Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 45
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Adv Cogn Psychol ; 6: 1-14, 2010 Feb 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20689637

ABSTRACT

When observers are asked to localize the peripheral position of a small probe with respect to the mid-position of a spatially extended comparison stimulus, they tend to judge the probe as being more peripheral than the mid-position of the comparison stimulus. This relative mislocalization seems to emerge from differences in absolute localization, that is the comparison stimulus is localized more towards the fovea than the probe. The present study compared saccadic behaviour and relative localization judgements in three experiments and determined the quantitative relationship between both measures. The results showed corresponding effects in localization errors and saccadic behaviour. Moreover, it was possible to estimate the amount of the relative mislocalization by means of the saccadic amplitude.

2.
Psychol Res ; 67(1): 30-42, 2003 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12589448

ABSTRACT

Stroop dilution refers to the observation that the impact of a color word on the naming of a color bar is reduced if another word-like object is displayed simultaneously. Recently, Brown, Roos-Gilbert, and Carr (1995) concluded that Stroop dilution is due to early-visual interference. This conclusion was evaluated in three experiments. Experiment 1 showed that, contrary to the predictions of an early-visual interference account, (a) diluters that are similar in terms of visual complexity induced different amounts of dilution and (b) the size of the dilution effect is proportional to the size of the Stroop interference effect when the diluters are used as single distractors. Experiments 2 and 3 revealed that when the position of the color bar is pre-cued, Stroop dilution disappears. We argue that these findings support Van der Heijden's (1992) attention-capture account of Stroop dilution.


Subject(s)
Attention , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Perceptual Masking , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Color Perception , Cues , Humans , Models, Psychological , Netherlands , Reaction Time
3.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 27(3): 622-32, 2001 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11424649

ABSTRACT

In comparison with the situation in which target and distractor are presented until response, Stroop interference substantially decreases when the target color is removed from the display 150-160 ms after stimulus onset (W. La Heij, N. A. Kaptein, A. C. Kalff, & L. de Lange, 1995; O. Neumann, 1986). Six experiments showed that this paradoxical exposure-duration effect is robust, that it is not due to the target's exposure duration per se, and that it is not due to an increased spatial selectivity of input selection. The present findings support and extend earlier observations that changes in the display during stimulus exposure affect attentional selection. The findings are tentatively interpreted in terms of (a) reduced duration of input selection or (b) facilitation of attribute selection.


Subject(s)
Attention , Color Perception , Discrimination Learning , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reading , Time Perception , Adult , Female , Field Dependence-Independence , Humans , Male , Psychophysics , Reaction Time , Semantics
4.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 36(3): 199-209, 2000 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10754194

ABSTRACT

The goal of this study was to investigate habituation processes in the brain, and in particular, to identify the brain structures involved in these processes. Therefore, event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded in response to a series of repeated, task-irrelevant, salient stimuli presented against the background of a cognitive task which required the subjects to attend to a fixation sign on a TV screen. Stimuli were presented randomly in one of four possible quadrants of the TV screen. Dipole analyses were performed in order to localize the source of stimulus repetition effects (habituation). As expected, there was a decrease in amplitude of the N1 and P3 components as a function of stimulus repetition; habituation of N1 occurred faster than that of the P3 component. It is suggested from the results of dipole analyses that the N1 effect concerns diffuse cortical activation. The P3 habituation effect seemed to involve the temporal cortex especially.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Habituation, Psychophysiologic/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Electrooculography , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation
5.
Psychol Res ; 64(2): 93-104, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11195309

ABSTRACT

Perceptual inertia refers to a potential explanation for the observation that during a short period after stimulus onset, the visual system is insensitive to discriminatory detail. The present study attempted to replicate this empirical result in a simple one-item letter-identification task. The results provided little if any support for the construct of perceptual inertia. In the five experiments reported, evidence consistent with perceptual inertia was only obtained in Experiment 5. However, the Experiment 5 results can at least equally parsimoniously be explained in terms of two standard explanatory factors: an effect of precuing of the target position (selective attention) and foreperiod duration (general preparation).


