Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 26
Filter
2.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 44-49, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28993236

ABSTRACT

The human brain can process facial expressions of emotions rapidly and without awareness. Several studies in patients with damage to their primary visual cortices have shown that they may be able to guess the emotional expression on a face despite their cortical blindness. This non-conscious processing, called affective blindsight, may arise through an intact subcortical visual route that leads from the superior colliculus to the pulvinar, and thence to the amygdala. This pathway is thought to process the crude visual information conveyed by the low spatial frequencies of the stimuli. In order to investigate whether this is the case, we studied a patient (TN) with bilateral cortical blindness and affective blindsight. An fMRI paradigm was performed in which fearful and neutral expressions were presented using faces that were either unfiltered, or filtered to remove high or low spatial frequencies. Unfiltered fearful faces produced right amygdala activation although the patient was unaware of the presence of the stimuli. More importantly, the low spatial frequency components of fearful faces continued to produce right amygdala activity while the high spatial frequency components did not. Our findings thus confirm that the visual information present in the low spatial frequencies is sufficient to produce affective blindsight, further suggesting that its existence could rely on the subcortical colliculo-pulvino-amygdalar pathway.


Subject(s)
Affect , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Amygdala/diagnostic imaging , Amygdala/physiopathology , Blindness, Cortical/diagnostic imaging , Facial Expression , Facial Recognition , Fear/psychology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Stroke/complications , Stroke/psychology , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Visual Pathways/physiopathology , Visual Perception
3.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 315-324, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29237554

ABSTRACT

Training chronic, cortically-blind (CB) patients on a coarse [left-right] direction discrimination and integration (CDDI) task recovers performance on this task at trained, blind field locations. However, fine direction difference (FDD) thresholds remain elevated at these locations, limiting the usefulness of recovered vision in daily life. Here, we asked if this FDD impairment can be overcome by training CB subjects with endogenous, feature-based attention (FBA) cues. Ten CB subjects were recruited and trained on CDDI and FDD with an FBA cue or FDD with a neutral cue. After completion of each training protocol, FDD thresholds were re-measured with both neutral and FBA cues at trained, blind-field locations and at corresponding, intact-field locations. In intact portions of the visual field, FDD thresholds were lower when tested with FBA than neutral cues. Training subjects in the blind field on the CDDI task improved FDD performance to the point that a threshold could be measured, but these locations remained impaired relative to the intact field. FDD training with neutral cues resulted in better blind field FDD thresholds than CDDI training, but thresholds remained impaired relative to intact field levels, regardless of testing cue condition. Importantly, training FDD in the blind field with FBA lowered FDD thresholds relative to CDDI training, and allowed the blind field to reach thresholds similar to the intact field, even when FBA trained subjects were tested with a neutral rather than FBA cue. Finally, FDD training appeared to also recover normal integration thresholds at trained, blind-field locations, providing an interesting double dissociation with respect to CDDI training. In summary, mechanisms governing FBA appear to function normally in both intact and impaired regions of the visual field following V1 damage. Our results mark the first time that FDD thresholds in CB fields have been seen to reach intact field levels of performance. Moreover, FBA can be leveraged during visual training to recover normal, fine direction discrimination and integration performance at trained, blind-field locations, potentiating visual recovery of more complex and precise aspects of motion perception in cortically-blinded fields.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Discrimination, Psychological , Adult , Aged , Attention , Blindness, Cortical/diagnostic imaging , Blindness, Cortical/rehabilitation , Cues , Female , Humans , Learning , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Motion Perception , Orientation , Recovery of Function , Stroke/complications , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Stroke Rehabilitation , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Visual Fields , Young Adult
4.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 150-165, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29753019

