Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 13 de 13
Filter
1.
Child Abuse Negl ; 149: 106699, 2024 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38417291

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Understanding how child maltreatment is passed down from one generation to the next is crucial for the development of intervention and prevention strategies that may break the cycle of child maltreatment. Changes in emotion recognition due to childhood maltreatment have repeatedly been found, and may underly the intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment. OBJECTIVE: In this study we, therefore, examined whether the ability to recognize emotions plays a role in the intergenerational transmission of child abuse and neglect. PARTICIPANTS AND SETTING: A total of 250 parents (104 males, 146 females) were included that participated in a three-generation family study. METHOD: Participants completed an emotion recognition task in which they were presented with series of photographs that depicted the unfolding of facial expressions from neutrality to the peak emotions anger, fear, happiness, and sadness. Multi-informant measures were used to examine experienced and perpetrated child maltreatment. RESULTS: A history of abuse, but not neglect, predicted a shorter reaction time to identify fear and anger. In addition, parents who showed higher levels of neglectful behavior made more errors in identifying fear, whereas parents who showed higher levels of abusive behavior made more errors in identifying anger. Emotion recognition did not mediate the association between experienced and perpetrated child maltreatment. CONCLUSIONS: Findings highlight the importance of distinguishing between abuse and neglect when investigating the precursors and sequalae of child maltreatment. In addition, the effectiveness of interventions that aim to break the cycle of abuse and neglect could be improved by better addressing the specific problems with emotion processing of abusive and neglectful parents.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse , Extended Family , Male , Female , Humans , Child , Emotions , Child Abuse/psychology , Fear/psychology , Anger
2.
J Fam Psychol ; 35(6): 735-744, 2021 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33705174

ABSTRACT

In the current study, the associations between multiple types of child maltreatment (CM), parent-offspring interactions, and family cohesion were examined in an extended family study. A total of 366 parent-offspring pairs from 137 nuclear families participated. Parents (Mage = 52.8 years, age range: 26.6-88.4 years, 57% female) reported about perpetrated CM and offspring (Mage = 25.7 years, range: 7.5-65.5 years, 58% female) about experienced CM during their childhood. Parent-offspring interactions were observed during a conflict interaction task. Cohesion within the nuclear family was observed during a playful tower building task. Results showed that parents and offspring displayed more aversive behavior in parent-offspring dyads characterized by higher levels of child abuse, but not in dyads characterized by higher levels of child neglect. In addition, less dyadic affective similarity was observed in parent-offspring dyads characterized by higher levels of child neglect, whereas dyadic affective similarity was higher in dyads characterized by higher levels of child abuse. Findings imply that interventions focused on parent-offspring interactions with a somewhat different content for neglectful and abusive families may be efficacious for families in the child welfare system. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Child Abuse , Adult , Affect , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Child , Child Welfare , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Parent-Child Relations , Parents
4.
Front Psychiatry ; 11: 387, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32457665

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Experiencing maltreatment during childhood exerts substantial stress on the child and increases the risk for overweight and obesity later in life. The current study tests whether hair cortisol-a measure of chronic stress-and its metabolite cortisone mediate the relation between abuse and neglect on the one hand, and body mass index (BMI) on the other. METHOD: The sample consisted of 249 participants aged 8 to 87 years (M = 36.13, SD = 19.33). We collected data on child abuse and neglect using questionnaires, measured cortisol and cortisone concentrations in hair, and BMI. In a structural model, the effects of abuse and neglect on hair cortisol, hair cortisone, and BMI were tested, as well as the covariance between hair cortisol and BMI, and hair cortisone and BMI. RESULTS: Within the sample, 23% were overweight but not obese and 14% were obese. Higher levels of experienced abuse were related to higher cortisone concentrations in hair (ß = 0.24, p < .001) and higher BMI (ß = 0.17, p =.04). Neglect was not related to hair cortisol, hair cortisone, or BMI. Hair cortisol and cortisone did not mediate the association between maltreatment, and BMI. Sensitivity analyses demonstrate the same pattern of results in a subsample of adult participants currently not living with their parents. However, in younger participants who were still living with their parents, the associations between abuse and cortisone (ß = 0.14, p =.35) and abuse and BMI (ß = 0.02, p =.92) were no longer significant. CONCLUSION: These findings confirm that experiencing abuse is related to higher BMI but suggest that hair cortisol and cortisone are not the mechanism underlying the association between child maltreatment and BMI. This is the first study to show abuse may be associated to elevated concentrations of hair cortisone-evidence of long-term alterations in chronic stress levels. Future research may benefit from exploring the effects of maltreatment on weight gain in longitudinal designs, including measures of other potential mediators such as eating as a coping mechanism, and more direct indicators of metabolic health.

