Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 5.777
Filter
2.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 12268, 2024 May 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38806588

ABSTRACT

In this study, we propose an approach to detect deception during investigative interviews by integrating response latency and error analysis with the unexpected question technique. Sixty participants were assigned to an honest (n = 30) or deceptive group (n = 30). The deceptive group was instructed to memorize the false biographical details of a fictitious identity. Throughout the interviews, participants were presented with a randomized sequence of control, expected, and unexpected open-ended questions about identity. Responses were audio recorded for detailed examination. Our findings indicate that deceptive participants showed markedly longer latencies and higher error rates when answering expected (requiring deception) and unexpected questions (for which premeditated deception was not possible). Longer response latencies were also observed in participants attempting deception when answering control questions (which necessitated truthful answers). Moreover, a within-subject analysis highlighted that responding to unexpected questions significantly impaired individuals' performance compared to answering control and expected questions. Leveraging machine-learning algorithms, our approach attained a classification accuracy of 98% in distinguishing deceptive and honest participants. Additionally, a classification analysis on single response levels was conducted. Our findings underscore the effectiveness of merging response latency metrics and error rates with unexpected questioning as a robust method for identity deception detection in investigative interviews. We also discuss significant implications for enhancing interview strategies.


Subject(s)
Deception , Lie Detection , Reaction Time , Humans , Male , Female , Adult , Lie Detection/psychology , Reaction Time/physiology , Interviews as Topic , Young Adult , Machine Learning
3.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 9(1): 28, 2024 05 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38713308

ABSTRACT

Fake news can have enduring effects on memory and beliefs. An ongoing theoretical debate has investigated whether corrections (fact-checks) should include reminders of fake news. The familiarity backfire account proposes that reminders hinder correction (increasing interference), whereas integration-based accounts argue that reminders facilitate correction (promoting memory integration). In three experiments, we examined how different types of corrections influenced memory for and belief in news headlines. In the exposure phase, participants viewed real and fake news headlines. In the correction phase, participants viewed reminders of fake news that either reiterated the false details (complete) or prompted recall of missing false details (partial); reminders were followed by fact-checked headlines correcting the false details. Both reminder types led to proactive interference in memory for corrected details, but complete reminders produced less interference than partial reminders (Experiment 1). However, when participants had fewer initial exposures to fake news and experienced a delay between exposure and correction, this effect was reversed; partial reminders led to proactive facilitation, enhancing correction (Experiment 2). This effect occurred regardless of the delay before correction (Experiment 3), suggesting that the effects of partial reminders depend on the number of prior fake news exposures. In all experiments, memory and perceived accuracy were better when fake news and corrections were recollected, implicating a critical role for integrative encoding. Overall, we show that when memories of fake news are weak or less accessible, partial reminders are more effective for correction; when memories of fake news are stronger or more accessible, complete reminders are preferable.


Subject(s)
Deception , Mental Recall , Humans , Adult , Young Adult , Female , Male , Mental Recall/physiology
5.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 11763, 2024 05 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38782940

ABSTRACT

The present work is the first to comprehensively analyze the gravity of the misinformation problem in Hungary, where misinformation appears regularly in the pro-governmental, populist, and socially conservative mainstream media. In line with international data, using a Hungarian representative sample (Study 1, N = 991), we found that voters of the reigning populist, conservative party could hardly distinguish fake from real news. In Study 2, we demonstrated that a prosocial intervention of ~ 10 min (N = 801) helped young adult participants discern misinformation four weeks later compared to the control group without implementing any boosters. This effect was the most salient regarding pro-governmental conservative fake news content, leaving real news evaluations intact. Although the hypotheses of the present work were not preregistered, it appears that prosocial misinformation interventions might be promising attempts to counter misinformation in an informational autocracy in which the media is highly centralized. Despite using social motivations, it does not mean that long-term cognitive changes cannot occur. Future studies might explore exactly how these interventions can have an impact on the long-term cognitive processing of news content as well as their underlying neural structures.


