Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 6.472
Filter
1.
J Vis ; 24(6): 5, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38842835

ABSTRACT

Ensemble processing allows the visual system to condense visual information into useful summary statistics (e.g., average size), thereby overcoming capacity limitations to visual working memory and attention. To examine the role of attention in ensemble processing, we conducted three experiments using a novel paradigm that merged the action effect (a manipulation of attention) and ensemble processing. Participants were instructed to make a simple action if the feature of a cue word corresponded to a subsequent shape. Immediately after, they were shown an ensemble display of eight ovals of varying sizes and were asked to report either the average size of all ovals or the size of a single oval from the set. In Experiments 1 and 2, participants were cued with a task-relevant feature, and in Experiment 3, participants were cued with a task-irrelevant feature. Overall, the task-relevant cues that elicited an action influenced reports of average size in the ensemble phase more than the cues that were passively viewed, whereas task-irrelevant cues did not bias the reports of average size. The results of this study suggest that attention influences ensemble processing only when it is directed toward a task-relevant feature.


Subject(s)
Attention , Cues , Photic Stimulation , Humans , Attention/physiology , Young Adult , Male , Female , Photic Stimulation/methods , Adult , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Size Perception/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
2.
J Vis ; 24(6): 6, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38843389

ABSTRACT

Infant primates see poorly, and most perceptual functions mature steadily beyond early infancy. Behavioral studies on human and macaque infants show that global form perception, as measured by the ability to integrate contour information into a coherent percept, improves dramatically throughout the first several years after birth. However, it is unknown when sensitivity to curvature and shape emerges in early life or how it develops. We studied the development of shape sensitivity in 18 macaques, aged 2 months to 10 years. Using radial frequency stimuli, circular targets whose radii are modulated sinusoidally, we tested monkeys' ability to radial frequency stimuli from circles as a function of the depth and frequency of sinusoidal modulation. We implemented a new four-choice oddity task and compared the resulting data with that from a traditional two-alternative forced choice task. We found that radial frequency pattern perception was measurable at the youngest age tested (2 months). Behavioral performance at all radial frequencies improved with age. Performance was better for higher radial frequencies, suggesting the developing visual system prioritizes processing of fine visual details that are ecologically relevant. By using two complementary methods, we were able to capture a comprehensive developmental trajectory for shape perception.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Macaca mulatta , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Photic Stimulation , Animals , Form Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Male , Female
3.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(6)2024 Jun 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38832533

ABSTRACT

The two visual pathways model posits that visual information is processed through two distinct cortical systems: The ventral pathway promotes visual recognition, while the dorsal pathway supports visuomotor control. Recent evidence suggests the dorsal pathway is also involved in shape processing and may contribute to object perception, but it remains unclear whether this sensitivity is independent of attentional mechanisms that were localized to overlapping cortical regions. To address this question, we conducted two fMRI experiments that utilized different parametric scrambling manipulations in which human participants viewed novel objects in different levels of scrambling and were instructed to attend to either the object or to another aspect of the image (e.g. color of the background). Univariate and multivariate analyses revealed that the large-scale organization of shape selectivity along the dorsal and ventral pathways was preserved regardless of the focus of attention. Attention did modulate shape sensitivity, but these effects were similar across the two pathways. These findings support the idea that shape processing is at least partially dissociable from attentional processes and relies on a distributed set of cortical regions across the visual pathways.