ABSTRACT

Patients with injury to early visual cortex or its inputs can display the Riddoch phenomenon: preserved awareness for moving but not stationary stimuli. We provide a detailed case report of a patient with the Riddoch phenomenon, MC. MC has extensive bilateral lesions to occipitotemporal cortex that include most early visual cortex and complete blindness in visual field perimetry testing with static targets. Nevertheless, she shows a remarkably robust preserved ability to perceive motion, enabling her to navigate through cluttered environments and perform actions like catching moving balls. Comparisons of MC's structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data to a probabilistic atlas based on controls reveals that MC's lesions encompass the posterior, lateral, and ventral early visual cortex bilaterally (V1, V2, V3A/B, LO1/2, TO1/2, hV4 and VO1 in both hemispheres) as well as more extensive damage to right parietal (inferior parietal lobule) and left ventral occipitotemporal cortex (VO1, PHC1/2). She shows some sparing of anterior occipital cortex, which may account for her ability to see moving targets beyond ~15 degrees eccentricity during perimetry. Most strikingly, functional and structural MRI revealed robust and reliable spared functionality of the middle temporal motion complex (MT+) bilaterally. Moreover, consistent with her preserved ability to discriminate motion direction in psychophysical testing, MC also shows direction-selective adaptation in MT+. A variety of tests did not enable us to discern whether input to MT+ was driven by her spared anterior occipital cortex or subcortical inputs. Nevertheless, MC shows rich motion perception despite profoundly impaired static and form vision, combined with clear preservation of activation in MT+, thus supporting the role of MT+ in the Riddoch phenomenon.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/diagnostic imaging , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Motion Perception , Visual Cortex/pathology , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Infarction/pathology , Cerebral Infarction/psychology , Contrast Sensitivity , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Middle Aged , Neuroimaging , Psychophysics , Visual Perception
5.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 204-208, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30102905

ABSTRACT

Blindsight has been primarily and extensively studied by Lawrence Weiskrantz. Residual visual abilities following a hemispheric lesion leading to homonymous hemianopia encompass a variety of visual-perceptual and visuo-motor functions. Attention blindsight produces the more salient subjective experiences, especially for motion (Riddoch phenomenon). Action blindsight illustrates visuo-motor abilities despite the patients' feeling that they produce random movements. Perception blindsight seems to be the weakest residual function observed in blindsight, e.g. for wavelength sensitivity. Discriminating motion produced by isoluminant colours does not give rise to blindsight for motion but the outcome of the reciprocal test is not known. Here we tested whether moving stimuli could give rise to colour discrimination in a patient with homonymous hemianopia. It was found that even though the patient exhibited nearly perfect performances for motion direction discrimination his colour discrimination for the same moving stimulus remained at chance level. It is concluded that easily discriminated moving stimuli do not give rise to colour discrimination and implications for the 3 levels of blindsight taxonomy are discussed.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Color Perception , Hemianopsia/psychology , Motion Perception , Adult , Aged , Attention , Discrimination, Psychological , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance , Stroke/complications , Stroke/psychology , Visual Perception , Young Adult
6.
Arch Soc Esp Oftalmol (Engl Ed) ; 93(11): 555-557, 2018 Nov.
Article in English, Spanish | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29859732

ABSTRACT

CLINICAL CASE: A 22 year-old woman complained about blurred vision after an episode of recovered cardiorespiratory arrest. She had bilateral low visual acuity («count fingers¼) and no ophthalmological or visual pathways changes. She also had an apparent lack of awareness of the deficit. The Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) showed ischaemic changes in both occipital lobes. As a result, she was diagnosed with Anton-Babinski syndrome. DISCUSSION: This is a rare disease that should be suspected in strange or poorly congruent visual loss. It is usually due to an ischaemic injury in this region of brain, manifesting itself with low vision not perceived by the patient (visual confabulation). It can simulate a non-organic visual loss or psychiatric disease.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/diagnosis , Brain Ischemia/diagnosis , Visual Cortex/blood supply , Agnosia/etiology , Blindness, Cortical/diagnostic imaging , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Brain Ischemia/diagnostic imaging , Brain Ischemia/psychology , Female , Heart Arrest/complications , Humans , Hypesthesia/etiology , Hypoxia-Ischemia, Brain/etiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Paresis/etiology , Tomography, Optical Coherence , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Young Adult
7.
J Neurol Phys Ther ; 41(3): 164-172, 2017 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28628550

