Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 83
Filter
1.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 28(5): 467-480, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38548492

ABSTRACT

The vividness of imagery varies between individuals. However, the existence of people in whom conscious, wakeful imagery is markedly reduced, or absent entirely, was neglected by psychology until the recent coinage of 'aphantasia' to describe this phenomenon. 'Hyperphantasia' denotes the converse - imagery whose vividness rivals perceptual experience. Around 1% and 3% of the population experience extreme aphantasia and hyperphantasia, respectively. Aphantasia runs in families, often affects imagery across several sense modalities, and is variably associated with reduced autobiographical memory, face recognition difficulty, and autism. Visual dreaming is often preserved. Subtypes of extreme imagery appear to be likely but are not yet well defined. Initial results suggest that alterations in connectivity between the frontoparietal and visual networks may provide the neural substrate for visual imagery extremes.


Subject(s)
Imagination , Humans , Imagination/physiology , Memory, Episodic , Dreams/physiology
2.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 29(6): 2096-2107, 2022 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35680760

ABSTRACT

Aphantasia refers to the inability to summon images to one's own mind's eye, resulting in selective deficits of voluntary object imagery. In the present study, we investigated whether M. X., a case of acquired aphantasia, can still retain some form of spatial transformation processes even though he is unable to subjectively experience voluntary object imagery. M. X. and a group of control participants were asked to complete a letter mental rotation task (MRT), typically used to assess the nature of the spatial transformation, while behavioural and electrophysiological responses were recorded. M. X. was able to complete the MRTs as accurately as controls, showing the pattern of increasing RTs as a function of rotation angle typical of MRTs. However, event-related potential (ERP) results showed systematic differences between M. X. and controls. On canonical letter trials, the rotation-related negativity (RRN), an ERP component considered as the psychophysiological correlate of the spatial transformation of mental rotation (MR), was present in both M. X. and controls and similarly modulated by rotation angle. However, no such modulation was observed for M. X. on mirror-reversed letter trials. These findings suggest that, at least under specific experimental conditions, the inability to create a depictive representation of the stimuli does not prevent the engagement of spatial transformation in aphantasia. However, the ability to apply spatial transformation varies with tasks and might be accounted for by the specific type of mental representation that can be accessed.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials , Imagination , Male , Humans , Imagination/physiology
4.
Cortex ; 150: 149-152, 2022 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35314076

ABSTRACT

Recently, the term 'aphantasia' has become current in scientific and public discourse to denote the absence of mental imagery. However, new terms for aphantasia or its subgroups have recently been proposed, e.g., 'dysikonesia' or 'anauralia', which complicates the literature, research communication and understanding for the general public. Before further terms emerge, we advocate the consistent use of the term 'aphantasia' as it can be used flexibly and precisely, and is already widely known in the scientific community and among the general public.


Subject(s)
Imagination , Visual Perception , Humans , Imagery, Psychotherapy
5.
Epilepsia ; 63(5): 1115-1129, 2022 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35253220

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Transient epileptic amnesia (TEA) is a form of adult-onset epilepsy where presenting features are well described, but little is known regarding prognosis. This study aimed to elucidate the long-term prognosis of TEA regarding seizure control, memory, medical comorbidities, and life expectancy. METHODS: Up-to-date clinical information was collected for 47 people diagnosed with TEA who had joined the The Impairment of Memory in Epilepsy (TIME) study 10 years earlier. At entry to the study, information about comorbid conditions was systematically collected. Details regarding subsequent diagnoses, seizure activity, changes to treatment, or reports of cognitive impairment were obtained through the family doctor. The variables of interest were compared with UK population data. RESULTS: Mortality in the cohort was 21 of 47 (45%), with an average age at death of 82.5 years. Seizures remained well controlled for the majority but medications required adjustments in dose and type for some (28%). A small number (three cases) remained seizure-free without medication. History of cardiovascular disorders was frequent (78.7%), typically involving hypertension (55.3%). Autoimmune disorders (25.5%), cancer (23.4%), and depression (21.3%) were also commonly reported. Although persisting memory problems were often noted, dementia was diagnosed in seven cases (14.9%). Life expectancy and comorbidities in TEA did not differ from available population norms. SIGNIFICANCE: Results suggest that life expectancy is not reduced in TEA. Although TEA does not appear to be a self-limiting form of epilepsy, seizures are typically well controlled via medication. Because adjustments to medication may be required, even after long periods of stability, ongoing medical monitoring is recommended. Comorbid vascular disorders are frequent but appear similar to general population estimates. Monitoring mood may be important, given that people with chronic conditions are often vulnerable to depression. Because of persisting memory difficulties, the development of effective memory interventions for people with TEA is warranted.