Subject(s)
Attention , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Photic Stimulation , Visual Pathways , Humans , Attention/physiology , Male , Female , Visual Pathways/physiology , Visual Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Adult , Young Adult , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Photic Stimulation/methods , Brain Mapping/methods , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging
4.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(5): 33, 2024 May 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38771569

ABSTRACT

Purpose: This study explored early (contrast discrimination) and intermediate (global form perception) visual processing in primary subtypes of glaucoma: primary open-angle glaucoma (POAG) and primary angle-closure glaucoma (PACG). We aimed to understand early and intermediate visual processing in POAG and PACG, matched for similar visual field defect severity. Methods: Early visual processing was measured using a contrast discrimination task described by Porkorny and Smith (1997), and intermediate processing using a global form perception task using glass pattern coherence thresholds. Thresholds were determined centrally and at a single midperipheral location (12.5°) in a quadrant without visual field defects. Controls were tested in corresponding quadrants to individuals with glaucoma. Results: Sixty participants (20 POAG, 20 PACG, and 20 age-matched controls), aged 50 to 77 years, were included. Visual field defects were matched between POAG and PACG, with mean deviation values of -6.53 ± 4.46 (range: -1.5 to -16.85) dB and -6.2 ± 4.24 (range: -1.37 to -16.42) dB, respectively. Two-Way ANOVA revealed significant differences in thresholds between the glaucoma groups and the control group for both contrast discrimination and global form perception tasks, with higher thresholds in the glaucoma groups. Post hoc analyses showed no significant contrast discrimination difference between POAG and PACG, but POAG had significantly higher thresholds than PACG for form perception. Conclusions: In form perception, POAG showed slightly worse performance than PACG, suggesting that individuals with POAG may experience more severe functional damage than PACG of similar visual field severity.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity , Form Perception , Glaucoma, Angle-Closure , Glaucoma, Open-Angle , Visual Fields , Humans , Glaucoma, Open-Angle/physiopathology , Glaucoma, Angle-Closure/physiopathology , Middle Aged , Aged , Male , Female , Visual Fields/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Intraocular Pressure/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Field Tests
5.
PLoS One ; 19(5): e0303562, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38809944

ABSTRACT

Classical experiments using hierarchical stimuli to investigate the ability of capuchin monkeys to integrate visual information based on global or local clues reported findings suggesting a behavioral preference for local information of the image. Many experiments using mosaics have been conducted with capuchin monkeys to identify some of their perceptual phenotypes. As the identification of an image in a mosaic demands the integration of elements that share some visual features, we evaluated the discrimination of shapes presented in solid and mosaic stimuli in capuchin monkeys. Shape discrimination performance was tested in 2 male adult capuchin monkeys in an experimental chamber with a touchscreen video monitor, in three experiments: (i) evaluation of global and local processing using hierarchical stimuli; (ii) evaluation of target detection using simple discrimination procedures; (iii) evaluation of shape discrimination using simple discrimination and delayed matching-to-sample procedures. We observed that both monkeys had preferences for local processing when tested by hierarchical stimuli. Additionally, detection performance for solid and mosaic targets was highly significant, but for shape discrimination tasks we found significant performance when using solid figures, non-significant performance when using circle and square shapes in mosaic stimuli, and significant performance when using Letter X and Number 8 shapes in mosaic stimuli. Our results are suggestive that the monkeys respond to local contrast and partly to global contrast in mosaic stimuli.


Subject(s)
Sapajus , Animals , Male , Photic Stimulation , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
6.
J Vis ; 24(5): 12, 2024 May 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38787569

ABSTRACT

Materials exhibit an extraordinary range of visual appearances. Characterizing and quantifying appearance is important not only for basic research on perceptual mechanisms but also for computer graphics and a wide range of industrial applications. Although methods exist for capturing and representing the optical properties of materials and how they vary across surfaces (Haindl & Filip, 2013), the representations are typically very high-dimensional, and how these representations relate to subjective perceptual impressions of material appearance remains poorly understood. Here, we used a data-driven approach to characterizing the perceived appearance characteristics of 30 samples of wood veneer using a "visual fingerprint" that describes each sample as a multidimensional feature vector, with each dimension capturing a different aspect of the appearance. Fifty-six crowd-sourced participants viewed triplets of movies depicting different wood samples as the sample rotated. Their task was to report which of the two match samples was subjectively most similar to the test sample. In another online experiment, 45 participants rated 10 wood-related appearance characteristics for each of the samples. The results reveal a consistent embedding of the samples across both experiments and a set of nine perceptual dimensions capturing aspects including the roughness, directionality, and spatial scale of the surface patterns. We also showed that a weighted linear combination of 11 image statistics, inspired by the rating characteristics, predicts perceptual dimensions well.


