Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 6 de 6
Filter
Add more filters










Language
Publication year range
1.
Podium (Pinar Río) ; 18(3)dic. 2023.
Article in Spanish | LILACS-Express | LILACS | ID: biblio-1521352

ABSTRACT

El profesional de cultura física contribuye, con aportes importantes, al perfeccionamiento de los programas; aplica a la educación el ejercicio físico desde las primeras edades y favorece la formación integral. El objetivo de este trabajo consistió en diseñar una metodología para el desarrollo de las cualidades coordinativas básicas en la Educación Física del nivel inicial 2. Para ello, se realizó un estudio de diseño preexperimental de control mínimo, con dos años de duración (2020-2021). La muestra seleccionada respondió a un muestreo intencional estratificado, no probabilístico. Se emplearon métodos de los niveles teórico y empírico, tales como: análisis y síntesis, inductivo-deductivo, sistémico-estructural funcional, medición, experimento, criterio de expertos y estadística descriptiva, así como técnicas de investigación entre los que destacan, análisis documental, observación estructurada, entrevista y encuesta. La metodología se realizó mediante la implementación de ejercicios físicos. Participaron 229 niños del nivel inicial 2; a partir del diagnóstico realizado se determinó que el equilibrio dinámico (46,3 %) y la coordinación dinámica general (43,7%) fueron evaluados de regular, además de dificultades en todos los patrones de movimiento, la motricidad gruesa, la fina, los reflejos, las reacciones y patrones posturales. El estudio mostró que se puede mejorar el desarrollo de las cualidades coordinativas básicas en la Educación Física del nivel inicial 2.


O profissional de cultura física contribui, com contribuições importantes, para o aprimoramento dos programas; Aplica o exercício físico à educação desde tenra idade e promove uma formação integral. O objetivo deste trabalho foi desenhar uma metodologia para o desenvolvimento de qualidades coordenativas básicas em Educação Física do nível inicial 2. Para tanto, foi realizado um estudo de desenho pré-experimental de controle mínimo, com duração de dois anos (2020-2021). ). ). A amostra selecionada respondeu a uma amostragem estratificada e não probabilística intencional. Foram utilizados métodos dos níveis teórico e empírico tais como: análise e síntese indutivo-dedutivo sistêmico-estrutural-funcional medição experimento julgamento de especialistas e estatística descritiva bem como técnicas de pesquisa dentre as quais se destacam a análise documental observação estruturada, entrevista e inquérito. A metodologia foi realizada por meio da execução de exercícios físicos. Participaram 229 crianças do nível inicial 2; A partir do diagnóstico realizado, constatou-se que o equilíbrio dinâmico (46,3%) e a coordenação dinâmica geral (43,7%) foram avaliados como regulares, além de dificuldades em todos os padrões de movimento, motricidade grossa e fina, reflexos, reações e padrões posturais. O estudo mostrou que o desenvolvimento das qualidades coordenativas básicas pode ser melhorado na Educação Física no nível inicial 2.


The physical culture professional contributes, with important contributions, to the improvement of the programs; applies physical exercise to education, from the earliest ages and promotes comprehensive training. The objective of this work was to design a methodology for the development of basic coordinative qualities in Physical Education of the initial level 2. To this end, a pre-experimental design study of minimum control was carried out, lasting two years (2020-2021). The selected sample responded to a stratified, non-probabilistic intentional sampling. Methods from the theoretical and empirical levels were used, such as: analysis and synthesis, inductive-deductive, systemic-structural-functional, measurement, experiment, expert judgment and descriptive statistics, as well as research techniques, among which stand out documentary analysis, structured observation, interview and survey. The methodology was carried out through the implementation of physical exercises. 229 children from initial level 2 participated. Based on the diagnosis made, it was determined that dynamic balance (46.3 %) and general dynamic coordination (43.7 %) were evaluated as fair, in addition to difficulties in all movement patterns, gross and fine motor skills, reflexes, reactions and postural patterns. The study showed that the development of basic coordinative qualities can be improved in Physical Education at initial level 2.