Subject(s)
Amnesia , Epilepsy , Adult , Amnesia/epidemiology , Cohort Studies , Epilepsy/complications , Epilepsy/diagnosis , Epilepsy/epidemiology , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Seizures/complications
6.
Cereb Cortex Commun ; 2(2): tgab035, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34296179

ABSTRACT

Although Galton recognized in the 1880s that some individuals lack visual imagery, this phenomenon was mostly neglected over the following century. We recently coined the terms "aphantasia" and "hyperphantasia" to describe visual imagery vividness extremes, unlocking a sustained surge of public interest. Aphantasia is associated with subjective impairment of face recognition and autobiographical memory. Here we report the first systematic, wide-ranging neuropsychological and brain imaging study of people with aphantasia (n = 24), hyperphantasia (n = 25), and midrange imagery vividness (n = 20). Despite equivalent performance on standard memory tests, marked group differences were measured in autobiographical memory and imagination, participants with hyperphantasia outperforming controls who outperformed participants with aphantasia. Face recognition difficulties and autistic spectrum traits were reported more commonly in aphantasia. The Revised NEO Personality Inventory highlighted reduced extraversion in the aphantasia group and increased openness in the hyperphantasia group. Resting state fMRI revealed stronger connectivity between prefrontal cortices and the visual network among hyperphantasic than aphantasic participants. In an active fMRI paradigm, there was greater anterior parietal activation among hyperphantasic and control than aphantasic participants when comparing visualization of famous faces and places with perception. These behavioral and neural signatures of visual imagery vividness extremes validate and illuminate this significant but neglected dimension of individual difference.

7.
BMJ Case Rep ; 14(6)2021 Jun 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34158325

ABSTRACT

A previously well 54- year-old woman presented with a short history of diplopia, cognitive decline, hallucinations and hypersomnolence. The patient had progressive deterioration in short-term memory, ocular convergence spasm, tremor, myoclonus, gait apraxia, central fever, dream enactment and seizures. Results of investigations were normal including MRI brain, electroencephalogram, cerebrospinal fluid (CSF, including CSF prion protein markers) and brain biopsy. The patient died from pneumonia and pulmonary embolus. Brain postmortem analysis revealed neuropathological changes in keeping with Fatal familial insomnia (FFI); the diagnosis was confirmed on genetic testing. FFI is caused by an autosomal dominant and highly penetrant pathogenic Prion Protein gene PRNP Although usually familial, fatal insomnia (FI) also occurs in a rare sporadic form. FI is a rare human prion disease with prominent sleep disturbance, autonomic, motor, cognitive and behavioural involvement. Patient management is with best supportive care and early suspected diagnosis allows for timely palliation.


Subject(s)
Insomnia, Fatal Familial , Prion Diseases , Prions , Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders , Female , Humans , Insomnia, Fatal Familial/genetics , Middle Aged , Prion Proteins/genetics , Prions/genetics
8.
Brain Commun ; 3(2): fcab038, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33884371