Subject(s)
Communication , Humans , Male , Female , Adult , Young Adult , Hungary , Mass Media , Empowerment , Deception , Family/psychology , Adolescent
6.
Sci Data ; 11(1): 389, 2024 Apr 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38627400

ABSTRACT

Studying deception is vital for understanding decision-making and social dynamics. Recent EEG research has deepened insights into the brain mechanisms behind deception. Standard methods in this field often rely on memory, are vulnerable to countermeasures, yield false positives, and lack real-world relevance. Here, we present a comprehensive dataset from an EEG-monitored competitive, two-player card game designed to elicit authentic deception behavior. Our extensive dataset contains EEG data from 12 pairs (N = 24 participants with role switching), controlled for age, gender, and risk-taking, with detailed labels and annotations. The dataset combines standard event-related potential and microstate analyses with state-of-the-art decoding approaches of four scenarios: spontaneous/instructed truth-telling and lying. This demonstrates game-based methods' efficacy in studying deception and sets a benchmark for future research. Overall, our dataset represents a unique resource with applications in cognitive neuroscience and related fields for studying deception, competitive behavior, decision-making, inter-brain synchrony, and benchmarking of decoding frameworks in a difficult, high-level cognitive task.


Subject(s)
Competitive Behavior , Deception , Electroencephalography , Humans , Brain , Evoked Potentials
7.
JAMA ; 331(19): 1612-1613, 2024 05 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38669040

ABSTRACT

This Medical News article discusses a KFF poll about the public's exposure to and beliefs about inaccurate health information, as well as media use and trust in sources.


Subject(s)
Physicians , Humans , United States , Fraud/legislation & jurisprudence , Deception
8.
Fam Syst Health ; 42(1): 139, 2024 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38647500

ABSTRACT

This poem describes the ugly face of misinformation and lies-spewing bile as COVID shook us and humanity came undone. Vaccines were rejected and lockdowns were broken. We barely withstood. Who won? No one. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , SARS-CoV-2 , Humans , COVID-19/prevention & control , COVID-19/psychology , COVID-19/epidemiology , Deception , Communication , Pandemics
9.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 28(5): 383-385, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38575465

ABSTRACT

This article introduces a theoretical model of truth and honesty from a psychological perspective. We examine its application in political discourse and discuss empirical findings distinguishing between conceptions of honesty and their influence on public perception, misinformation dissemination, and the integrity of democracy.


Subject(s)
Deception , Humans , Politics , Democracy , Models, Psychological
10.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 24(3): 440-452, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38504050

ABSTRACT

Exposure to misleading information after witnessing an event can impair future memory reports about the event. This pervasive form of memory distortion, termed the misinformation effect, can be significantly reduced if individuals are warned about the reliability of post-event information before exposure to misleading information. The present fMRI study investigated whether such prewarnings improve subsequent memory accuracy by influencing encoding-related neural activity during exposure to misinformation. We employed a repeated retrieval misinformation paradigm in which participants watched a crime video (Witnessed Event), completed an initial test of memory, listened to a post-event auditory narrative that contained consistent, neutral, and misleading details (Post-Event Information), and then completed a final test of memory. At the behavioral level, participants who were given a prewarning before the Post-Event Information were less susceptible to misinformation on the final memory test compared with participants who were not given a warning (Karanian et al., Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117, 22771-22779, 2020). This protection from misinformation was accompanied by greater activity in frontal regions associated with source encoding (lateral PFC) and conflict detection (ACC) during misleading trials as well as a more global reduction in activity in auditory cortex and semantic processing regions (left inferior frontal gyrus) across all trials (consistent, neutral, misleading) of the Post-Event Information narrative. Importantly, the strength of these warning-related activity modulations was associated with better protection from misinformation on the final memory test (improved memory accuracy on misleading trials). Together, these results suggest that warnings modulate encoding-related neural activity during exposure to misinformation to improve memory accuracy.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Mental Recall , Humans , Female , Male , Young Adult , Adult , Mental Recall/physiology , Communication , Deception , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Adolescent , Brain Mapping , Memory/physiology
11.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 242: 105893, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38479320