Subject(s)
Attention , Perceptual Masking , Visual Perception , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Psychological Theory , Reaction Time
6.
Percept Psychophys ; 61(8): 1646-61, 1999 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10598476

ABSTRACT

We studied the ability to localize flashed stimuli, using a relative judgment task. When observers are asked to localize the peripheral position of a probe with respect to the midposition of a spatially extended comparison stimulus, they tend to judge the probe as being more toward the periphery than is the midposition of the comparison stimulus. We report seven experiments in which this novel phenomenon was explored. They reveal that the mislocalization occurs only when the probe and the comparison stimulus are presented in succession, independent of whether the probe or the comparison stimulus comes first (Experiment 1). The size of the mislocalization is dependent on the stimulus onset asynchrony (Experiment 2) and on the eccentricity of presentation (Experiment 3). In addition, the illusion also occurs in an absolute judgment task, which links mislocalization with the general tendency to judge peripherally presented stimuli as being more foveal than they actually are (Experiment 4). The last three experiments reveal that relative mislocalization is affected by the amount of spatial extension of the comparison stimulus (Experiment 5) and by its structure (Experiments 6 and 7). This pattern of results allows us to evaluate possible explanations of the illusion and to relate it to comparable tendencies observed in eye movement behavior. It is concluded that the system in charge of the guidance of saccadic eye movements is also the system that provides the metric in perceived visual space.


Subject(s)
Attention , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Visual Fields , Adult , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Optical Illusions , Reaction Time , Saccades , Space Perception
7.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 103(1-2): 91-102, 1999 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10555487

ABSTRACT

Shiu and Pashler ((1994). Negligible effect of spatial precuing on identification of single digits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception of Performance, 20, 1037-1054) argue that in single-item identification and localization tasks irrelevant, small, elements can induce costs. For explaining the results, they advance a Noise Reduction explanation as an alternative for the traditional Signal Enhancement explanation. The results of a single-item identification and localization task are reported that investigated the effect of irrelevant, small, elements. The experiment supported the view that the noise elements induce costs. The total pattern of results obtained is compatible with the Signal Enhancement explanation and with an elaborated version of the Noise Reduction explanation. It is argued that an explanation with location selection only, that is, an explanation without a second step with Signal Enhancement or Noise Reduction, can also account for the results.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cues , Noise , Visual Perception/physiology , Humans , Random Allocation
8.
Psychol Res ; 62(1): 20-35, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10356970

ABSTRACT

It has often been reported that, in the presence of static reference stimuli, briefly presented visual targets are perceived as being closer to the fixation point than they actually are. The first purpose of the present study was to investigate whether the same phenomenon can be demonstrated in a situation without static reference stimuli. Experiment 1, with position naming as the task, showed that such a central shift is also observed under these conditions. This finding is of importance because it completes an explanation for central near-location errors in the partial-report bar-probe task. The second purpose of the present study was to provide an explanation for these central shifts. For this explanation information about the exact size of the central shift is required. In Exps. 2, 3, and 4, with cursor setting as the task, it was attempted to assess more precisely the size of the central shifts. These experiments revealed that two different factors determine the results in cursor setting tasks; a factor "target position" and a factor "cursor position." Experiment 5 showed that it is the point of fixation, not the fixation point, that serves, at least in part, as the reference point in this type of task. All the findings together allow us to conclude that the target positions are underestimated by about 10%. From vision research it is known that saccadic eye movements, performed for bringing a target in the fovea, also show an undershoot of about 10%. It is therefore concluded that the system in charge of saccadic eye movements also provides the metric in visual space within a single eye fixation.


Subject(s)
Visual Perception/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Models, Theoretical
9.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol ; 108(2): 110-22, 1998 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9566624

ABSTRACT

A sudden visual onset is thought to 'attract attention to its location' within less than 100 ms. We attempted to measure the effect of this attentional process on the event-related potential (ERP) to a probe presented about 140 ms after the onset, and to delineate the spatiotemporal characteristics of such an effect, if any. ERPs were recorded from 30 channels from 6 subjects while they performed a target detection task. Both targets and probes could be located in each of the 4 quadrants (eccentricities 6.1 degrees and 7 degrees, respectively). For a given single target, the subsequent probe was either presented near the location of the target ('valid target') or at the diagonal opposite ('invalid target'). Appropriate 'neutral' conditions (probes preceded by no target, or by simultaneous targets in all quadrants) were applied, and ERPs to probes were corrected for the contribution of the ERPs to targets. The earliest effect of (in)validity was found at about 120 ms after probe onset for lower field probes. This effect consisted of enhanced posterior positivity for valid relative to neutral relative to invalid conditions. This positivity was superposed on a contralateral, extrastriate negative ongoing wave peaking at about 150 ms ('N150'). Source localization suggested that the (in)validity effects originate from deep medial parietal areas. The source corresponding to the N150 activity was not influenced by (in)validity. An earlier deflection to the probe at 80 ms ('NP80') depended on location, but not on (in)validity, and seemed to be of striate origin. Results are discussed in terms of a model postulating an attention-independent 'input module' from which activation is fed to a 'location module' embodying the actual attention mechanism.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Electrodes/standards , Electroencephalography , Electrooculography , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Reproducibility of Results
10.
Mem Cognit ; 25(5): 641-52, 1997 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9337582