ABSTRACT

The term transient epileptic amnesia was coined in 1990 to describe a form of epilepsy causing predominantly amnestic seizures which could be confused with episodes of Transient Global Amnesia. Subsequent descriptions have highlighted its association with 'atypical' forms of memory disturbance including accelerated long-term forgetting, disproportionate autobiographical amnesia and topographical amnesia. However, this highly treatment-responsive condition remains under-recognized and undertreated. We describe the clinical and neuropsychological features in 65 consecutive cases of transient epileptic amnesia referred to our study, comparing these to our previous cohort of 50 patients and to those reported in 102 literature cases described since our 2008 review. Findings in our two cohorts are substantially consistent: The onset of transient epileptic amnesia occurs at an average age of 62 years, giving rise to amnestic episodes at a frequency of around 1/month, typically lasting 15-30 min and often occurring on waking. Amnesia is the only manifestation of epilepsy in 24% of patients; olfactory hallucinations occur in 43%, motor automatisms in 41%, brief unresponsiveness in 39%. The majority of patients describe at least one of the atypical forms of memory disturbance mentioned above; easily provoked tearfulness is a common accompanying feature. There is a male predominance (85:30). Epileptiform changes were present in 35% of cases, while suspected causative magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities were detected in only 5%. Seizures ceased with anticonvulsant treatment in 93% of cases. Some clinical features were detected more commonly in the second series than the first, probably as a result of heightened awareness. Neuropsychological testing and comparison to two age and IQ-matched control groups (n = 24 and 22) revealed consistent findings across the two cohorts, namely elevated mean IQ, preserved executive function, mild impairment at the group level on standard measures of memory, with additional evidence for accelerated long-term forgetting and autobiographical amnesia, particularly affecting episodic recollection. Review of the literature cases revealed broadly consistent features except that topographical amnesia, olfactory hallucinations and emotionality have been reported rarely to date by other researchers. We conclude that transient epileptic amnesia is a distinctive syndrome of late-onset limbic epilepsy of unknown cause, typically occurring in late middle age. It is an important, treatable cause of memory loss in older people, often mistaken for dementia, cerebrovascular disease and functional amnesia. Its aetiology, the monthly occurrence of seizures in some patients and the mechanisms and interrelationships of the interictal features-amnestic and affective-all warrant further study.

9.
Handb Clin Neurol ; 178: 277-296, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33832681

ABSTRACT

Visual imagery allows us to revisit the appearance of things in their absence and to test out virtual combinations of sensory experience. Visual imagery has been linked to many cognitive processes, such as autobiographical and visual working memory. Imagery also plays symptomatic and mechanistic roles in neurologic and mental disorders and is utilized in treatment. A large network of brain activity spanning frontal, parietal, temporal, and visual cortex is involved in generating and maintain images in mind. The ability to visualize has extreme variations, ranging from completely absent (aphantasia) to photo-like (hyperphantasia). The anatomy and functionality of visual cortex, including primary visual cortex, have been associated with individual differences in visual imagery ability, pointing to a potential correlate for both aphantasia and hyperphantasia. Preliminary evidence suggests that lifelong aphantasia is associated with prosopagnosia and reduction in autobiographical memory; hyperphantasia is associated with synesthesia. Aphantasic individuals can also be highly imaginative and are able to complete many tasks that were previously thought to rely on visual imagery, demonstrating that visualization is only one of many ways of representing things in their absence. The study of extreme imagination reminds us how easily invisible differences can escape detection.


Subject(s)
Visual Cortex , Humans , Imagination , Memory, Short-Term , Nervous System Physiological Phenomena , Visual Perception
10.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 26(2): 107-121, 2021 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33467984

ABSTRACT

Introduction: While short-term cognitive impairment following electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) is well described and acknowledged, the relationship between ECT and persistent memory impairment, particularly of autobiographical memory, has been controversial. Methods: We describe the case of a 70-year-old consultant neurophysiologist, AW, who developed prominent, selective autobiographical memory loss following two courses of ECT for treatment-resistant depression. Results: His performance on standard measures of IQ, semantic and episodic memory, executive function and mood was normal, while he performed significantly below controls on measures of episodic autobiographical memory. Conclusions: Explanations in terms of mood-related memory loss and somatoform disorder appear unlikely. We relate AW's autobiographical memory impairment, following his ECT, to reports of similar autobiographical memory impairment occurring in the context of epilepsy, and emphasise the importance of using sensitive approaches to AbM assessment.


