Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 28
Filter
1.
Dev Psychobiol ; 58(5): 578-99, 2016 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26956370

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: This study tested the hypothesis that maternal physiological and psychological variables during pregnancy discriminate between theoretically informed infant stress reactivity profiles. METHODS: The sample comprised 254 women and their infants. Maternal mood, salivary cortisol, respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), and salivary α-amylase (sAA) were assessed at 15 and 32 weeks gestational age. Infant salivary cortisol, RSA, and sAA reactivity were assessed in response to a structured laboratory frustration task at 6 months of age. Infant responses were used to classify them into stress reactivity profiles using three different classification schemes: hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA)-axis, autonomic, and multi-system. Discriminant function analyses evaluated the prenatal variables that best discriminated infant reactivity profiles within each classification scheme. RESULTS: Maternal stress biomarkers, along with self-reported psychological distress during pregnancy, discriminated between infant stress reactivity profiles. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that maternal psychological and physiological states during pregnancy have broad effects on the development of the infant stress response systems. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Dev Psychobiol 58: 578-599, 2016.


Subject(s)
Autonomic Nervous System/physiology , Hypothalamo-Hypophyseal System/physiology , Infant Behavior/classification , Pituitary-Adrenal System/physiology , Pregnancy Complications , Pregnancy/physiology , Stress, Psychological , Adult , Affect/physiology , Biomarkers , Female , Humans , Hydrocortisone/metabolism , Hypothalamo-Hypophyseal System/metabolism , Infant , Infant Behavior/physiology , Infant Behavior/psychology , Longitudinal Studies , Pituitary-Adrenal System/metabolism , Pregnancy/metabolism , Pregnancy/psychology , Pregnancy Complications/metabolism , Pregnancy Complications/physiopathology , Pregnancy Complications/psychology , Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia/physiology , Stress, Psychological/metabolism , Stress, Psychological/physiopathology , Stress, Psychological/psychology , alpha-Amylases/metabolism
2.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 52: 32-42, 2015 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25459891

ABSTRACT

Infant neurobehavior, a potential sentinel of future mental and behavioral morbidity characterized in part by reflex symmetry, excitability and habituation to stimuli, is influenced by aspects of the intrauterine environment partially through epigenetic alterations of genes involved in the stress response. DNA methylation of two related cortisol response genes, the glucocorticoid receptor (NR3C1), a nuclear receptor to which cortisol binds, and 11-beta hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase (HSD11B2), the enzyme responsible for conversion of cortisol into inactive cortisone, independently associate with infant neurobehavior. Although these factors are part of a common cortisol regulation pathway, the combined effect of DNA methylation of these factors on infant neurobehavior has not been characterized. Therefore, we conducted an examination of the joint contribution of NR3C1 and HSD11B2 DNA methylation on infant neurobehavior. Among 372 healthy term newborns, we tested the interaction between placental NR3C1 and HSD11B2 DNA methylation in association with neurobehavior as assessed with the validated NICU Network Neurobehavioral Scales. Controlling for confounders, interactions between DNA methylation of these genes were detected for distinct domains of neurobehavior (habituation, excitability, asymmetrical reflexes). Moreover, different patterns of DNA methylation across the cortisol regulation pathway associated with different neurobehavioral phenotypes. Those with low NR3C1 methylation but high HSD11B2 methylation had lower excitability scores; those with high NR3C1 methylation but low HSD11B2 methylation had more asymmetrical reflexes; those with high DNA methylation across the entire pathway had higher habituation scores. These results suggest that epigenetic alterations across the cortisol regulation pathway may contribute to different neurobehavioral phenotypes, likely though varying degrees of glucocorticoid exposure during gestation. While the postnatal environment may continue to affect neurobehavioral risk, this study provides novel insights into the molecular basis for fetal origins of mental conditions.