2.
La Paz; s.n; 2020. 200 p. Tab. (T-142).
Thesis in Spanish | LIBOCS | ID: biblio-1344686

ABSTRACT

El trabajo de investigación tiene como propósito identificar los recursos estratégicos en la escucha empatica a través del análisis del abordaje corporal de las educadoras de nivel inicial en la Unidad Educativa Hermana María Teresa la Justicia Fe y Alegria de la ciudad de El Alto.


Subject(s)
Empathy
3.
Interdisciplinaria ; 31(1): 25-37, jun. 2014. tab
Article in Spanish | LILACS | ID: lil-734347

ABSTRACT

El trabajo que se informa se propuso explorar las diferencias individuales en las habilidades asociadas al aprendizaje de letras en 50 niños de 5 años de edad y de nivel socioeconómico bajo. Motivó este estudio la evidencia existente acerca de las dificultades que el dominio de las letras representa para parte de los niños en contextos de pobreza. A fin de identificar las variables que diferenciaban a los niños con mayor o menor facilidad para el aprendizaje de letras, se administraron pruebas que evalúan sensibilidad fonológica, memoria visual, memoria fonológica y denominación rápida. A continuación, se llevó adelante una situación experimental de aprendizaje de letras desconocidas, a las que se asignaron pseudo -nombres elaborados en base a las características de los nombres de las letras en español. En dos sesiones se presentaron cuatro letras (dos por sesión) y se evaluó la capacidad de los niños para reconocerlas por su nombre. En función del desempeño en la situación de aprendizaje de letras se dividió al grupo en niños con alto y bajo nivel de desempeño y se comparó a los subgrupos en las restantes habilidades evaluadas. La única tarea en la que se obtuvieron diferencias significativas fue en la prueba de denominación rápida de objetos, tarea que capturaría el componente de asociación visual-verbal del aprendizaje de letras. Se analizan las implicancias educativas de estos hallazgos.


Together with phonological sensitivity, letter knowledge has been shown to be one of the best predictors of reading acquisition (Lerväg, Bratën & Hulme, 2009; Muter, Hulme, Snowling & Stevenson, 2004). But letter learning represents a considerable challenge for many children growing in poverty contexts, as evidenced by studies conducted with both selected and unselected samples of different ages (Diuk, Serrano & Ferroni, 2013; Duncan & Seymour, 2000). Even though letter knowledge has been largely attributed to experiential factors such as learning opportunities provided by a child's home or school, there is growing evidence that individual differences in several cognitive and linguistic variables are related to the ease with which children learn letter names or sounds. This paper aimed at exploring letter learning in 50 Argentine, 5-year-old children growing in poverty. Children were tested towards the end of their kindergarten year. An experimental training situation was adapted from Levin, Shatil-Carmon and Asif-Rave (2006). In this situation children were presented with unknown letters (letters from the Cyrillic alphabet). Pseudonames for these letters were created attending to the characteristics of letter names in Spanish. Training involved a card game consisting of a mixed pile of letter cards and picture cards. In each round of the game, the child uncovered one card after the other from the pile and, when a letter card turned up, he or she was asked to provide the letter's name. Children were presented with four letters, two per session. A point was awarded when the child could accurately name each letter. In order to identify cognitive and linguistic variables which might establish individual differences among children in the ease of acquiring letter names, before the training situation children were given tests evaluating phonological sensitivity (syllable and initial sound recognition), phonological memory (digit span and pseudoword repetition), visual memory and rapid naming (RAN) of objects, colors and digits. Based on the scores in the letter learning situation, two subgroups of children were established. The high- and the low - performing subgroups were then compared on the cognitive and linguistic variables previously examined. No differences between groups were obtained in either phonological sensitivity test, in phonological memory (digit span or pseudoword repetition) or visual memory. Statistically significant differences were only found in the rapid naming of objects (results from the rapid naming of colors and digits tests were excluded from analyses given that a high percentage of the sample could not name all five colors or numbers included in the task). The precise cognitive substrate tapped by rapid naming tasks has given rise to considerable debate in the field of Reading Psychology, but there is a growing consensus that these tasks probably capture the efficiency of basic neural mechanisms participating in the establishment of visual-verbal associations (Lervåg & Hulme, 2009). In terms of educational implications, rapid naming has proven to be hard to improve (de Jong & Vrielink, 2004). However, abilities strongly associated with RAN, such as reading fluency, can be enhanced through practice, even if rapid naming itself doesn't improve. RAN performance could consequently be considered an indication of the intensity of the intervention a child will need to achieve learning. Given that many low-income children have few opportunities for learning letter names and sounds in their communities, the fact that current kindergarten curricula do not include teaching letters may put many children in the situation of having to acquire alphabet knowledge in a short period of time once they enter 1st grade. Individual differences in rapid naming can thus be expected to play a role in the ease with which children learn in such a demanding context.