Subject(s)
Wood , Humans , Female , Adult , Male , Young Adult , Surface Properties , Photic Stimulation/methods , Form Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
7.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(6): 605-625, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38573695

ABSTRACT

Object-based warping is a visual illusion in which dots appear farther apart from each other when superimposed on an object. Previous research found that the illusion's strength varies with the perceived objecthood of the display. We tested whether objecthood alone determines the strength of the visual illusion or if low-level factors separable from objecthood also play a role. In Experiments 1-2, we varied low-level features to assess their impact on the warping illusion. We found that the warping illusion is equally strong for a variety of shapes but varies with the elements by which shape is defined. Shapes composed of continuous edges produced larger warping effects than shapes defined by disconnected elements. In Experiment 3, we varied a display's objecthood while holding low-level features constant. Displays with matched low-level features produced warping effects of the same size even when the perceived unity of the elements in the display varied. In Experiments 4-6, we tested whether displays with low-level features predicted to be important in spatial warping produced the visual illusion even when the display weakly configured into a single object. Results showed that the presence of low-level features like contour solidity and convexity determined warping effect sizes over and above what could be accounted for by the display's perceived objecthood. Our findings challenge the view that the spatial warping illusion is solely object-based. Other factors like the solidity of contours and contours' position relative to reference dots appear to play separate and important roles in determining warping effect sizes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Optical Illusions , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Adult , Young Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Male , Female , Space Perception/physiology
8.
Jpn J Ophthalmol ; 68(3): 183-191, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38598144

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To assess the impact of glaucoma on perceiving three-dimensional (3D) shapes based on monocular depth cues. STUDY DESIGN: Clinical observational study. METHODS: Twenty glaucoma patients, subjected to binocular visual-field sensitivity (binocular-VFS) tests using a Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer, and 20 age-matched healthy volunteers, underwent two tasks: identifying the nearest vertex of a 3D shape using monocular shading (3D-SfS), texture (3D-SfT), or motion (3D-SfM) cues, and distinguishing elementary one-dimensional (1D) features of these cues. The association of the visual-field index (VFI) of binocular-VFS with 3D shape perception in glaucoma patients was also examined. RESULTS: Glaucoma patients demonstrated reduced accuracy in distinguishing 1D luminance brightness and a larger "error-in-depth" between the perceived and actual depths for 3D-SfM and 3D-SfS compared to healthy volunteers. Six glaucoma patients with a 100% VFI for binocular-VFS exhibited a similar error-in-depth to the other fourteen glaucoma patients; they had a larger error-in-depth for 3D-SfM compared to healthy volunteers. No correlation between the error-in-depth values and the VFI values of binocular-VFS was observed. CONCLUSIONS: The 3D shape perception in glaucoma patients varies based on the depth cue's characteristics. Impaired 1D discrimination and larger thresholds for 3D-SfM in glaucoma patients with a 100% VFI for binocular-VFS indicate more pronounced perceptual deficits of lower-level elementary features for 3D-SfS and higher-level visual processing of 3D shapes for 3D-SfM. The effects of the location and degree of binocular visual-field defects on 3D shape perception remain to be elucidated. Our research provides insights into the 3D shape extraction mechanism in glaucoma.