Subject(s)
11-beta-Hydroxysteroid Dehydrogenase Type 2/metabolism , DNA Methylation , Epigenesis, Genetic/physiology , Gene Expression Regulation , Infant Behavior/physiology , Placenta/metabolism , Receptors, Glucocorticoid/metabolism , 11-beta-Hydroxysteroid Dehydrogenase Type 2/genetics , Epigenesis, Genetic/genetics , Female , Humans , Infant Behavior/classification , Infant, Newborn , Male , Phenotype , Pregnancy , Receptors, Glucocorticoid/genetics
3.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 22(2): 103-15, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23076366

ABSTRACT

Precursors of child psychiatric disorders are often present in infancy, but little is known about the prevalence and course of general psychopathology in population-based samples of children 0-3 years. We examined whether homogeneous behavioural and developmental profiles could be identified in children aged 14-15 months (M = 14.84; SD = 2.19), and we explored whether or not these profiles corresponded with existing classifications of DSM-IV-TR, ICD-10, and DC 0-3R. Parents of 6,330 children answered 74 items about externalizing, internalizing, and social-communicative behaviour. Exploratory factor analysis revealed nine factors: deviant communication, negative emotionality, deviant reactive behaviour, deviant play behaviour, demanding behaviour, social anxiety/inhibition, advanced social interaction problems, basic social interaction problems, and sleep problems. Latent class analysis yielded five profiles, of which three were associated with increased behavioural and developmental problems. Some infants (5.7 %) had communication and social interaction problems corresponding to multisystem developmental disorders (DC 0-3R) and suggestive of anxiety, mood, or pervasive developmental disorders (DSM-IV-TR, ICD-10). Other infants (16.4 %) had communication problems, possibly precursors of communication, language, or speech disorders (DSM-IV-TR, ICD-10). Yet other infants (10.8 %) showed negative and demanding behaviour suggestive of regulation disorders (DC 0-3R), attention-deficit and disruptive behaviour disorders (DSM-IV-TR), or hyperkinetic and conduct disorders (ICD-10). Thus, even in infancy certain distinct behavioural and developmental profiles can be recognized. This combined approach will enable follow-up research into the stability of factors, classes, and profiles over time, and will facilitate early detection, diagnosis, and treatment of behavioural and developmental problems.


Subject(s)
Child Development/physiology , Infant Behavior/physiology , Mental Disorders/diagnosis , Prodromal Symptoms , Child Development/classification , Cohort Studies , Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant Behavior/classification , International Classification of Diseases , Male , Netherlands/epidemiology , Parents , Surveys and Questionnaires
4.
Semin Fetal Neonatal Med ; 17(6): 324-9, 2012 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22809484

ABSTRACT

The human brain is very sensitive to environmental changes affecting its growth and development. Environmental changes influence neonatal behavior after birth, enabling continuity between prenatal and postnatal behavior, but postnatal adaptation could be considered as discontinuity. Thus there is the question of environmental discontinuity between intrauterine conditions characterized by existence of microgravity and extrauterine life with gravity as a developmental condition sine qua non. Four-dimensional ultrasound is currently being assessed as a functional prenatal screening test for detection of neurological impairment in utero. The Kurjak Antenatal Neurodevelopmental Test (KANET) combines the assessment of fetal behavior, general movements, and three out of four signs that have been postnatally considered as symptoms of possible neurodevelopmental impairment (neurological thumb, overlapping sutures and small head circumference). Although the KANET has been tested on normal and high-risk pregnancies, the significance of the test for detection and prevention of neurodevelopmental disability is still questionable.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological , Central Nervous System Diseases/diagnosis , Diagnostic Techniques, Neurological , Environment , Fetal Diseases/diagnostic imaging , Ultrasonography, Prenatal/methods , Central Nervous System/growth & development , Central Nervous System/physiopathology , Early Diagnosis , Female , Fetal Development , Humans , Infant Behavior/classification , Infant Behavior/physiology , Infant, Newborn , Multicenter Studies as Topic , Organ Dysfunction Scores , Predictive Value of Tests , Pregnancy , Pregnancy Outcome , Pregnancy, High-Risk , Weightlessness
5.
Behav Res Methods ; 44(4): 1063-78, 2012 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22648694

ABSTRACT

In the present article, we introduce the continuous unified electronic (CUE) diary method, a longitudinal, event-based, electronic parent report method that allows real-time recording of infant and child behavior in natural contexts. Thirty-nine expectant mothers were trained to identify and record target behaviors into programmed handheld computers. From birth to 18 months, maternal reporters recorded the initial, second, and third occurrences of seven target motor behaviors: palmar grasp, rolls from side to back, reaching when sitting, pincer grip, crawling, walking, and climbing stairs. Compliance was assessed as two valid entries per behavior: 97 % of maternal reporters met compliance criteria. Reliability was assessed by comparing diary entries with researcher assessments for three of the motor behaviors: palmar grasp, pincer grip and walking. A total of 81 % of maternal reporters met reliability criteria. For those three target behaviors, age of emergence was compared across data from the CUE diary method and researcher assessments. The CUE diary method was found to detect behaviors earlier and with greater sensitivity to individual differences. The CUE diary method is shown to be a reliable methodological tool for studying processes of change in human development.