Subject(s)
Electroconvulsive Therapy , Memory, Episodic , Aged , Amnesia/etiology , Electroconvulsive Therapy/adverse effects , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/etiology
11.
Cortex ; 130: 426-440, 2020 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32446532

ABSTRACT

Visual imagery typically enables us to see absent items in the mind's eye. It plays a role in memory, day-dreaming and creativity. Since coining the terms aphantasia and hyperphantasia to describe the absence and abundance of visual imagery, we have been contacted by many thousands of people with extreme imagery abilities. Questionnaire data from 2000 participants with aphantasia and 200 with hyperphantasia indicate that aphantasia is associated with scientific and mathematical occupations, whereas hyperphantasia is associated with 'creative' professions. Participants with aphantasia report an elevated rate of difficulty with face recognition and autobiographical memory, whereas participants with hyperphantasia report an elevated rate of synaesthesia. Around half those with aphantasia describe an absence of wakeful imagery in all sense modalities, while a majority dream visually. Aphantasia appears to run within families more often than would be expected by chance. Aphantasia and hyperphantasia appear to be widespread but neglected features of human experience with informative psychological associations.


Subject(s)
Imagination , Memory, Episodic , Humans , Imagery, Psychotherapy , Surveys and Questionnaires , Synesthesia
12.
J Sleep Res ; 29(6): e12980, 2020 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32166824

ABSTRACT

Sleep is a pillar of health, alongside adequate nutrition and exercise. Problems with sleep are common and often treatable. Twenty years ago, UK medical school education on sleep disorders had a median teaching time of 15 min; we investigate whether education on sleep disorders has improved. This is a cross-sectional survey, including time spent on teaching sleep medicine, subtopics covered and forms of assessment. Thirty-four medical degree courses in the UK were investigated via a questionnaire. We excluded responses not concerned with general undergraduate education (i.e. optional modules). Twenty-five (74%) medical schools responded. Time spent teaching undergraduates sleep medicine was: median, 1.5 hr; mode, <1 hr; mean, 3.2 hr (SD = 2.6). Only two schools had a syllabus or core module (8%) and five (22%) were involved in sleep disorders research. Despite the above, half of the respondents thought provision was sufficient. Free-text comments had recurring themes: sleep medicine is subsumed into other specialties, obstructive sleep apnea dominates teaching, knowledge of sleep disorders is optional, and there is inertia regarding change. A substantial minority of respondents were enthusiastic about improving provision. In conclusion, little has changed over 20 years: sleep medicine is neglected despite agreement on its importance for general health. Sleep research is the exception rather than the rule. Obstacles to change include views that "sleep is not a core topic" or "the curriculum is too crowded". However, there is enthusiasm for improvement. We recommend establishment of a sleep medicine curriculum. Without better teaching, doctors will remain ill-equipped to recognize and treat these common conditions.


Subject(s)
Curriculum/standards , Education, Medical, Undergraduate/standards , Sleep Wake Disorders/diagnosis , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Students, Medical , Time Factors , United Kingdom
13.
Neurology ; 94(12): e1320-e1335, 2020 03 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31980582

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: We investigated the nature and neural foundations of pathologic tearfulness in a uniquely large cohort of patients who had presented with autoimmune limbic encephalitis (aLE). METHODS: We recruited 38 patients (26 men, 12 women; median age 63.06 years; interquartile range [IQR] 16.06 years) in the postacute phase of aLE who completed questionnaires probing emotion regulation. All patients underwent structural/functional MRI postacutely, along with 67 age- and sex-matched healthy controls (40 men, 27 women; median age 64.70 years; IQR 19.87 years). We investigated correlations of questionnaire scores with demographic, clinical, neuropsychological, and brain imaging data across patients. We also compared patients diagnosed with pathologic tearfulness and those without, along with healthy controls, on gray matter volume, resting-state functional connectivity, and activity. RESULTS: Pathologic tearfulness was reported by 50% of the patients, while no patient reported pathologic laughing. It was not associated with depression, impulsiveness, memory impairment, executive dysfunction in the postacute phase, or amygdalar abnormalities in the acute phase. It correlated with changes in specific emotional brain networks: volume reduction in the right anterior hippocampus, left fusiform gyrus, and cerebellum, abnormal hippocampal resting-state functional connectivity with the posteromedial cortex and right middle frontal gyrus, and abnormal hemodynamic activity in the left fusiform gyrus, right inferior parietal lobule, and ventral pons. CONCLUSIONS: Pathologic tearfulness is common following aLE, is not a manifestation of other neuropsychiatric features, and reflects abnormalities in networks of emotion regulation beyond the acute hippocampal focus. The condition, which may also be present in other neurologic disorders, provides novel insights into the neural basis of affective control and its dysfunction in disease.