ABSTRACT

In studies using Averbach and Coriell's (1961) partial-report bar-probe paradigm with linear arrays, most errors involve the naming of an item that was in the display but in a position other than the cued one. Up to now, there is no general agreement on the origin of these location errors. Point of departure in this paper is that part of the location errors arises from inappropriate application of the cue. It is tested whether this originates from problems to perceive the position of the cue ("cue-displacement hypothesis") or from confusion about the order of the items in the array ("item-order hypothesis"). The results of two bar-probe experiments are reported. A novel, crucial, finding in both experiments is that, among the location errors, there was a preponderance of response letters that came from the central side of the cued item. In the second experiment, this was observed not only in the usual postcue conditions but also when the cue preceded the array. These results positively corroborate the cue-displacement hypothesis and do not support the item-order hypothesis: The cue tends to be perceived more toward the center of the visual field than it actually is exposed-that is, there is a central drift of the cue.


Subject(s)
Attention , Discrimination Learning , Optical Illusions , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Psychophysics , Visual Fields
11.
Conscious Cogn ; 6(2-3): 413-28, 1997.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9262419

ABSTRACT

Everybody knows the phenomena summarized with the term attention: concentration, focalization, limitation, selection, and intensification (see, e.g., James, 1890/1950). The explanation of these phenomena is, however, a different matter. Problems easily arise with regard to what has to be explained and with regard to the style of explanation. A problem of the first kind is the "methodology of 'bad focus'": the explanation starts with and is fixated on an intuitively striking but nonessential behavioral feature or cognitive achievement. A problem of the second kind is a "virtus dormitiva" explanation: the explanation starts with emphasizing one aspect of the observed phenomena, the emphasized aspect receives an interesting and suggestive name, and that name with its connotations is used as a concept in the explanation. At the start of contemporary, behavior-based, information processing psychology, a virtus dormitiva explanation infiltrated the functional accounts of the phenomena of attention; the empirical observation that people show performance limitations was translated into the theoretical concept of a communication channel with a limited capacity. That limited capacity notion became the core concept in what can be called the standard theory of attention. This standard theory of attention faced severe difficulties in explaining the guidance of attention by the information processor's goals and intentions. Subsequent modifications, concerned with removing these difficulties, revealed that selection, guided by goals and intentions, is the essential behavioral feature and that the observed performance limitations are a result of this selection. So, the limited capacity theorizing was not only plagued by a virtus dormitiva explanation, it also suffered from the methodology of bad focus.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Humans , Visual Perception
12.
Conscious Cogn ; 6(2-3): 437-40, 1997.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9262421
13.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 23(6): 1798-812, 1997 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9425682

ABSTRACT

M. J. Nissen (1985) showed that color and shape are processed independently. M. A. Monheit and J. C. Johnston (1994) argued that dependence of color and shape in these experiments was reduced due to random guessing. They conducted 4 experiments in which they attempted to reduce the effect of random guessing. A strong dependence of color and shape reports was observed. In this article the effects that random guessing can have on statistical (in)dependence are analyzed. The authors argue that Monheit and Johnston's analysis is incorrect and inconsistent with the data they report and that random guessing can be the cause of statistical dependence of color and shape reports.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Form Perception , Attention , Humans , Models, Psychological
14.
Percept Psychophys ; 58(8): 1224-37, 1996 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8961833