5.
PLoS One ; 15(3): e0225839, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32163421

ABSTRACT

In the current study a three-generational design was used to investigate intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment (ITCM) using multiple sources of information on child maltreatment: mothers, fathers and children. A total of 395 individuals from 63 families reported on maltreatment. Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was used to combine data from mother, father and child about maltreatment that the child had experienced. This established components reflecting the convergent as well as the unique reports of father, mother and child on the occurrence of maltreatment. Next, we tested ITCM using the multi-informant approach and compared the results to those of two more common approaches: ITCM based on one reporter and ITCM based on different reporters from each generation. Results of our multi-informant approach showed that a component reflecting convergence between mother, father, and child reports explained most of the variance in experienced maltreatment. For abuse, intergenerational transmission was consistently found across approaches. In contrast, intergenerational transmission of neglect was only found using the perspective of a single reporter, indicating that transmission of neglect might be driven by reporter effects. In conclusion, the present results suggest that including multiple informants may be necessary to obtain more valid estimates of ITCM.


Subject(s)
Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/psychology , Child Abuse/psychology , Fathers/psychology , Intergenerational Relations , Mother-Child Relations/psychology , Mothers/psychology , Adult , Child , Child, Preschool , Humans
6.
Child Maltreat ; 25(3): 289-299, 2020 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31773993

ABSTRACT

Child-driven genetic factors can contribute to negative parenting and may increase the risk of being maltreated. Experiencing childhood maltreatment may be partly heritable, but results of twin studies are mixed. In the current study, we used a cross-sectional extended family design to estimate genetic and environmental effects on experiencing child maltreatment. The sample consisted of 395 individuals (225 women; Mage = 38.85 years, rangeage = 7-88 years) from 63 families with two or three participating generations. Participants were oversampled for experienced maltreatment. Self-reported experienced child maltreatment was measured using a questionnaire assessing physical and emotional abuse, and physical and emotional neglect. All maltreatment phenotypes were partly heritable with percentages for h2 ranging from 30% (SE = 13%) for neglect to 62% (SE = 19%) for severe physical abuse. Common environmental effects (c2) explained a statistically significant proportion of variance for all phenotypes except for the experience of severe physical abuse (c2 = 9%, SE = 13%, p = .26). The genetic correlation between abuse and neglect was ρg = .73 (p = .02). Common environmental variance increased as socioeconomic status (SES) decreased (p = .05), but additive genetic and unique environmental variances were constant across different levels of SES.


Subject(s)
Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/psychology , Child Abuse/psychology , Family/psychology , Adult , Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/statistics & numerical data , Child , Child Abuse/statistics & numerical data , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Risk Factors
7.
Dev Psychopathol ; 31(1): 157-172, 2019 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30757990

ABSTRACT

Child maltreatment has been associated with various cumulative risk factors. However, little is known about the extent to which genetic and environmental factors contribute to individual differences between parents in perpetrating child maltreatment. To estimate the relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors to perpetrating maltreatment we used a parent-based extended family design. Child-reported perpetrated maltreatment was available for 556 parents (283 women) from 63 families. To explore reporter effects (i.e., child perspective on maltreatment), child reports were compared to multi-informant reports. Based on polygenic model analyses, most of the variance related to the perpetration of physical abuse and emotional neglect was explained by common environmental factors (physical abuse: c2 = 59%, SE = 12%, p = .006; emotional neglect: c2 = 47%, SE = 8%, p < .001) whereas genetic factors did not significantly contribute to the model. For perpetrated emotional abuse, in contrast, genetic factors did significantly contribute to perpetrated emotional abuse (h2 = 33%, SE = 8%, p < .001), whereas common environment factors did not. Multi-informant reports led to similar estimates of genetic and common environmental effects on all measures except for emotional abuse, where a multi-informant approach yielded higher estimates of the common environmental effects. Overall, estimates of unique environment, including measurement error, were lower using multi-informant reports. In conclusion, our findings suggest that genetic pathways play a significant role in perpetrating emotional abuse, while physical abuse and emotional neglect are transmitted primarily through common environmental factors. These findings imply that interventions may need to target different mechanisms dependings on maltreatment type.


Subject(s)
Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/psychology , Child Abuse/psychology , Family/psychology , Gene-Environment Interaction , Adult , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Multifactorial Inheritance , Physical Abuse/psychology , Risk Factors
8.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 103: 266-275, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30754001