Subject(s)
Autoimmune Diseases/complications , Autoimmune Diseases/pathology , Brain/pathology , Crying/physiology , Emotional Regulation/physiology , Limbic Encephalitis/complications , Limbic Encephalitis/pathology , Aged , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged
14.
Front Neurol ; 10: 1266, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31866927

ABSTRACT

The prevalence of epileptic seizures is increased in patients in the clinical stages of Alzheimer's disease (AD) when compared to age-matched cognitively normal populations. In previously reported work from the Presentation of Epileptic Seizures in Dementia (PrESIDe) study, we identified a clinical suspicion of epilepsy in between 12.75 and 28.43% of patients with AD recruited from a memory clinic. EEGs were not performed in this study. Patients with epilepsy performed similarly to patients without epilepsy on cognitive testing at the time of recruitment but were more impaired on two measures of everyday functioning [Cambridge Behavioral Inventory-Revised and Clinical Dementia Rating (CBI-R and CDR)]. On repeated testing in this 12-month follow-up study, patients in whom a suspicion of epilepsy was identified performed significantly worse on cognitive function testing (p = 0.028) in addition to maintaining a difference on the informant questionnaires (CBI-R p < 0.001, CDR p = 0.020). These findings suggest that seizures in this population could be a marker of a more rapid decline and worse prognosis.

15.
Seizure ; 71: 83-92, 2019 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31226566

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To determine the prevalence and clinical features of epileptic seizures occurring in a memory clinic population. METHOD: We recruited patients receiving a diagnosis of dementia or mild cognitive impairment (MCI) at a regional memory clinic. We interviewed patients and informants using a proforma designed to elicit symptoms suggestive of epilepsy. Informants also completed the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR) and the Cambridge Behavioural Inventory- Revised (CBI-R). Patients underwent cognitive testing using the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination - III (ACE-III). We also recruited an age- and gender- matched control group with no history of cognitive impairment. Diagnoses of dementia/MCI were checked against current diagnostic criteria. RESULTS: We recruited 144 patients (mean age 77.98, mean ACE-III 74.16, 124 with dementia, 20 with MCI). We diagnosed epilepsy in 25.7%: probable in 12.5% (17 with dementia, 1 with MCI), possible 13.2% (18 with dementia, 1 with MCI). Seizure features included altered responsiveness, speech/behavioural arrest, oral/pharyngeal automatism, olfactory/gustatory aura, focal motor seizure, other sensory phenomena (including hallucination), and amnesia on waking. Epilepsy prevalence was significantly increased in the dementia and MCI group vs controls (p = 0.004). Cognitive performance in the patient groups did not distinguish those in whom epilepsy was suspected from those in whom it was not. Patients in whom epilepsy was suspected were more impaired on informant completed measures of daily function. CONCLUSIONS: The prevalence of epilepsy is increased in dementia. The seizures are often subtle and easily missed. The presence of epilepsy predicts more severe impairment in the activities of daily living.


Subject(s)
Activities of Daily Living , Cognitive Dysfunction/epidemiology , Dementia/epidemiology , Epilepsy/epidemiology , Memory Disorders/epidemiology , Seizures/epidemiology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Comorbidity , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Prevalence
16.
J Neuroimmunol ; 327: 41-43, 2019 02 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30686545

ABSTRACT

Transient Epileptic Amnesia (TEA) is a subtype of temporal lobe epilepsy, typically presenting in a person's early 60s, and of unknown aetiology. Encephalitis caused by antibodies to NMDA receptors (NMDARE) has not previously been documented in TEA. We describe a 47-year-old male who satisfied criteria for TEA, but given his atypical symptoms, was also screened for autoimmune epilepsy. High levels of serum NMDAR antibodies were found, suggesting NMDARE. Immunosuppressive treatment gradually eliminated the NMDA receptor antibodies. Our case extends the clinical spectrum associated with neuronal cell-surface autoantibodies to include atypical cases of TEA.