ABSTRACT

In contemporary theorizing, there is a controversy about the role of spatial location in the selection of visual information; some theories postulate that position plays a unique role, whereas other theories hold that position is just one selection dimension that is not different from other dimensions, such as color and shape. In this context, a paradigm introduced by Tsal and Lavie (1988) promised to be of fundamental importance. With that paradigm, Tsal and Lavie found that, after reporting a first letter of a prespecified color, subjects preferred to switch their reporting to letters from array positions adjacent to that letter over continuing to report letters of the same color as that of the first letter. This switch from color to position provided firm evidence in favor of the "position-special" views as opposed to the "all-attributes-are-equal" views. In the present study, six experiments, employing Tsal and Lavie's paradigm and variations of that paradigm, are reported. Experiments 1,2,4, and 5 show that evidence for a switch from selection on the basis of color to selection on the basis of position is not obtained when subjects are forced to fixate the fixation point and possibly also not under normal contrast conditions without fixation controls. Experiment 3 shows that switching from color to position is difficult. Experiments 2, 5, and 6 show that evidence for a switch is obtained only under low-contrast conditions when subjects are not forced to fixate the fixation point. It is concluded that the Tsal and Lavie paradigm is an asymmetric paradigm. The results reported by Tsal and Lavie constituted a major threat for the "all-attributes-are-equal" theories and provided firm support for the "position-special" theories. The results reported in the present study are compatible with the all-attributes-are-equal theories, but, at the same time, do not constitute a major threat for the contemporary position-special theories.


Subject(s)
Attention , Color Perception , Fixation, Ocular , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation , Psychophysics
15.
Percept Psychophys ; 53(3): 345-9; discussion 350-2, 1993 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8483698

ABSTRACT

Nissen (1985) presented results showing that in partial-report experiments, selection by location was different from selection by color or by shape. Bundesen (1991) presented a reinterpretation of these results, claiming that they can also be explained by a model in which location selection is treated on a par with selection by color or by shape. We will show that the analysis given by Bundesen is based on a special relation between an "element" in the visual field and its location, which in fact reduces the expressions given by Bundesen to those given by Nissen. Furthermore, we will show that in Bundesen's theory, it is assumed that in the process of perceptual categorization an element in the visual field can be referred to without the use of one of its attributes. This assumption is untenable from a theoretical point of view. In all, the results presented by Nissen can still be regarded as evidence for the difference between selection by location and selection by color or by shape.


Subject(s)
Attention , Color Perception , Form Perception , Visual Fields , Discrimination Learning , Humans , Orientation , Psychophysics
16.
Psychol Res ; 56(1): 44-58, 1993.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8310104

ABSTRACT

Within contemporary visual-information-processing psychology, two classes of selective-attention theories can be distinguished: position-not-special theories and position-special theories. The position-not-special theories postulate that attentional selection by colour, by form, and by position are equivalent selective operations. The position-special theories assume that selection by position is more basic or direct than selection by colour or by form. Examples of both types of theory are briefly described, and irrelevant and relevant evidence is critically discussed. It is concluded that the relevant evidence is directly compatible with the position-special views and that the position-not-special theories require additional extraneous assumptions. The position-special model presented in Van der Heijden (1992) is elaborated in further detail. It is shown that this model is compatible with two important and often substantiated assumptions of the position-not-special theories: the assumption that pre-attentive analysers organize the visual scene in objects against a background, and the assumption that visual-selective attention can be directed at objects isolated in this way. This position-special theory is a parsimonious theory because it can identify the mentalistic concept selective attention with the materialistic concept spatial position.


Subject(s)
Attention , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Space Perception , Color Perception , Discrimination Learning , Humans , Psychophysics
17.
Cogn Psychol ; 22(3): 273-341, 1990 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2376113

ABSTRACT

SLAM, the SeLective Attention Model, performs visual selective attention tasks, an analysis of which shows that two processes, object and attribute selection, are both necessary and sufficient. It is based upon the McClelland and Rumelhart (1981) model for visual word recognition, with the addition of a response selection and evaluation mechanism. The responses may be correct or incorrect and, in particular conditions, SLAM may not make a response at all. Moreover, it allows for the generation of specific responses in time. SLAM's main characteristics are parallelism restricted by competition within modules, heterarchical processing in a hierarchical structure, and generation of responses as a result of relaxation given the conjoint constraints of stimulation, object, and attribute selection. The model is considered to represent an individual subject performing filtering tasks and demonstrates appropriate selective behavior. It is also tested quantitatively using a single tentative set of model parameters. The study reports simulations of four different filtering experiments, modeling response latencies, and error proportions. Specifications are made to take account of instructions, previous trials, and the effect of a barmarker cue and of asynchronies in stimulus and cue onsets. The model is then extended in order to provide simulations of a number of Stroop experiments, which can be regarded as filtering tasks with nonequivalent stimuli. The extension required for Stroop simulations is the addition of direct connections between compatible stimulus and response aspects. The direct connections do not affect the simulation of simpler filtering tasks. A variety of different experiments carried out by different authors is simulated. The model is discussed in terms of how modular architecture and the interaction of excitation and inhibition generate facilitation or inhibition of response latencies.