Subject(s)
Child Development/classification , Documentation/methods , Electronic Health Records , Health Records, Personal , Infant Behavior/classification , Adolescent , Adult , Checklist , Computers, Handheld , Electronic Health Records/standards , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Mothers , Parents , Patient Compliance , Reproducibility of Results , Young Adult
6.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 51(3): 249-260.e25, 2012 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22365461

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To chart the emergence of precursors and early signs of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and autistic traits in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, a prospective longitudinal cohort study of the surviving offspring of 14,541 pregnant women from southwestern England with an expected delivery date between April 1991 and December 1992. METHOD: Parents' contemporaneous reports of their infant's development (241 questionnaire responses collected up to 30 months of age) were examined in relation to the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder by age 11 years (n = 86) and a measure of autistic traits, derived by factor analysis. RESULTS: Among the children later diagnosed with ASD, concerns about vision and hearing were more often reported in the first year, and differences in social, communication, and fine motor skills were evident from 6 months of age. Repetitive behaviors and differences in play, imitation, and feeding habits were reported in the second year. Differences in temperament emerged at 24 months of age and bowel habit by 30 months. All of these early signs were strongly associated with the presence of autistic traits in the rest of the population and these differences were often evident in the first year of development. Over the first 30 months of development, the best predictors of both later ASD and autistic traits included the Social Achievement and Communication scores from the Denver Developmental Screening Test, measures of communicative skills (Vocabulary and Combines Words) from the MacArthur Infant Communicative Development Inventories, and a repetitive behavior score. CONCLUSIONS: Precursors, early signs, and other developmental differences were reported in the first year of development among children from the general population who later developed autism spectrum disorder and subtler autistic traits. Other differences emerged and unfolded as development progressed. The findings confirm the long-held suspicion that early differences underscore the multifaceted nature of autism spectrum disorder and the broader autism phenotype, and highlight the centrality of impairments in social communication skills.


Subject(s)
Child Development Disorders, Pervasive , Child Development , Developmental Disabilities , Infant Behavior , Age of Onset , Child , Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/diagnosis , Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/epidemiology , Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/etiology , Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/psychology , Child, Preschool , Communication , Developmental Disabilities/complications , Developmental Disabilities/diagnosis , Developmental Disabilities/psychology , Early Diagnosis , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant Behavior/classification , Infant Behavior/psychology , Male , Parents/psychology , Play and Playthings/psychology , Pregnancy , Prospective Studies , Social Behavior , Stereotyped Behavior
7.
Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev ; 15(2): 113-28, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22262040

ABSTRACT

Behavioral and emotional problems are highly prevalent in early childhood and represent an important focus of practice for clinical child and pediatric psychologists. Although psychological or psychiatric disorders are not typically diagnosed in children under the age of 2 years, recent research has demonstrated the appropriateness of assessing behavioral and emotional problems during the first 2 years of life (defined throughout as "infancy"). The current paper provides a systematic review of assessment procedures used to identify behavioral and emotional problems during infancy. Existing assessment procedures for infants take the form of parent- or caregiver-report questionnaires, observational coding procedures, and diagnostic classification systems. The questionnaires and observational coding procedures both had substantial psychometric evidence for use with infants, although observational coding may have limited utility in clinical practice. The classification systems have less empirical support for use with infants, and further research is necessary to demonstrate the appropriateness of these procedures with infants. Utilizing the reviewed procedures to assess behavioral and emotional problems in infants can have a substantial impact in research and practice settings, and further research is needed to determine the usefulness of these procedures in developing, testing, and implementing preventive and early intervention programs for infants and their families.


Subject(s)
Infant Behavior/psychology , Parent-Child Relations , Psychology, Child/methods , Risk Assessment , Humans , Infant , Infant Behavior/classification , Psychometrics , Surveys and Questionnaires , Temperament
8.
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev ; 42(6): 623-33, 2011 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21667157

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to analyze and compare diagnoses of patients from a special outpatient department for infants, toddlers and preschoolers. Specifically, overlap, age and gender differences according to the two classification systems DC: 0-3R and ICD-10 were examined. 299 consecutive children aged 0-5; 11 years received both ICD-10 and DC: 0-3R diagnoses. All ICD-10 diagnoses were given prospectively at the initial contact. The DC: 0-3R diagnoses were allotted in mutual consensus conferences. The most common ICD-10 disorders were Oppositional Defiant (ODD), Hyperkinetic (HKD) and Emotional Disorders. In DC: 0-3R, nearly half of the children could not be coded adequately ('Other Disorders'). Sleep Behavior Disorders were followed by Feeding Behavior and Regulation Disorders. Both classification systems are needed in infant, toddler and preschool psychiatry. There is a good concordance of the two systems for Feeding, Sleeping, Adjustment and Attachment Disorders, whereas the ICD-10 diagnoses HKD and ODD are not adequately represented in DC: 0-3R.