Subject(s)
Cues , Depth Perception , Glaucoma , Vision, Binocular , Vision, Monocular , Visual Fields , Humans , Male , Female , Depth Perception/physiology , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Middle Aged , Aged , Glaucoma/physiopathology , Glaucoma/diagnosis , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Visual Field Tests , Intraocular Pressure/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Adult
9.
J Vis ; 24(4): 23, 2024 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38662346

ABSTRACT

This paper reviews projection models and their perception in realistic pictures, and proposes hypotheses for three-dimensional (3D) shape and space perception in pictures. In these hypotheses, eye fixations, and foveal vision play a central role. Many past theories and experimental studies focus solely on linear perspective. Yet, these theories fail to explain many important perceptual phenomena, including the effectiveness of nonlinear projections. Indeed, few classical paintings strictly obey linear perspective, nor do the best distortion-avoidance techniques for wide-angle computational photography. The hypotheses here employ a two-stage model for 3D human vision. When viewing a picture, the first stage perceives 3D shape for the current gaze. Each fixation has its own perspective projection, but, owing to the nature of foveal and peripheral vision, shape information is obtained primarily for a small region of the picture around the fixation. As a viewer moves their eyes, the second stage continually integrates some of the per-gaze information into an overall interpretation of a picture. The interpretation need not be geometrically stable or consistent over time. It is argued that this framework could explain many disparate pictorial phenomena, including different projection styles throughout art history and computational photography, while being consistent with the constraints of human 3D vision. The paper reviews open questions and suggests new studies to explore these hypotheses.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Depth Perception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Fovea Centralis/physiology
10.
Vision Res ; 220: 108400, 2024 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38603923

ABSTRACT

It is well known that objects become grouped in perceptual organization when they share some visual feature, like a common direction of motion. Less well known is that grouping can change how people perceive a set of objects. For example, when a pair of shapes consistently share a common region of space, their aspect ratios tend to be perceived as more similar (are attracted toward each other). Conversely, when shapes are assigned to different regions in space their aspect ratios repel from each other. Here we examine whether the visual system produce both attractive and repulsive distortions when the state of grouping between a pair of shapes changes on a moment-to-moment basis. Observers viewed a pair of ellipses that differed in terms of how flat or tall they were and reported the aspect ratio of one ellipse from the pair. Each ellipse was defined by a cloud of coherently-moving dots, and the dots within the two ellipses had either the same or different directions of motion, varying from trial-to-trial. We found that the cued ellipse's aspect ratio was reported to be repelled from the aspect ratio of the uncued ellipse when the shapes had different directions of motion compared to when they had the same direction of motion. These results suggest that the visual system can adaptively alter visual experience based on grouping, in particular, repelling the appearance of objects when they do not appear to go together, and it can do so quickly and flexibly.


Subject(s)
Judgment , Motion Perception , Photic Stimulation , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Judgment/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Adult , Cues , Male , Female , Psychophysics , Young Adult , Analysis of Variance , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
11.
Vision Res ; 219: 108394, 2024 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38579407

ABSTRACT

Contour Integration (CI) is the ability to integrate elemental features into objects and is a basic visual process essential for object perception and recognition, and for functioning in visual environments. It is now well documented that people with schizophrenia (SZ), in addition to having cognitive impairments, also have several visual perceptual deficits, including in CI. Here, we retrospectively characterize the performance of both SZ and neurotypical individuals (NT) on a series of contour shapes, made up of Gabor elements, that varied in terms of closure and curvature. Participants in both groups performed a CI training task that included 7 different families of shapes (Lines, Ellipse, Blobs, Squiggles, Spiral, Circle and Letters) for up to 40 sessions. Two parameters were manipulated in the training task: Orientation Jitter (OJ, i.e., orientation deviations of individual Gabor elements from ideal for each shape) and Inducer Number (IN, i.e., number of Gabor elements defining the shape). Results show that both OJ and IN thresholds significantly differed between the groups, with higher (OJ) and lower (IN) thresholds observed in the controls. Furthermore, we found significant effects as a function of the contour shapes, with differences between groups emerging with contours that were considered more complex, e.g., due to having a higher degree of curvature (Blobs, Spiral, Letters). These data can inform future work that aims to characterize visual integration impairments in schizophrenia.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Schizophrenia , Humans , Form Perception/physiology , Schizophrenia/physiopathology , Adult , Female , Male , Middle Aged , Retrospective Studies , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Case-Control Studies , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Young Adult
12.
Nature ; 627(8005): 821-829, 2024 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38448584