Subject(s)
Arousal , Attention , Discrimination Learning , Form Perception , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Psychomotor Performance , Computer Simulation , Humans , Psychophysics , Reaction Time
18.
Psychol Res ; 52(1): 13-21, 1990.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2377720

ABSTRACT

In the literature two different views on the time course of activation of "recognition units"--hypothetical units that represent identity information--are proposed. The first view assumes that the recognition units accumulate activation gradually over time. The second assumes that the activation levels increase fast initially and then start to decrease. Hagenzieker and Van der Heijden (1990) have proposed a more general information-processing model, which is compatible with both views. In this model a threshold determines what type of time course will be obtained. For below-threshold-activation values a gradual accumulation of activation will be found; for above-threshold values activation levels will first increase and then, because of inhibition, decrease. This model served as a starting-point for the present paper. Elaboration of this model led to two testable hypotheses with regard to identification performance. The first prediction is that evidence for inhibition should be observed only under relatively bright luminance conditions. The second is that, given inhibition, the amount of the effect of this inhibition should increase over time. A partial-report bar-probe task was used to test the predictions. The experiment involved two luminance conditions and the information was sampled at two different moments in time. To control for possible localization artefacts a digit-naming task was also used. Evidence for inhibition was found in both luminance conditions. In accordance with the model the amount of the effect of inhibition increased over time.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Attention/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Humans , Light , Models, Neurological , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Psychophysics
19.
Psychol Res ; 52(2-3): 197-206, 1990.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2281128

ABSTRACT

The information-processing (IP) approach to perception and cognition arose as a reaction to behaviourism. This reaction mainly concerned the nature of explanation in scientific psychology. The "standard" account of behaviour, phrased in strictly external terms, was replaced by a "realist" account, phrased in terms of internal entities and processes. An analysis of the theoretical language used in IP psychology shows an undisciplined state of affairs. A great number of languages is simultaneously in use; no level of analysis is unambiguously referred to; and basic concepts such as information and processing remain largely undefined. Nevertheless, over the past 25 years the IP approach has developed into a disciplined and sophisticated experimental science. A look at actual practice hints at the basic reason for its success. The approach is not so much concerned with absolute or intrinsic properties of the human information processor, but with what can be called its relative or differential properties. A further analysis of this feature of the IP approach in terms of the formal language of a logical system makes explicit the basis of its success. The IP approach can be regarded as developing an empirical difference calculus on an unspecified class of objects, phrased in terms of a simulated "theory-neutral" observation language, and with operators that are structurally analogous to logical operators. This reinterpretation of what the IP approach is about brings a number of advantages. It strengthens its position as an independent science, clarifies its relation with other approaches within psychology and other sciences within the cognitive science group, and makes it independent of philosophical subtleties.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Information Theory , Perception , Humans , Logic , Psycholinguistics
20.
Psychol Res ; 52(1): 5-12, 1990.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2198608

ABSTRACT

In the literature two different views with regard to the time course of activation of "recognition units"--hypothetical entities that represent identity information--are proposed. Both views are derived from a restricted set of phenomena in visual perception. The first view gets its intuitions from phenomena such as those summarized by Bloch's law; these phenomena show integration or summation of activation over time. This "grow-and-grow" view assumes that recognition units accumulate activation gradually over time. The second view finds its inspiration in phenomena such as Mach-bands and simultaneous contrast; these phenomena show inhibition or steady-state suppression over time. This "grow-and-shrink" view assumes that the activation levels increase fast initially and then start to decrease. The perception literature strongly suggests that it may be light intensity that determines whether mainly integration (and phenomena such as Bloch's law) or inhibition (and phenomena such as Mach-bands) is obtained. So it is also likely that both information-processing views apply only to a limited range of intensity conditions; the "grow-and-grow" view to conditions of low-light intensities, and the "grow-and-shrink" view to conditions of intermediate- and high-light intensities. It is argued that, to arrive at a complete view, a visual phenomenon that applies to the complete range of light intensities should be taken as a background for generating hypotheses about time courses in visual-information processing. The Broca-Sulzer effect is such a phenomenon. Starting from this phenomenon a simple, synthesizing, information-processing model is derived that is compatible with both the "grow-and-grow" view and the "grow-and-shrink" view.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Attention/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Humans , Models, Neurological , Psychophysics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...