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Spontaneous visual recovery is rare after cortical blindness. While visual rehabilitation may improve performance, no visual therapy has been widely adopted, as clinical outcomes are variable and rarely translate into improvements in activities of daily living (ADLs). We explored the potential value of a novel rehabilitation approach "cognitive therapeutic exercises" for cortical blindness. CASE DESCRIPTION: The subject of this case study was 48-year-old woman with cortical blindness and tetraplegia after cardiac arrest. Prior to the intervention, she was dependent in ADLs and poorly distinguished shapes and colors after 19 months of standard visual and motor rehabilitation. Computed tomographic images soon after symptom onset demonstrated acute infarcts in both occipital cortices. INTERVENTION: The subject underwent 8 months of intensive rehabilitation with "cognitive therapeutic exercises" consisting of discrimination exercises correlating sensory and visual information. OUTCOMES: Visual fields increased; object recognition improved; it became possible to watch television; voluntary arm movements improved in accuracy and smoothness; walking improved; and ADL independence and self-reliance increased. Subtraction of neuroimaging acquired before and after rehabilitation showed that focal glucose metabolism increases bilaterally in the occipital poles. DISCUSSION: This study demonstrates feasibility of "cognitive therapeutic exercises" in an individual with cortical blindness, who experienced impressive visual and sensorimotor recovery, with marked ADL improvement, more than 2 years after ischemic cortical damage.Video Abstract available for additional insights from the authors (see Video, Supplemental Digital Content 1, available at: http://links.lww.com/JNPT/A173).


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Blindness, Cortical/rehabilitation , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy , Exercise Therapy , Activities of Daily Living , Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Middle Aged , Recovery of Function , Vision, Ocular , Walking
8.
Psychogeriatrics ; 15(1): 58-61, 2015 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25515048

ABSTRACT

Anton-Babinski syndrome is a rare disease featuring bilateral cortical blindness and anosognosia with visual confabulation, but without dementia or any memory impairment. It has a unique neuropsychiatric presentation and should be highly suspected in those with odd visual loss and imaging evidence of occipital lobe injury. In the case discussed herein, a 90-year-old man presented with bilateral blindness, obvious anosognosia, and vivid visual confabulation, which he had had for 3 days. Brain computed tomography demonstrated recent hypodense infarctions at the bilateral occipital lobes. Thus, the patient was diagnosed with Anton-Babinski syndrome. Because of his age and the thrombolytic therapy during the golden 3 hours after ischemic stroke, the patient received aspirin therapy rather than tissue plasminogen activator or warfarin. He gradually realized he was blind during the following week, but died of pneumonia 1 month later. In the literature, it is difficult to establish awareness of blindness in patients with Anton-Babinski syndrome, but optimistically, in one report, a patient was aware of blindness within 2 weeks, without vision improvement. Our case illustrates that elderly patients with Anton-Babinski syndrome can partially recover and that 1 week is the shortest time for the establishment of awareness of blindness for sufferers without vision improvement.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/diagnosis , Aged, 80 and over , Awareness/physiology , Blindness/diagnosis , Blindness/physiopathology , Blindness/psychology , Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Cerebral Infarction/diagnosis , Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Cerebral Infarction/psychology , Diagnosis, Differential , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Humans , Male , Occipital Lobe/pathology , Occipital Lobe/physiopathology , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
9.
Behav Neurol ; 26(1-2): 157-63, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22713392

ABSTRACT

We describe a 69-year-old patient with superior altitudinal hemianopia who contentiously denied having any visual impairment after stroke in the lower banks of both calcarine fissures. Although the patient did not produce intentional responses to visual stimuli in the blind fields, he showed reduced reaction times to stimuli presented in the inferior visual fields when they were primed by identical stimuli in the superior blind fields. Furthermore he showed left extinction to the double stimulation and delayed reaction times for left unprimed stimuli in the inferior fields. Based on these findings we discuss the possibility that blindsight and right hemisphere damage might be both necessary conditions for denying bilateral blindness.