ABSTRACT

A total of 76 children (Mage = 9 years 5 months, SD = 2.22 years) participated in a structured interview about their experiences with and knowledge of academic dishonesty. Overall, 27% of the sample reported having cheated in school. Most of these children were 10 to 13 years old, and the most prevalent form of cheating behavior reported was using forbidden materials during a test. Children's age group was a significant positive predictor of their reported cheating history; however, no significant difference was found between children's gender and engagement with cheating. Children's moral evaluations of cheating did not predict their reported cheating history, nor did children's parents' cheating history. Vignette type (cheating vs. non-cheating), age group, and the interaction between vignette type and age group were significant predictors of children's ability to accurately identify behaviors that constitute cheating. Children rated cheating behaviors as significantly less moral than non-cheating behaviors. Overall, the current results provide insight into what forms of cheating behavior children engage in at the elementary school-age level.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior , Deception , Child , Humans , Adolescent , Morals , Parents
12.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 242: 105894, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38493524

ABSTRACT

This research examines barriers to reporting academic dishonesty in early adulthood (Study 1; N = 92) and adolescence (Study 2; N = 137). Participants were asked to describe a recent time they observed a peer cheating and to reflect on their decision about whether to report the cheating. They also responded to hypothetical scenarios about observing typical cheating actions, and the presence of social motives (e.g., whether people who report tend to gain reputations for being snitches) was manipulated in each scenario. Even though participants judged reporting to be the morally right thing to do, doing so was rare and approval for it was low, especially in adolescence. Participants also tended to say they would rather be friends with people who do not report cheaters than with those who do. Participants reasoned about a variety of social concerns to support their judgments about reporting (e.g., concern about their relationship with the cheater, concerns for others' welfare), and the manipulated social motives in the hypothetical scenarios significantly influenced judgments about reporting. These findings inform our understanding of the social dynamics that contribute to decisions about policing academic honesty.


Subject(s)
Deception , Taboo , Adolescent , Humans , Animals , Rats , Adult , Students , Judgment , Motivation
13.
J Allied Health ; 53(1): 25-31, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38430493

ABSTRACT

For many decades, academic cheating has been prevalent across many institutions and majors. This problem has been exacerbated by new technology that has increased opportunities for students to access and use information dishonestly. There is fear amongst faculty that dishonesty in the academic world could negatively impact professionals in their future careers. The greater prevalence of cheating may be related to students misunderstanding what constitutes cheating. To better understand students' perceptions of cheating, a group of faculty surveyed students across 11 academic programs at a college of health professions using a slightly modified version of a validated tool via an online platform. Data about cheating perceptions were collected from more than 400 students. Although most respondents agreed that cheating is wrong, some were open to explanations of innocence related to possible cheating scenarios. Also, most respondents did not agree that a person who cheats is an unethical person. These findings suggest that students believe cheating can occur unintentionally. Although the findings support that more education about academic dishonesty would be valuable, this study can inform efforts to develop more targeted education and interventions to reduce cheating behaviors.


Subject(s)
Deception , Students , Humans , Faculty , Surveys and Questionnaires , Health Occupations
14.
Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw ; 27(4): 240-252, 2024 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38484319

ABSTRACT

Fake news impacts individuals' behavior and decision-making while also disrupting political processes, perceptions of medical advice, and societal trends. Improving individuals' ability to accurately assess fake news can reduce its harmful effects. However, previous research on media literacy interventions designed for improving fake news credibility assessments has yielded inconsistent results. We systematically collected 33 independent studies and performed a meta-analysis to examine the effects of media literacy interventions on assessing fake news credibility (n = 36,256). The results showed that media literacy interventions significantly improved fake news credibility assessments (Hedges' g = 0.53, 95% confidence interval [0.29-0.78], p < 0.001). Gaming interventions were the most effective intervention form. Conversely, the intervention channel, outcome measurement, and subject characteristics (age, gender, and country development level) did not influence the intervention effects.