Subject(s)
Anti-N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptor Encephalitis/complications , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/immunology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
17.
Cortex ; 110: 47-57, 2019 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29329640

ABSTRACT

Transient epileptic amnesia (TEA) is an epileptic syndrome characterized by recurrent, brief episodes of amnesia. Patients with TEA often complain of interictal (between attacks) retention deficits, characterised by an 'evaporation' of memories for recent events over days to weeks. Clinical tests of anterograde memory often fail to corroborate these complaints as TEA patients commonly perform within the normal range after the standard 10-30-min delay period. Modified laboratory tests that include a 1-3 week delay period frequently reveal clear evidence of 'accelerated long-term forgetting' (ALF). However, they are not used routinely and lack ecological validity. In the present study we examined whether 'real-life' ALF can be captured via a controlled incidental memory test in TEA patients. To this end, the experimenter told 27 TEA patients and 32 controls a well-rehearsed amusing story, apparently as a way of making light conversation before starting a set of research experiments. Without prior warning, the experimenter subsequently probed the participants' memory of this story via tests of free recall and forced choice recognition after 30 min or 1 week. After 30 min retention was comparable in TEA patients and controls. After 1 week TEA patients retained significantly less story material than controls, and significant ALF was revealed in the TEA patients in the recognition test. Our data show that ALF in a 'real-life' situation can occur even when standard memory tests indicate normal memory function. Moreover, our data suggest that incidental memory tests can capture real-life ALF, and that forced-choice recognition tests might be more sensitive than free recall tests for the detection of real-life ALF.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/physiopathology , Epilepsy , Mental Recall/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Adult , Aged , Epilepsy/psychology , Female , Humans , Learning/physiology , Male , Middle Aged , Recognition, Psychology/physiology
18.
Cortex ; 110: 16-36, 2019 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29122206

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Accelerated long-term forgetting (ALF) occurs when newly learned information decays faster than normal over extended delays. It has been recognised most frequently in temporal lobe epilepsy, including Transient Epileptic Amnesia (TEA), but can also be drug-induced. Little is known about the evolution of ALF over time and its impacts upon other memory functions, such as autobiographical memory (ABM). Here we investigate the long-term outcome of ALF and ABM in a group of patients with TEA and a single case of baclofen-induced ALF. METHODS: Study 1 involved a longitudinal follow-up of 14 patients with TEA over a 10-year period. Patients repeated a neuropsychological battery, three ALF measures (with free recall probed at 30-min and 1-week), and a modified Autobiographical Memory Interview (MAMI). Performance was compared with a group of healthy age-matched controls. In Study 2, patient CS, who previously experienced baclofen-induced ALF, was followed over 4 years, and re-tested now, 18 months after ceasing baclofen. CS repeated a neuropsychological battery, three ALF experimental tasks (each probed after 30 min and 1 week), and a modified autobiographical interview (AI). Her performance was compared with healthy age-matched controls. RESULTS: On ALF measures, the TEA group performed significantly below controls, but when analysed individually, 4 of the 7 patients who originally showed ALF no longer did so. In two, this was accompanied by improvements in ABM for recent but not remote memory. Patient CS no longer demonstrated ALF on standard lab-based tests and now appeared to retain new episodic autobiographical events with a similar degree of episodic richness as controls. CONCLUSION: Long-term follow up suggests that ALF can resolve, with improvements translating to recent ABM in some cases.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/psychology , Memory Disorders/psychology , Mental Recall/physiology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Epilepsy/psychology , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Memory, Episodic , Neuropsychological Tests , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Time and Motion Studies
19.
Cortex ; 110: 127-140, 2019 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29861041