Subject(s)
Attention , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Delusions/psychology , Visual Perception , Aged , Blindness, Cortical/diagnosis , Delusions/complications , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time , Visual Fields
10.
Brain ; 135(Pt 11): 3440-52, 2012 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23169923

ABSTRACT

Brain responses (from functional magnetic resonance imaging) and components of information processing were investigated in nine cortically blind observers performing a global direction discrimination task. Three of these subjects had responses in perilesional cortex in response to blind field stimulation, whereas the others did not. We used the EZ-diffusion model of decision making to understand how cortically blind subjects make a perceptual decision on stimuli presented within their blind field. We found that these subjects had slower accumulation of information in their blind fields as compared with their good fields and to intact controls. Within cortically blind subjects, activity in perilesional tissue, V3A and hMT+ was associated with a faster accumulation of information for deciding direction of motion of stimuli presented in the blind field. This result suggests that the rate of information accumulation is a critical factor in the degree of impairment in cortical blindness and varies greatly among affected individuals. Retraining paradigms that seek to restore visual functions might benefit from focusing on increasing the rate of information accumulation.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Aged , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Case-Control Studies , Decision Making/physiology , Female , Functional Neuroimaging/methods , Functional Neuroimaging/psychology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/psychology , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Psychological , Stroke/complications , Stroke/physiopathology , Stroke/psychology , Time Factors
11.
J Neurol ; 259(6): 1062-70, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22064977

ABSTRACT

The role of the primary visual cortex in visual mental imagery has provided significant debate in the imagery literature. Functional neuroimaging studies show considerable variation depending on task and technique. Patient studies can be difficult to interpret due to the diverse nature of cortical damage. The type of cortical damage in patient SBR is exceedingly rare as it is restricted to the gray matter of the calcarine sulcus. In this study, we show that in spite of his near-complete cortical blindness, SBR exhibits vivid visual mental imagery both behaviorally and when measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging. The pattern of cortical activation to visual mental imagery in SBR is indistinguishable from individual sighted subjects, in contrast to the visual perceptual responses, which are greatly attenuated.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Imagery, Psychotherapy/methods , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Humans , Imagination/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Young Adult
12.
Cortex ; 47(10): 1219-27, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21194684

ABSTRACT

A 51-year-old woman presented with anosognosia for hemiplegia (AHP), neglect, and a complete loss of vision, for which she was almost immediately aware. Neuroimaging studies revealed intracranial hemorrhages in the medial temporal lobes bilaterally, extending back to the occipital cortex, but sparing the calcarine cortex. A large right frontal-parietal hemorrhage which extended to the posterior body of the corpus callosum was also observed. The patient's vision slowly improved, and by 11 months post onset, formal visual fields revealed improvement primarily in the left upper quadrants only. In contrast, resolution of her AHP occurred between the 26th and 31st day post onset. Awareness of motor impairment was correlated with her ability to initiate finger tapping in her left hemiplegic/paretic hand. During the time she was unaware of her motor deficits but aware of her visual impairments, her dreams did not reflect concerns over visual or motor limitations. The findings support a "modular" theory of anosognosia.