ABSTRACT

Animals in the natural world constantly encounter geometrically complex landscapes. Successful navigation requires that they understand geometric features of these landscapes, including boundaries, landmarks, corners and curved areas, all of which collectively define the geometry of the environment1-12. Crucial to the reconstruction of the geometric layout of natural environments are concave and convex features, such as corners and protrusions. However, the neural substrates that could underlie the perception of concavity and convexity in the environment remain elusive. Here we show that the dorsal subiculum contains neurons that encode corners across environmental geometries in an allocentric reference frame. Using longitudinal calcium imaging in freely behaving mice, we find that corner cells tune their activity to reflect the geometric properties of corners, including corner angles, wall height and the degree of wall intersection. A separate population of subicular neurons encode convex corners of both larger environments and discrete objects. Both corner cells are non-overlapping with the population of subicular neurons that encode environmental boundaries. Furthermore, corner cells that encode concave or convex corners generalize their activity such that they respond, respectively, to concave or convex curvatures within an environment. Together, our findings suggest that the subiculum contains the geometric information needed to reconstruct the shape and layout of naturalistic spatial environments.


Subject(s)
Environment , Form Perception , Hippocampus , Neurons , Animals , Female , Male , Mice , Calcium/analysis , Calcium/metabolism , Form Perception/physiology , Hippocampus/cytology , Hippocampus/physiology , Neurons/metabolism , Neurons/physiology , Surface Properties
13.
eNeuro ; 11(3)2024 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38423791

ABSTRACT

The cortical visual area, V4, has been considered to code contours that contribute to the intermediate-level representation of objects. The neural responses to the complex contour features intrinsic to natural contours are expected to clarify the essence of the representation. To approach the cortical coding of natural contours, we investigated the simultaneous coding of multiple contour features in monkey (Macaca fuscata) V4 neurons and their population-level representation. A substantial number of neurons showed significant tuning for two or more features such as curvature and closure, indicating that a substantial number of V4 neurons simultaneously code multiple contour features. A large portion of the neurons responded vigorously to acutely curved contours that surrounded the center of classical receptive field, suggesting that V4 neurons tend to code prominent features of object contours. The analysis of mutual information (MI) between the neural responses and each contour feature showed that most neurons exhibited similar magnitudes for each type of MI, indicating that many neurons showing the responses depended on multiple contour features. We next examined the population-level representation by using multidimensional scaling analysis. The neural preferences to the multiple contour features and that to natural stimuli compared with silhouette stimuli increased along with the primary and secondary axes, respectively, indicating the contribution of the multiple contour features and surface textures in the population responses. Our analyses suggested that V4 neurons simultaneously code multiple contour features in natural images and represent contour and surface properties in population.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Visual Cortex , Animals , Macaca mulatta , Visual Cortex/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Photic Stimulation
14.
Nat Hum Behav ; 8(2): 320-335, 2024 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37996497

ABSTRACT

Many surface cues support three-dimensional shape perception, but humans can sometimes still see shape when these features are missing-such as when an object is covered with a draped cloth. Here we propose a framework for three-dimensional shape perception that explains perception in both typical and atypical cases as analysis-by-synthesis, or inference in a generative model of image formation. The model integrates intuitive physics to explain how shape can be inferred from the deformations it causes to other objects, as in cloth draping. Behavioural and computational studies comparing this account with several alternatives show that it best matches human observers (total n = 174) in both accuracy and response times, and is the only model that correlates significantly with human performance on difficult discriminations. We suggest that bottom-up deep neural network models are not fully adequate accounts of human shape perception, and point to how machine vision systems might achieve more human-like robustness.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Humans , Form Perception/physiology , Neural Networks, Computer , Cues
15.
J Vis ; 23(14): 4, 2023 Dec 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38091030

ABSTRACT

Gestalten in visual perception are defined by emergent properties of the whole, which cannot be predicted from the sum of its parts; rather, they arise by virtue of inherent principles, the Laws of Seeing. This review attempts to assign neurophysiological correlates to select emergent properties in motion and contour perception and proposes parallels to the processing of local versus global attributes by classical versus contextual receptive fields. The aim is to identify Gestalt neurons in the visual system to account for the Laws of Seeing in causal terms and to explain "Why do things look as they do" (Koffka, 1935, p. 76).


