Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 2 de 2
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Psychiatry Res ; 280: 112517, 2019 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31446216

ABSTRACT

The Dutch version of the Visual Hallucination Questionnaire was used to assess lifetime visual hallucinations (VH) characteristics in 27 patients with psychosis. Our results confirmed substantial variance in many VH characteristics. Most patients reported multiple VH types. Complex VH were most prevalent, mainly consisting of people and animals, followed by simple, then geometric VH. Few patients experienced only simple VH. The VH generally had features resembling real perceptions. Insight was usually reduced. VH ranged from 'appropriate' and neutral to peculiar and delusion-associated. VH accompanied by fright and sound seem to be related to experiencing complex or multiple types of VH.


Subject(s)
Hallucinations/epidemiology , Hallucinations/psychology , Psychotic Disorders/epidemiology , Psychotic Disorders/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Adult , Delusions/diagnosis , Delusions/epidemiology , Delusions/psychology , Female , Hallucinations/diagnosis , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Netherlands/epidemiology , Prevalence , Psychotic Disorders/diagnosis
2.
Psychol Med ; 46(8): 1735-47, 2016 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26984533

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Little is known about visual hallucinations (VH) in psychosis. We investigated the prevalence and the role of bottom-up and top-down processing in VH. The prevailing view is that VH are probably related to altered top-down processing, rather than to distorted bottom-up processing. Conversely, VH in Parkinson's disease are associated with impaired visual perception and attention, as proposed by the Perception and Attention Deficit (PAD) model. Auditory hallucinations (AH) in psychosis, however, are thought to be related to increased attention. METHOD: Our retrospective database study included 1119 patients with non-affective psychosis and 586 controls. The Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences established the VH rate. Scores on visual perception tests [Degraded Facial Affect Recognition (DFAR), Benton Facial Recognition Task] and attention tests [Response Set-shifting Task, Continuous Performance Test-HQ (CPT-HQ)] were compared between 75 VH patients, 706 non-VH patients and 485 non-VH controls. RESULTS: The lifetime VH rate was 37%. The patient groups performed similarly on cognitive tasks; both groups showed worse perception (DFAR) than controls. Non-VH patients showed worse attention (CPT-HQ) than controls, whereas VH patients did not perform differently. CONCLUSIONS: We did not find significant VH-related impairments in bottom-up processing or direct top-down alterations. However, the results suggest a relatively spared attentional performance in VH patients, whereas face perception and processing speed were equally impaired in both patient groups relative to controls. This would match better with the increased attention hypothesis than with the PAD model. Our finding that VH frequently co-occur with AH may support an increased attention-induced 'hallucination proneness'.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Hallucinations/epidemiology , Psychotic Disorders/epidemiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Databases, Factual , Facial Recognition , Female , Hallucinations/physiopathology , Hallucinations/psychology , Humans , Male , Netherlands/epidemiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Prevalence , Psychotic Disorders/psychology , Retrospective Studies , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...