Subject(s)
Agnosia/complications , Blindness, Cortical/complications , Hemiplegia/complications , Intracranial Hemorrhages/pathology , Perceptual Disorders/complications , Agnosia/pathology , Agnosia/psychology , Awareness , Blindness, Cortical/pathology , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Dreams/psychology , Female , Hemiplegia/pathology , Hemiplegia/psychology , Humans , Intracranial Hemorrhages/complications , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Middle Aged , Perceptual Disorders/pathology , Perceptual Disorders/psychology , Temporal Lobe/pathology , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
13.
NeuroRehabilitation ; 27(4): 321-5, 2010.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21160121

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Cortical blindness is a rare complication of posterior circulation stroke. However, its complex presentation with sensory, physical, cognitive and behavioural impairments makes it one of the most challenging. Appropriate approach from a rehabilitation standpoint was never reported. AIM: Our study aims to discuss the rehabilitation methods and outcomes of a cohort of patients with cortical blindness. METHOD: The notes of all patients with cortical blindness referred to a local NHS rehabilitation service in the last 6~years were examined. Patients' demographics, presenting symptoms, scan findings, rehabilitation programmes and outcomes were documented. RESULTS: Seven patients presented to our service, six of them were males. The mean age was 63. Patients 1, 2 and 3 had total blindness with severe cognitive and behavioural impairments, wandering and akathisia. All of them failed to respond to any rehabilitation effort and the focus was on damage limitation. Pharmacological interventions had a modest impact on behaviour and sleep pattern. The 3 patients were discharged to a nursing facility. Patients 4, 5, 6 and 7 had partial blindness with variable severity. All of them suffered from significant memory impairment. However, none suffered from any behavioural, physical or other cognitive impairment. Rehabilitation efforts on 3 patients were carried out collaboratively between brain injury occupational therapists and sensory disability officers. All patients experienced significant improvement in handicap and they all maintained community placements. CONCLUSION: This small cohort of patients suggests that the rehabilitation philosophy and outcomes of these 2 distinct groups of either total or partial cortical blindness differ significantly.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/etiology , Blindness, Cortical/rehabilitation , Stroke/complications , Adult , Aged , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Cohort Studies , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Patient Discharge , Stroke/psychology , Stroke Rehabilitation , Treatment Outcome
14.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 106(42): 17661-6, 2009 Oct 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19805044

ABSTRACT

The spontaneous tendency to synchronize our facial expressions with those of others is often termed emotional contagion. It is unclear, however, whether emotional contagion depends on visual awareness of the eliciting stimulus and which processes underlie the unfolding of expressive reactions in the observer. It has been suggested either that emotional contagion is driven by motor imitation (i.e., mimicry), or that it is one observable aspect of the emotional state arising when we see the corresponding emotion in others. Emotional contagion reactions to different classes of consciously seen and "unseen" stimuli were compared by presenting pictures of facial or bodily expressions either to the intact or blind visual field of two patients with unilateral destruction of the visual cortex and ensuing phenomenal blindness. Facial reactions were recorded using electromyography, and arousal responses were measured with pupil dilatation. Passive exposure to unseen expressions evoked faster facial reactions and higher arousal compared with seen stimuli, therefore indicating that emotional contagion occurs also when the triggering stimulus cannot be consciously perceived because of cortical blindness. Furthermore, stimuli that are very different in their visual characteristics, such as facial and bodily gestures, induced highly similar expressive responses. This shows that the patients did not simply imitate the motor pattern observed in the stimuli, but resonated to their affective meaning. Emotional contagion thus represents an instance of truly affective reactions that may be mediated by visual pathways of old evolutionary origin bypassing cortical vision while still providing a cornerstone for emotion communication and affect sharing.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Emotions/physiology , Facial Expression , Kinesics , Nonverbal Communication/physiology , Nonverbal Communication/psychology , Aged , Electromyography , Facial Muscles/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Visual Cortex/injuries , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Fields , Visual Pathways/physiopathology
16.
Brain Nerve ; 59(1): 37-44, 2007 Jan.
Article in Japanese | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17354377