Subject(s)
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , Infant Behavior/classification , International Classification of Diseases , Mental Disorders/classification , Child Psychiatry/methods , Child, Preschool , Classification/methods , Female , Humans , Infant , Male
9.
Acta Paediatr ; 97(1): 21-6, 2008 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18201309

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Sucking problems in preterm infants can be specified by means of visual observation. The Neonatal Oral-Motor Assessment Scale (NOMAS) is the visual observation method most commonly used to assess the non-nutritive sucking (NNS) and nutritive sucking (NS) skills of infants up to approximately 8 weeks postterm. During the first 2 min of a regular feeding the infant's sucking skill is assessed, either immediately or on video. Although NOMAS has been used since 1993, little is known about the method's reliability. The aim of our study was to determine the test-retest and inter-rater reliability of NOMAS. METHODS: The 75 infants included in this study were born at 26-36 weeks postmenstrual age (PMA). Four observers participated in the study. They were trained and certified to administer NOMAS in the Netherlands by M.M. Palmer between 2000 and 2002. RESULTS: We found the test-retest agreement of NOMAS to be 'fair' to 'almost perfect' (Cohen's kappa [kappa] between 0.33 and 0.94), whereas the inter-rater agreement with respect to the diagnosis was 'moderate' to 'substantial' (Cohen's kappa, between 0.40 and 0.65). As a diagnostic tool, however, the current version of NOMAS cannot be used for both full-term and preterm infants. For a measuring instrument such as NOMAS, one should aim at reliability coefficients for inter-rater and test-retest agreement of at least 0.8. A Cohen's kappa of 0.6 or less we find unacceptable. Nonetheless, by observing sucking and swallowing according to a protocol much useful information can be gathered about the development of an infant's sucking skills. For instance, whether the infant is able to co-ordinate sucking and swallowing, whether the infant can maintain sucking, swallowing and breathing during the continuous phase and whether the infant is able to suck rhythmically with equally long bursts. In addition, NOMAS offers useful aids for intervention. CONCLUSIONS: NOMAS should be re-adjusted in order to improve inter-rater agreement, and at the same time current insights into the development of sucking and swallowing should be incorporated in the method.


Subject(s)
Feeding Behavior/classification , Infant Behavior/classification , Neurologic Examination/instrumentation , Sucking Behavior/classification , Deglutition , Feeding Behavior/physiology , Humans , Infant , Infant Behavior/physiology , Infant Nutritional Physiological Phenomena , Infant, Extremely Low Birth Weight , Infant, Newborn , Infant, Premature , Neuropsychological Tests , Observation , Reproducibility of Results , Sucking Behavior/physiology
10.
Clin Pediatr (Phila) ; 46(9): 801-5, 2007 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17641122

ABSTRACT

Because services for children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are scarce, when children fail a broadband screening measure, providers need to carefully discern which children need ASD evaluations and which do not. This research considers how well a broadband screening test sorts those with and without probable ASD. The subjects were 427 children between 18 and 59 months of age with elevated risk scores on broadband screening, ie, Parents' Evaluation of Developmental Status (PEDS), a 10-item measure eliciting parents' concerns. Parents also completed the Modified Checklist of Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT), an autism specific screen. The results showed that of the 427 children at risk on PEDS, 34% (N = 144) passed the M-CHAT. To determine whether these potential overreferrals could be reduced, parents' concerns on PEDS were used to predict M-CHAT results. Three or more discrete types of concerns, varying by age, characterized children who failed the M-CHAT while fewer than 3 were associated with passing. This reduced overreferrals by 70% while maintaining high levels of sensitivity (81%). Although compliance with the American Academy of Pediatrics recommendations for both broadband and autism-specific screening at 18 and 24 months is still recommended, viewing performance patterns on a broadband screening test can substantially reduce overreferrals to autism specialty services.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/diagnosis , Child Behavior/classification , Infant Behavior/classification , Parents/psychology , Child, Preschool , Humans , Infant , Logistic Models , Surveys and Questionnaires
11.
Pediatrics ; 114(1): 65-72, 2004 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15231909