Subject(s)
Deception , Mass Media , Humans , Trust
15.
Subst Use Misuse ; 59(8): 1261-1270, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38503716

ABSTRACT

Background: The COVID-19 pandemic has accelerated and amplified the use of virtual research methods. While online research has several advantages, it also provides greater opportunity for individuals to misrepresent their identities to fraudulently participate in research for financial gain. Participant deception and fraud have become a growing concern for virtual research. Reports of deception and preventative strategies have been discussed within online quantitative research, particularly survey studies. Though, there is a dearth of literature surrounding these issues pertaining to qualitative studies, particularly within substance use research. Results: In this commentary, we detail an unforeseen case study of several individuals who appeared to deliberately misrepresent their identities and information during participation in a virtual synchronous qualitative substance use study. Through our experiences, we offer strategies to detect and prevent participant deception and fraud, as well as challenges to consider when implementing these approaches. Conclusions: Without general awareness and protective measures, the integrity of virtual research methods remains vulnerable to inaccuracy. As online research continues to expand, it is essential to proactively design innovative solutions to safeguard future studies against increasingly sophisticated deception and fraud.


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , Deception , Fraud , Qualitative Research , Substance-Related Disorders , Humans , Fraud/prevention & control , COVID-19/prevention & control , Substance-Related Disorders/prevention & control
16.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 242: 105888, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38430869

ABSTRACT

The current study investigated the association of children's age, gender, ethnicity, Big Five personality traits, and self-efficacy with their academic cheating behaviors. Academic cheating is a rampant problem that has been documented in adolescents and adults for nearly a century, but our understanding of the early development and factors influencing academic cheating is still weak. Using Zoom, the current study recruited children aged 4 to 12 years (N = 388), measured their cheating behaviors through six tasks simulating academic testing scenarios, and assessed their Big Five personality traits and self-efficacy through a modified Berkeley Puppet Interview paradigm, as well as age and gender. We found that children cheated significantly less with increased age and that boys cheated significantly more than girls. However, neither Big Five personality traits nor self-efficacy were significantly correlated with children's cheating. These findings suggest that academic cheating is a developing issue from early to middle childhood and that factors such as gender socialization may play a role in such development. Personal characteristics such as personality traits and self-efficacy may undergo additional development before their associations with cheating become robust, as reported in the adult literature.


Subject(s)
Personality , Self Efficacy , Male , Adult , Female , Adolescent , Humans , Child, Preschool , Child , Deception
17.
Brain Cogn ; 175: 106140, 2024 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38359486

ABSTRACT

Collaborative crime poses severe social hazards. In collaborative crime scenarios, previous studies have indicated that perpetrators' collaborative encoding can impair the detection efficiency of P300-based complex trial protocols due to the collaborative encoding deficit. The feedback concealed information test (fCIT), a unique variation of the concealed information test, provides participants with feedback on how well they conceal information from memory. The fCIT, which has proven to be highly efficient, detects concealed information using recognition P300 along with feedback-related event-related potentials, and reflects the subject's motivation to conceal. However, no studies have examined the fCIT's effectiveness in identifying collaborative criminals. We propose that the fCIT's efficiency persists in cases of collaborative crime and test this hypothesis using a sample of 48 participants. The participants in the collaborative groups were instructed to have hushed conversations about theft to simulate the collaborative crime process. Subsequently, they completed the fCIT. The findings indicate a significant decline in recognition P300's detection efficiency when participants committed crimes collaboratively. Nevertheless, the detection efficiency of feedback P300 and feedback-related negativity remained high. This study's outcomes illustrate the capacity of the fCIT to detect perpetrators involved in collaborative crime.