ABSTRACT

Several neuropsychological studies have reported dissociation between motion vision and object vision. One patient with motion blindness had a bilateral MT/V5 lesion and could see objects, but could not see the motion of the objects (Zihl et al, 1983). By contrast, some blindsight patients with primary visual cortex lesions cannot see objects but can see their movement (e.g. Riddoch, 1917). These results imply that movement vision and form vision rely on independent mechanisms. However one patient with motion blindness had controversial symptoms concerning motion vision. She could not perceive the movement of objects, although she could walk without colliding with obstacles and could catch incoming objects. It has also been reported that patients with a bilateral parietal lesion had well-preserved primary motion vision, but had problems walking and catching a ball (Vaina, 1998). Therefore, motion for vision and motion for action might have independent mechanisms. Such dissociation has also been noted in patients with Bálint syndrome. Some patients behave like a blind person but can walk and catch a ball, while other patients bump into obstacles while walking. These results indicate that the neural bases of motion vision are distinct from those of form vision and that there are subdivisions of motion vision.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Motion Perception/physiology , Motion , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Perception/physiology , Agnosia/physiopathology , Agnosia/psychology , Ataxia/physiopathology , Ataxia/psychology , Craniocerebral Trauma/physiopathology , Craniocerebral Trauma/psychology , Humans , Ocular Motility Disorders/physiopathology , Ocular Motility Disorders/psychology , Syndrome
17.
Nat Neurosci ; 10(2): 257-61, 2007 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17237774

ABSTRACT

The lack of an accepted measure of awareness has made claims that accurate decisions can be made without awareness controversial. Here we introduce a new objective measure of awareness, post-decision wagering. We show that participants fail to maximize cash earnings by wagering high following correct decisions in blindsight, the Iowa gambling task and an artificial grammar task. This demonstrates, without the uncertainties associated with the conventional subjective measures of awareness (verbal reports and confidence ratings), that the participants were not aware that their decisions were correct. Post-decision wagering may be used to study the neural correlates of consciousness.


Subject(s)
Awareness/physiology , Consciousness/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Gambling/psychology , Unconscious, Psychology , Adult , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Brain/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Language Tests , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Verbal Behavior/physiology
18.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 29(7): 1035-46, 2005.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16143169

ABSTRACT

Blindsight broadly refers to the paradoxical neurological condition where patients with a visual field defect due to a cortical lesion nevertheless demonstrate implicit residual visual sensitivity within their field cut. The aim of this paper is twofold. First, through a selective review of the blindsight literature we propose a new taxonomy for the subtypes of residual abilities described in blindsight. Those patients able to accurately act upon blind field stimuli (e.g. by pointing or saccading towards them) are classified as having 'action-blindsight', those whose residual functions can be said to rely to some extent upon attentive processing of blind field stimuli are classified as demonstrating 'attention-blindsight', while finally, patients who have somewhat accurate perceptual judgements for blind field stimuli despite a complete lack of any conscious percept, are classified as having 'agnosopsia'--literally meaning 'not knowing what one sees'. We also address the possible neurological substrates of these residual sensory processes. Our second aim was to investigate the most striking subtype of blindsight, action-blindsight. We review the data relevant to this subtype and the hypotheses proposed to account for it, before speculating on how action-blindsight may inform our normal models of visuomotor control.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Awareness , Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Visual Perception/physiology , Blindness, Cortical/classification , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology
19.
Nat Neurosci ; 7(4): 339-40, 2004 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15034586

ABSTRACT

To investigate the neural substrates underlying emotional feelings in the absence of a conscious stimulus percept, we presented a visual stimulus in the blind field of partially cortically blind patients and measured cortical activity (by functional magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI) before and after the stimulus had been paired with an aversive event. After pairing, self-reported negative emotional valence and blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) responses in somatosensory association areas were enhanced, whereby somatosensory activity predicted highly corresponding reported feelings and startle reflex amplitudes across subjects. Our data provide direct evidence that cortical activity representing physical emotional states governs emotional feelings.


Subject(s)
Affective Symptoms/physiopathology , Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Brain Mapping , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Somatosensory Cortex/physiology , Affective Symptoms/psychology , Association , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Neural Pathways/physiology , Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Vision, Ocular/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...