ABSTRACT

Altered processing of emotional faces due to childhood maltreatment has repeatedly been reported, and may be a key process underlying the intergenerational transmission of maltreatment. The current study is the first to examine the role of neural reactivity to emotional and neutral faces in the transmission of maltreatment, using a multi-generational family design including 171 participants of 51 families of two generations with a large age range (8-69 years). The impact of experienced and perpetrated maltreatment (abuse and neglect) on face processing was examined in association with activation in the amygdala, hippocampus, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and insula in response to angry, fearful, happy and neutral faces. Results showed enhanced bilateral amygdala activation in response to fearful faces in older neglected individuals, whereas reduced amygdala activation was found in response to these faces in younger neglected individuals. Furthermore, while experienced abuse was associated with lower IFG activation in younger individuals, experience of neglect was associated with higher IFG activation in this age group, pointing to potentially differential effects of abuse and neglect and significant age effects. Perpetrated abusive and neglectful behavior were not related to neural activation in any of these regions. Hence, no indications for a role of neural reactivity to emotional faces in the intergenerational transmission of maltreatment were found.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse/psychology , Emotions/physiology , Facial Recognition/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Amygdala/physiology , Anger , Brain/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Child , Facial Expression , Fear/psychology , Female , Happiness , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology
9.
Dev Psychobiol ; 61(6): 888-902, 2019 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30727029

ABSTRACT

Although childhood maltreatment has been shown to compromise adaptive parental behavior, little is known what happens in terms of physiological regulation when parents with a history of childhood maltreatment interact with their offspring. Using a sample of 229 parents (131 women), the present study examined whether childhood maltreatment experiences are associated with parents' behavioral and autonomic responses while resolving conflict with their offspring. Self-reported experienced child maltreatment was measured using a questionnaire assessing abuse and neglect. Parents (Mage  = 52.7 years, rangeage  = 26.6-88.4 years) and their offspring (Mage  = 24.6 years, rangeage  = 7.5-65.6 years) participated in a videotaped parent-offspring conflict interaction task. Parental warmth, negativity, and emotional support were coded. In addition, their pre-ejection period and respiratory sinus arrhythmia were measured as indicators of underlying sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system reactivity, respectively. Findings demonstrated that experiences of abuse and neglect were associated with behavioral and physiological responses in different ways. Separating these two types of maltreatment in research and in clinical practice might be important.


Subject(s)
Adult Survivors of Child Abuse , Adverse Childhood Experiences , Autonomic Nervous System/physiology , Parent-Child Relations , Parents , Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Adult Children , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Child , Conflict, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
10.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 13(6): 616-627, 2018 06 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29897537

ABSTRACT

Rejection by parents is an important aspect of child maltreatment. Altered neural responses to social rejection have been observed in maltreated individuals. The current study is the first to examine the impact of experienced and perpetrated abuse and neglect on neural responses to social exclusion by strangers versus family using a multigenerational family design, including 144 participants. The role of neural reactivity to social exclusion in the intergenerational transmission of maltreatment was also examined. Exclusion by strangers was especially associated with increased activation in the left insula, while exclusion by a family member was mainly associated with increased activation in the ACC. Neural reactivity to social exclusion by strangers in the insula, ACC and dmPFC, was associated with experienced maltreatment but not with perpetrated maltreatment. In abusive parents, altered neural reactivity during exclusion was found in other brain areas, indicating different neural correlates of experienced and perpetrated maltreatment. Hence, no mechanisms could be identified that are involved in the transmission of maltreatment. Hypersensitivity to social rejection by strangers in neglected individuals underscores the importance to distinguish between effects of abuse and neglect and suggests that the impact of experiencing rejection and maltreatment by your own parents extends beyond the family context.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse/psychology , Family/psychology , Psychological Distance , Adolescent , Adult , Affect , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex , Child , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Parents/psychology , Young Adult
11.
Child Abuse Negl ; 77: 23-34, 2018 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29294414

ABSTRACT

In the current study associations between parents' experiences of childhood maltreatment and their perceptual, behavioral and autonomic responses to infant emotional signals were examined in a sample of 160 parents. Experienced maltreatment (both physical and emotional abuse and neglect) was reported by the participants and, in approximately half of the cases, also by their parents. During a standardized infant vocalization paradigm, participants were asked to squeeze a handgrip dynamometer at maximal and at half strength while listening to infant crying and laughter sounds and to rate their perception of the sounds. In addition, their heart rate (HR), pre-ejection period (PEP), and vagal tone (RSA) were measured as indicators of underlying sympathetic and parasympathetic reactivity. Results indicated that participants did not differ in their perceptions of the infant vocalizations signals according to their maltreatment experiences. However, maltreatment experiences were associated with the modulation of behavioral responses. Experiences of neglect during childhood were related to more handgrip force during infant crying and to less handgrip force during infant laughter. Moreover, a history of neglect was associated with a higher HR and a shorter PEP during the entire infant vocalization paradigm, which may indicate chronic cardiovascular arousal. The findings imply that a history of childhood neglect negatively influences parents' capacities to regulate their emotions and behavior, which would be problematic when reacting to children's emotional expressions.