Subject(s)
Deception , Lie Detection , Humans , Feedback , Event-Related Potentials, P300 , Crime
18.
PLoS One ; 19(2): e0298889, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38421983

ABSTRACT

The interconnected power system connects the power grids of different regions through transmission lines, achieving power interconnection and resource sharing. However, data is transmitted through open power networks and is more susceptible to network attacks. To improve the stability of interconnected power systems under deception attacks, three scenarios of system security load frequency control were studied. Based on the construction of a dynamic model of load frequency control, an event-triggered strategy was used to reduce the communication frequency between nodes, resulting in a reduction in the amount of network transmission data. A sliding mode controller was constructed to solve the problem of event-triggered sliding mode security load frequency control. Elastic event-triggered sliding mode load frequency control for interconnected power systems under mixed attacks. The simulation results showed that using the load frequency control model triggered by events, the load frequency deviation of the interconnected power system can be stabilized at around 12 seconds, effectively saving the cost of network resources. Under the regulation of the load frequency control model based on sliding mode control, the interconnected power system stabilized in 10 seconds, reducing the load of network transmission. The elastic event-triggered sliding mode load frequency control model can ensure stable transmission of power data under various attacks and has good anti-interference performance. The results of this study have played an important role in achieving the stability of power resource supply. Compared with previous studies on individual power systems, this study solves the attack problem of interconnected power systems and considers the frequency control problem of system security loads under mixed attacks, enabling the system to recover stability faster.


Subject(s)
Communication , Computer Systems , Computer Simulation , Electric Power Supplies , Deception
19.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 241: 105863, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38306738

ABSTRACT

Children are often third-party observers of conversations between informants and receivers. Although 5- and 6-year-olds can identify and reject informants' false testimony, it remains unclear whether they expect others to do the same. Accurately assessing others' impressions of informants and their testimony in a conversational setting is essential for children's navigation of the social world. Using a novel second-order lie detection task, the current study examined whether 4- to 7-year-olds (N = 74; Mage = 69 months) take receivers' epistemic states into account when predicting whether a receiver would think an informant is truthful or deceptive. We pitted children's firsthand observations of reality against informants' false testimony while manipulating receivers' perceptual access to a sticker-hiding event. Results showed that when the receiver had perceptual access and was knowledgeable, children predicted that the receiver would think the informant is lying. Critically, when the receiver lacked perceptual access and was ignorant, children were significantly more likely to predict that the receiver would think the informant is telling the truth. Second-order theory of mind and executive function strengthened this effect. Findings are interpreted using a dual-process framework and provide new insights into children's understanding of others' selective trust and susceptibility to deception.


Subject(s)
Cues , Judgment , Child , Humans , Child, Preschool , Trust , Executive Function , Deception
20.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 241: 105869, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38350253

ABSTRACT

Children and young adolescents often tend to behave dishonestly in order to serve their self-interests. This study focused on how empathic abilities affect children's tendency to deceive others. Deception is the act of causing others to form a false belief to get them to act in a way that serves the deceiver's interests. As such, it requires the ability to predict how others might use the provided information. In two experiments, 274 participants (aged 10-16 years) played a game in which they could send a deceptive message to another participant to boost their own payoff at the other player's expense. We measured participants' cognitive and emotional empathy using different measures. We found that a measure of cognitive empathy, namely the fantasy scale, was associated with less deception of another player when that other player was not identified and was presented only as "Player B." However, when Player B was identified by name, empathy did not predict deception. In such cases, the only factors affecting deception rates were the gain for the participant (higher possible gains lead to more deception) and loss to the other player (higher possible losses lead to less deception). Overall, the findings suggest that even by 11 years of age, children can understand the impact of their unethical behavior on another child and adjust their actions accordingly. However, when the other child is not identified, children need to possess high levels of cognitive empathy toward imagined individuals to resist the temptation to deceive the other child.


Subject(s)
Deception , Empathy , Child , Humans , Adolescent , Emotions , Motivation , Cognition
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...