ABSTRACT

The accelerated forgetting of newly learned information is common amongst patients with epilepsy and, in particular, in the syndrome of transient epileptic amnesia (TEA). However, the neural mechanisms underlying accelerated forgetting are poorly understood. It has been hypothesised that interictal epileptiform activity during longer retention intervals disrupts normally established memory traces. Here, we tested a distinct hypothesis-that accelerated forgetting relates to the abnormal encoding of memories. We studied a group of 15 patients with TEA together with matched, healthy control subjects. Despite normal performance on standard anterograde memory tasks, patients showed accelerated forgetting of a word list over one week. We used a subsequent memory paradigm to compare encoding-related brain activity in patients and controls. Participants studied a series of visually presented scenes whilst undergoing functional MRI scanning. Recognition memory for these scenes was then probed outside the scanner after delays of 45 min and of 4 days. Patients showed poorer memory for the scenes compared with controls. In the patients but not the controls, subsequently forgotten stimuli were associated with reduced hippocampal activation at encoding. Furthermore, patients demonstrated reduced deactivation of posteromedial cortex regions upon viewing subsequently remembered stimuli as compared to subsequently forgotten ones. These data suggest that abnormal encoding-related activity in key memory areas of the brain contributes to accelerated forgetting in TEA. We propose that abnormally encoded memory traces may be particularly vulnerable to interference from subsequently encountered material and hence be forgotten more rapidly. Our results shed light on the mechanisms underlying memory impairment in epilepsy, and offer support to the proposal that accelerated forgetting may be a useful marker of subtle dysfunction in memory-related brain systems.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Epilepsy/physiopathology , Memory/physiology , Aged , Amnesia/diagnosis , Amnesia/psychology , Epilepsy/complications , Female , Humans , Learning/physiology , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychotic Disorders/physiopathology , Psychotic Disorders/psychology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology
20.
Diabetes Care ; 42(2): 215-224, 2019 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30377186

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Central nervous system (CNS) features in children with permanent neonatal diabetes (PNDM) due to KCNJ11 mutations have a major impact on affected families. Sulfonylurea therapy achieves outstanding metabolic control but only partial improvement in CNS features. The effects of KCNJ11 mutations on the adult brain and their functional impact are not well understood. We aimed to characterize the CNS features in adults with KCNJ11 PNDM compared with adults with INS PNDM. RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS: Adults with PNDM due to KCNJ11 mutations (n = 8) or INS mutations (n = 4) underwent a neurological examination and completed standardized neuropsychological tests/questionnaires about development/behavior. Four individuals in each group underwent a brain MRI scan. Test scores were converted to Z scores using normative data, and outcomes were compared between groups. RESULTS: In individuals with KCNJ11 mutations, neurological examination was abnormal in seven of eight; predominant features were subtle deficits in coordination/motor sequencing. All had delayed developmental milestones and/or required learning support/special schooling. Half had features and/or a clinical diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. KCNJ11 mutations were also associated with impaired attention, working memory, and perceptual reasoning and reduced intelligence quotient (IQ) (median IQ KCNJ11 vs. INS mutations 76 vs. 111, respectively; P = 0.02). However, no structural brain abnormalities were noted on MRI. The severity of these features was related to the specific mutation, and they were absent in individuals with INS mutations. CONCLUSIONS: KCNJ11 PNDM is associated with specific CNS features that are not due to long-standing diabetes, persist into adulthood despite sulfonylurea therapy, and represent the major burden from KCNJ11 mutations.


Subject(s)
Behavior/physiology , Central Nervous System/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Diabetes Mellitus/genetics , Diabetes Mellitus/psychology , Potassium Channels, Inwardly Rectifying/genetics , Adolescent , Adult , Autism Spectrum Disorder/diagnosis , Autism Spectrum Disorder/epidemiology , Autism Spectrum Disorder/etiology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Central Nervous System/diagnostic imaging , Central Nervous System/physiopathology , Diabetes Mellitus/diagnosis , Diabetes Mellitus/epidemiology , Female , Humans , Infant, Newborn , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Mutation/physiology , Nervous System Diseases/diagnosis , Nervous System Diseases/epidemiology , Nervous System Diseases/etiology , Neurologic Examination , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...