secondary motion stimuli, whereas a mechanism able to extract secondary motion will be inherently sensitive to primary motion.


Subject(s)
Learning , Motion Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Humans , Middle Aged , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Factors
18.
Biol Cybern ; 80(2): 109-16, 1999 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12440388

ABSTRACT

A prominent model of visual motion detection is the so-called correlation or Reichardt detector. Whereas this model can account for many properties of motion vision, from humans to insects (review, Borst and Egelhaaf 1989), it has been commonly assumed that this scheme of motion detection is not well suited to the measurement of image velocity. This is because the commonly used version of the model, which incorporates two unidirectional motion detectors with opposite preferred directions, produces a response which varies not only with the velocity of the image, but also with its spatial structure and contrast. On the other hand, information on image velocity can be crucial in various contexts, and a number of recent behavioural experiments suggest that insects do extract velocity for navigational purposes (review, Srinivasan et al. 1996). Here we show that other versions of the correlation model, which consists of a single unidirectional motion detector or incorporates two oppositely directed detectors with unequal sensitivities, produce responses which vary with image speed and display tuning curves that are substantially independent of the spatial structure of the image. This surprising feature suggests simple strategies of reducing ambiguities in the estimation of speed by using components of neural hardware that are already known to exist in the visual system.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception , Models, Theoretical
19.
Perception ; 27(2): 167-81, 1998.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9709449

ABSTRACT

First steps of visual-information processing in primates are characterised by a highly ordered representation of the outside world on the cortex. Two prominent features of cortical organisation are the retinotopic mapping of position in the visual field on the first stages of the visual stream, and the systematic variation of orientation preference in the same areas. In an attempt to understand the relation of position and orientation representation, we need to know the minimum spatial requirements for orientation detection. In the present paper, the spatial limits for detecting orientation are analysed by simulating simple orientation filters and testing the ability of human observers to detect the orientation of small lines at various positions in the visual field. At sufficiently high contrast levels, the minimum physical length of a line to discriminate orientation differences of 45 degrees-90 degrees is not constant when presented at various eccentricities, but covaries inversely with the cortical magnification factor. In consequence, a line needs to correspond to about 0.2 mm of cortical surface, independently of the actual eccentricity at which the stimulus is presented, in order to allow observers to recognise its orientation. This has consequences for our understanding of orientation detection. (i) In combination with simulation experiments, it becomes clear that the elementary process underlying orientation detection is a local operation, which seems to focus on small regions compared with cortical receptive fields. (ii) With respect to the number of inputs to the visual cortex, the performance of this local operation approaches the physical limits, requiring hardly more than three-four input LGN axons to be activated for detecting the orientation of a highly visible line segment. Comparing these spatial characteristics with the receptive fields of orientation-sensitive neurons in the primate visual system could suggest new insights into the neuronal circuits underlying orientation mapping in the human cortex.


Subject(s)
Computer Simulation , Models, Psychological , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Humans , Psychological Tests
20.
Aust N Z J Ophthalmol ; 26 Suppl 1: S104-7, 1998 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9685039

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Visual information processing in the human cortex is based on a highly ordered representation of the surrounding world. In addition to the retinotopic mapping of the visual field, systematic variations of the orientation tuning of neurons have been described in the primary visual cortex. As a step to understanding the relationship between position and orientation representation, we investigated psychophysically the minimum spatial requirements for the determination of orientation at various positions across the visual field. We know that the shortest line whose orientation can be resolved varies with eccentricity, such that its length corresponds to slightly less than 0.2 mm projected onto the cortical surface. Along the horizontal meridian horizontal lines are detected with higher precision than vertical or oblique lines. In the present experiments, we tested whether this is a preference for horizontal lines or for lines that are orientated radially away from the fovea. METHODS: Human observers were tested with lines positioned at one vertical, two horizontal and two oblique meridians at eccentricities between 5 and 25 degrees. RESULTS/CONCLUSION: Three of the four subjects were most sensitive for targets aligned with the meridian of presentation. This suggests that the visual system has the highest resolution in directions radiating from the fovea, which may be particularly useful for the analysis of flow fields resulting from forward translation.


Subject(s)
Orientation , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Humans , Psychophysics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...