4.
Interdisciplinaria ; 31(1): 25-37, jun. 2014. tab
Article in Spanish | BINACIS | ID: bin-131480

ABSTRACT

El trabajo que se informa se propuso explorar las diferencias individuales en las habilidades asociadas al aprendizaje de letras en 50 niños de 5 años de edad y de nivel socioeconómico bajo. Motivó este estudio la evidencia existente acerca de las dificultades que el dominio de las letras representa para parte de los niños en contextos de pobreza. A fin de identificar las variables que diferenciaban a los niños con mayor o menor facilidad para el aprendizaje de letras, se administraron pruebas que evalúan sensibilidad fonológica, memoria visual, memoria fonológica y denominación rápida. A continuación, se llevó adelante una situación experimental de aprendizaje de letras desconocidas, a las que se asignaron pseudo -nombres elaborados en base a las características de los nombres de las letras en español. En dos sesiones se presentaron cuatro letras (dos por sesión) y se evaluó la capacidad de los niños para reconocerlas por su nombre. En función del desempeño en la situación de aprendizaje de letras se dividió al grupo en niños con alto y bajo nivel de desempeño y se comparó a los subgrupos en las restantes habilidades evaluadas. La única tarea en la que se obtuvieron diferencias significativas fue en la prueba de denominación rápida de objetos, tarea que capturaría el componente de asociación visual-verbal del aprendizaje de letras. Se analizan las implicancias educativas de estos hallazgos.(AU)


Together with phonological sensitivity, letter knowledge has been shown to be one of the best predictors of reading acquisition (Lervõg, BratÙn & Hulme, 2009; Muter, Hulme, Snowling & Stevenson, 2004). But letter learning represents a considerable challenge for many children growing in poverty contexts, as evidenced by studies conducted with both selected and unselected samples of different ages (Diuk, Serrano & Ferroni, 2013; Duncan & Seymour, 2000). Even though letter knowledge has been largely attributed to experiential factors such as learning opportunities provided by a childs home or school, there is growing evidence that individual differences in several cognitive and linguistic variables are related to the ease with which children learn letter names or sounds. This paper aimed at exploring letter learning in 50 Argentine, 5-year-old children growing in poverty. Children were tested towards the end of their kindergarten year. An experimental training situation was adapted from Levin, Shatil-Carmon and Asif-Rave (2006). In this situation children were presented with unknown letters (letters from the Cyrillic alphabet). Pseudonames for these letters were created attending to the characteristics of letter names in Spanish. Training involved a card game consisting of a mixed pile of letter cards and picture cards. In each round of the game, the child uncovered one card after the other from the pile and, when a letter card turned up, he or she was asked to provide the letters name. Children were presented with four letters, two per session. A point was awarded when the child could accurately name each letter. In order to identify cognitive and linguistic variables which might establish individual differences among children in the ease of acquiring letter names, before the training situation children were given tests evaluating phonological sensitivity (syllable and initial sound recognition), phonological memory (digit span and pseudoword repetition), visual memory and rapid naming (RAN) of objects, colors and digits. Based on the scores in the letter learning situation, two subgroups of children were established. The high- and the low - performing subgroups were then compared on the cognitive and linguistic variables previously examined. No differences between groups were obtained in either phonological sensitivity test, in phonological memory (digit span or pseudoword repetition) or visual memory. Statistically significant differences were only found in the rapid naming of objects (results from the rapid naming of colors and digits tests were excluded from analyses given that a high percentage of the sample could not name all five colors or numbers included in the task). The precise cognitive substrate tapped by rapid naming tasks has given rise to considerable debate in the field of Reading Psychology, but there is a growing consensus that these tasks probably capture the efficiency of basic neural mechanisms participating in the establishment of visual-verbal associations (LervÕg & Hulme, 2009). In terms of educational implications, rapid naming has proven to be hard to improve (de Jong & Vrielink, 2004). However, abilities strongly associated with RAN, such as reading fluency, can be enhanced through practice, even if rapid naming itself doesnt improve. RAN performance could consequently be considered an indication of the intensity of the intervention a child will need to achieve learning. Given that many low-income children have few opportunities for learning letter names and sounds in their communities, the fact that current kindergarten curricula do not include teaching letters may put many children in the situation of having to acquire alphabet knowledge in a short period of time once they enter 1st grade. Individual differences in rapid naming can thus be expected to play a role in the ease with which children learn in such a demanding context.(AU)