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Motion Perception , Humans , Visual Perception/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology
16.
Neural Comput ; 36(1): 33-74, 2023 Dec 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38052088

ABSTRACT

Under difficult viewing conditions, the brain's visual system uses a variety of recurrent modulatory mechanisms to augment feedforward processing. One resulting phenomenon is contour integration, which occurs in the primary visual (V1) cortex and strengthens neural responses to edges if they belong to a larger smooth contour. Computational models have contributed to an understanding of the circuit mechanisms of contour integration, but less is known about its role in visual perception. To address this gap, we embedded a biologically grounded model of contour integration in a task-driven artificial neural network and trained it using a gradient-descent variant. We used this model to explore how brain-like contour integration may be optimized for high-level visual objectives as well as its potential roles in perception. When the model was trained to detect contours in a background of random edges, a task commonly used to examine contour integration in the brain, it closely mirrored the brain in terms of behavior, neural responses, and lateral connection patterns. When trained on natural images, the model enhanced weaker contours and distinguished whether two points lay on the same versus different contours. The model learned robust features that generalized well to out-of-training-distribution stimuli. Surprisingly, and in contrast with the synthetic task, a parameter-matched control network without recurrence performed the same as or better than the model on the natural-image tasks. Thus, a contour integration mechanism is not essential to perform these more naturalistic contour-related tasks. Finally, the best performance in all tasks was achieved by a modified contour integration model that did not distinguish between excitatory and inhibitory neurons.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Visual Cortex , Visual Cortex/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Form Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Learning
17.
J Vis ; 23(7): 2, 2023 07 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37405737

ABSTRACT

Eye tracking studies suggest that refixations-fixations to locations previously visited-serve to recover information lost or missed during earlier exploration of a visual scene. These studies have largely ignored the role of precursor fixations-previous fixations on locations the eyes return to later. We consider the possibility that preparations to return later are already made during precursor fixations. This process would mark precursor fixations as a special category of fixations, that is, distinct in neural activity from other fixation categories such as refixations and fixations to locations visited only once. To capture the neural signals associated with fixation categories, we analyzed electroencephalograms (EEGs) and eye movements recorded simultaneously in a free-viewing contour search task. We developed a methodological pipeline involving regression-based deconvolution modeling, allowing our analyses to account for overlapping EEG responses owing to the saccade sequence and other oculomotor covariates. We found that precursor fixations were preceded by the largest saccades among the fixation categories. Independent of the effect of saccade length, EEG amplitude was enhanced in precursor fixations compared with the other fixation categories 200 to 400 ms after fixation onsets, most noticeably over the occipital areas. We concluded that precursor fixations play a pivotal role in visual perception, marking the continuous occurrence of transitions between exploratory and exploitative modes of eye movement in natural viewing behavior.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular , Form Perception , Humans , Eye Movements , Saccades , Visual Perception/physiology , Form Perception/physiology
18.
J Neurosci ; 43(29): 5378-5390, 2023 07 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37369590