Subject(s)
Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/psychology , Arousal/physiology , Autonomic Nervous System/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Laughter/psychology , Parent-Child Relations , Adult , Female , Hand Strength , Humans , Infant , Laughter/physiology , Male , Parents/psychology
12.
J Fam Violence ; 32(2): 207-217, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28163367

ABSTRACT

Parent-child agreement on child maltreatment was examined in a multigenerational study. Questionnaires on perpetrated and experienced child maltreatment were completed by 138 parent-child pairs. Multi-level analyses were conducted to explore whether parents and children agreed about levels of parent-to-child maltreatment (convergence), and to examine whether parents and children reported equal levels of child maltreatment (absolute differences). Direct and moderating effects of age and gender were examined as potential factors explaining differences between parent and child report. The associations between parent- and child-reported maltreatment were significant for all subtypes, but the strength of the associations was low to moderate. Moreover, children reported more parent-to-child neglect than parents did. Older participants reported more experienced maltreatment than younger participants, without evidence for differences in actual exposure. These findings support the value of multi-informant assessment of child maltreatment to improve accuracy, but also reveal the divergent perspectives of parents and children on child maltreatment.

13.
Eur J Psychotraumatol ; 8(1): 1380470, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29435199

ABSTRACT

Background: There is an ongoing debate about the validity of the A1 criterion of PTSD. Whereas the DSM-5 has opted for a more stringent A1 criterion, the ICD-11 will leave it out as a key criterion. Objective: Here we investigated whether formal DSM-IV-TR traumatic (A1) and stressful (non-A1) events differ with regard to PTSD symptom profiles, and whether there is a gender difference in this respect. Method: This was examined in a large, mostly clinical sample from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (n = 1433). Participants described their most bothersome (index) event and were assigned to either an A1 or non-A1 event group according to this index event. Results: Remarkably, in men PTSD symptoms were even more severe after non-A1 than A1 events, whereas in women symptoms were equally severe after non-A1 and A1 events. Moreover, while women showed significantly higher PTSD symptoms after A1 events than men (29.9 versus 15.4% met PTSD criteria), there was no gender difference after non-A1 events (women: 28.2%; men: 31.3%). Furthermore, anxiety and perceived impact were higher in women than men, which was associated with PTSD symptom severity. Conclusion: In sum, while women showed similar levels of PTSD symptoms after both event types, men reported even higher levels of PTSD symptoms after non-A1 than A1 events. These findings shed a new light on the role of gender in PTSD symptomatology and the clinical usefulness of the A1 criterion.


Planteamiento: Los recuerdos intrusivos de trauma son un síntoma clave del trastorno por estrés postraumático (TEPT), por lo que interrumpir su recurrencia es muy importante. El desarrollo de las intrusiones fue obstaculizado por intervenciones visuoespaciales administradas hasta 24 horas después del trauma analógico. Se desconoce si las intervenciones pueden ser aplicadas posteriormente, y si la modalidad o la carga de la memoria de trabajo son factores cruciales. Objetivos: Este estudio intenta probar: 1) si una tarea visuoespacial daría lugar a menos intrusiones en comparación con un grupo solamente de reactivación cuando se aplicara después de la reactivación del recuerdo 4 días después de la exposición al trauma analógico (replicación extendida); 2) si ambas tareas)es decir, una dirigida a ser visuoespacial, una más verbal) darían lugar a menos intrusiones que el grupo solamente de reactivación (efecto de la intervención), y 3) si la supuesta modalidad de la tarea (visuoespacial o verbal) es un componente crítico (efecto de la modalidad). Método: Cincuenta y cuatro participantes fueron asignados aleatoriamente a reactivación + Tetris (visuospatial), reactivación + juegos de palabras (verbales), o sólo reactivación (sin tarea). Vieron una película aversiva (día 0) y registraron recuerdos intrusivos de la película en el diario A. El día 4, se reactivó el recuerdo, después de lo cual los participantes jugaron al Tetris, a juegos de palabras, o no hicieron ninguna tarea durante 10 minutos. Luego escribieron en un segundo diario (B). Las hipótesis informativas fueron evaluadas utilizando los factores de Bayes. Resultados: Reactivación + Tetris y reactivación + juegos de palabras resultaron en relativamente menos intrusiones desde el último día del diario A hasta el primer día del diario B que solo reactivación (objetivo 1 y 2). Por tanto, ambas tareas fueron eficaces incluso cuando se aplicaron días después del trauma analógico. La reactivación solo no fue efectiva. Reactivación+juegos de palabras parecen resultar en menos intrusiones que Reactivación+Tetris (objetivo 3, efecto de la modalidad), pero esta evidencia fue débil. Los análisis exploratorios mostraron que los juegos de palabras eran más difíciles que el Tetris. Conclusiones: La aplicación de una tarea 4 días después de la película del trauma (durante la reconsolidación de la memoria) fue eficaz. La modalidad versus el problema de la carga de la memoria de trabajo no es concluyente.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...