5.
Investig. psicol ; 8(1): 7-29, 2003. ilus
Article in Spanish | LILACS | ID: lil-730920

ABSTRACT

El objetivo del presente trabajo es investigar el nivel de comprensión operatoria de la noción de número en niños ingresantes a la escuela primaria. Para ello fueron diseñadas actividades para indagar las habilidades que traen del nivel inicial según lo indica el curriculum de dicho nivel. Dichas actividades requerían el dominio de nociones tales como la de cardinalidad mediante el conteo y la de ordinalidad mediante representaciones secuenciales y comparación de cantidades. Estas tareas fueron examinadas mediante la presentación de tres estímulos, registrándose en cada uno de ellos las respuestas de los niños. Se administraron a veintiocho alumnos de una escuela pública de nivel socioeconómico medio-bajo que habían cursado el preescolar. Se caracterizaron las conductas cognitivas y se analizaron las variables intervinientes. Se puede advertir la relación existente entre aspectos cognitivos y aspectos de personalidad tales como la seguridad e inseguridad en las respuestas y en las confrontaciones con otras opiniones. Se observó que, independientemente de las características de personalidad de los niños, a mayor nivel de estructuración cognitiva se tiende a manifestar mayor seguridad, aspecto éste que contribuye a la autonomía de pensamiento, valor fundamental de la educación.


Subject(s)
Humans , Child, Preschool , Child , Education, Primary and Secondary , Schools, Nursery , Educational Status , Mathematics
6.
Investig. psicol ; 7(3): 7-30, 2002. tab
Article in Spanish | LILACS | ID: lil-728005

ABSTRACT

El presente trabajo tiene como objetivo un estudio exploratorio y descriptivo de la comprensión operativa de la noción de número en alumnos que cursan el primer grado de la escuela primaria. Se desea estudiar la incidencia que posee en la resolución de situaciones problemáticas, distintos aspectos de la noción de número: la construcción de la secuencia numérica, la conservación de la cantidad, el aspecto ordinal y cardinal del número, la comparación de cantidades y la realización de operaciones. Se caracterizó el perfil cognitivo de la construcción del número del alumno que cursa el primer grado, lo que permitió definir las tareas propuestas y confeccionar protocolos específicos para la observación de dichas tareas. A partir de los resultados obtenidos sobre una muestra de veintiocho alumnos de primer grado de una escuela pública de la Ciudad de Buenos Aires de nivel socioeconómico medio-bajo se categorizaron las conductas cognitivas. Así mismo, se establecieron niveles de estructura cognitiva de complejidad creciente. Para la primera de las actividades planteadas se encontró que entre los procesos de anticipación de las acciones y la posterior realización experimental hubo invariancia en los niveles más altos y aprendizaje en los más bajos. Para la segunda actividad se encontró a nivel exploratorio la oposición de las variables aprendizaje y dificultad prevaleciendo el efecto de esta última en los resultados.


Subject(s)
Humans , Child , Cognition , Education, Primary and Secondary , Social Class , Argentina , Mathematics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...