ABSTRACT

Radial frequency (RF) patterns, created by sinusoidal modulations of a circle's radius, are processed globally when RF is low. These closed shapes therefore offer a useful way to interrogate the human visual system for global processing of curvature. RF patterns elicit greater responses than those to radial gratings in V4 and more anterior face-selective regions of the ventral visual pathway. This is largely consistent with work on nonhuman primates showing curvature processing emerges in V4, but is evident also higher up the ventral visual stream. Rather than contrasting RF patterns with other stimuli, we presented them at varied frequencies in a regimen that allowed tunings to RF to be derived from 8 human participants (3 female). We found tuning to low RF in lateral occipital areas and to some extent in V4. In a control experiment, we added a high-frequency ripple to the stimuli disrupting the local contour. Low-frequency tuning to these stimuli remained in the ventral visual stream, underscoring its role in global processing of shape curvature. We then used representational similarity analysis to show that, in lateral occipital areas, the neural representation was related to stimulus similarity, when it was computed with a model that captured how stimuli are perceived. We therefore show that global processing of shape curvature emerges in the ventral visual stream as early as V4, but is found more strongly in lateral occipital regions, which exhibit responses and representations that relate well to perception.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We show that tuning to low radial frequencies, known to engage global shape processing mechanisms, was localized to lateral occipital regions. When low-level stimulus properties were accounted for such tuning emerged in V4 and LO2 in addition to the object-selective region LO. We also documented representations of global shape properties in lateral occipital regions, and these representations were predicted well by a proxy of the perceptual difference between the stimuli.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Visual Pathways , Animals , Humans , Female , Visual Pathways/physiology , Radius , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Occipital Lobe , Form Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation
19.
J Neurosci ; 43(22): 4129-4143, 2023 05 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37185098

ABSTRACT

The mechanisms involved in transforming early visual signals to curvature representations in V4 are unknown. We propose a hierarchical model that reveals V1/V2 encodings that are essential components for this transformation to the reported curvature representations in V4. Then, by relaxing the often-imposed prior of a single Gaussian, V4 shape selectivity is learned in the last layer of the hierarchy from Macaque V4 responses. We found that V4 cells integrate multiple shape parts from the full spatial extent of their receptive fields with similar excitatory and inhibitory contributions. Our results uncover new details in existing data about shape selectivity in V4 neurons that with additional experiments can enhance our understanding of processing in this area. Accordingly, we propose designs for a stimulus set that allow removing shape parts without disturbing the curvature signal to isolate part contributions to V4 responses.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Selectivity to convex and concave shape parts in V4 neurons has been repeatedly reported. Nonetheless, the mechanisms that yield such selectivities in the ventral stream remain unknown. We propose a hierarchical computational model that incorporates findings of the various visual areas involved in shape processing and suggest mechanisms that transform the shape signal from low-level features to convex/concave part representations. Learning shape selectivity from Macaque V4 responses in the final processing stage in our model, we found that V4 neurons integrate shape parts from the full spatial extent of their receptive field with both facilitatory and inhibitory contributions. These results reveal hidden information in existing V4 data that with additional experiments can enhance our understanding of processing in V4.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Visual Cortex , Animals , Visual Cortex/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Macaca , Neurons/physiology , Brain , Visual Pathways/physiology , Photic Stimulation
20.
J Vis ; 23(5): 10, 2023 05 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37171805

ABSTRACT

A new source of information is proposed for the perception of three-dimensional (3D) shape from shading that identifies surface concavities from the curvature of the luminance field. Two experiments measured the abilities of human observers to identify concavities on smoothly curved shaded surfaces depicted with several different patterns of illumination and several different material properties. Observers were required to identify any apparent concavities along designated cross sections of the depicted objects and to mark each concavity with an adjustable dot. To analyze the results, we computed both the surface curvature and the luminance curvature along each image cross section. The results revealed that most responses were in concave regions of the luminance profiles, although they were often shifted in phase relative to the curvature of the depicted surfaces. This pattern of performance was surprisingly robust over large changes in the pattern of illumination or surface material properties. Our analysis predicts that observers should make false alarm responses in regions where a luminance concavity does not correspond to a surface concavity, and our empirical results confirm that prediction.


Subject(s)
Depth Perception , Form Perception , Humans , Depth Perception/physiology , Lighting , Photic Stimulation/methods , Surface Properties , Form Perception/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...