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program (NIDCAP) is widely used in neonatal intensive care units and comprises 85 discrete infant behaviors, some of which may communicate infant distress. The objective of this study was to identify developmentally relevant movements indicative of pain in preterm infants. METHODS: Forty-four preterm infants were assessed at 32 weeks' gestational age (GA) during 3 phases (baseline, lance/squeeze, and recovery) of routine blood collection in the neonatal intensive care unit. The NIDCAP and Neonatal Facial Coding System (NFCS) were coded from separate continuous bedside video recordings; mean heart rate (mHR) was derived from digitally sampled continuous electrographic recordings. Analysis of variance (phase x gender) with Bonferroni corrections was used to compare differences in NIDCAP, NFCS, and mHR. Pearson correlations were used to examine relationships between the NIDCAP and infant background characteristics. RESULTS: NFCS and mHR increased significantly to lance/squeeze. Eight NIDCAP behaviors also increased significantly to lance/squeeze. Another 5 NIDCAP behaviors decreased significantly to lance/squeeze. Infants who had lower GA at birth, had been sicker, had experienced more painful procedures, or had greater morphine exposure showed increased hand movements indicative of increased distress. CONCLUSIONS: Of the 85 NIDCAP behaviors, a subset of 8 NIDCAP movements were associated with pain. Particularly for infants who are born at early GAs, addition of these movements to commonly used measures may improve the accuracy of pain assessment.


Subject(s)
Infant Behavior , Infant, Premature/physiology , Pain Measurement/methods , Pain/diagnosis , Acute Disease , Analysis of Variance , Facial Expression , Female , Heart Rate , Humans , Infant Behavior/classification , Infant Care , Infant, Newborn , Infant, Premature/psychology , Intensive Care Units, Neonatal , Male , Movement , Sleep , Wakefulness
12.
Dev Psychol ; 39(3): 387-404, 2003 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12760508

ABSTRACT

Contemporary attachment research is based on the assumption that at least three types of infant attachment patterns exist: secure, avoidant, and resistant. It is not known, however, whether individual differences in attachment organization are more consistent with a continuous or a categorical model. The authors addressed this issue by applying P. E. Meehl's (1973, 1992) taxometric techniques for distinguishing latent types (i.e., classes, natural kinds) from latent continua (i.e., dimensions) to Strange Situation data on 1,139 fifteen-month-old children from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. The results indicate that variation in attachment patterns is largely continuous, not categorical. The discussion focuses on the implications of dimensional models of individual differences for attachment theory and research.


Subject(s)
Infant Behavior , Object Attachment , Personality Assessment/statistics & numerical data , Psychology, Child , Social Environment , Child Development , Female , Humans , Individuality , Infant , Infant Behavior/classification , Male , Models, Statistical , Psychological Theory , Psychology, Child/classification , Psychology, Child/statistics & numerical data , Psychometrics
13.
Dev Psychol ; 39(3): 405-8; discussion 423-9, 2003 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12760509

ABSTRACT

The article by R.C. Fraley and S.J. Spieker (2003) serves to remind the discipline of the possible virtues of assessing attachments on continua, a practice that has a long history in attachment research. In this commentary, the author further develops the potential contributions of this approach to assessment and advocates renewed efforts toward assessment of attachments on a single continuum of emotional security. The author contends that theory is essential as a guide for new directions in attachment assessment and that Bowlby's notions of secure base and emotional security provide the needed conceptual foundation for these further developments (E. Waters & E. M. Cummings, 2000). Moreover, challenges that have been made historically to the scoring of attachment on a security continuum are addressed. New means for continuously scoring attachment are advocated as a supplement to the primary direction of categorically assessing attachment patterns.


Subject(s)
Emotions , Infant Behavior , Object Attachment , Personality Assessment/statistics & numerical data , Psychology, Child , Emotions/classification , Female , Humans , Individuality , Infant , Infant Behavior/classification , Male , Models, Statistical , Psychology, Child/classification , Psychology, Child/statistics & numerical data , Psychometrics
14.
Dev Psychol ; 39(3): 409-12; discussion 423-9, 2003 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12760510

ABSTRACT

R.C. Fraley and S.J. Spieker (2003) refocused attention on the important question of continuous versus categorical measurement of infant attachment. This commentary begins with a brief response to Fraley and Spieker's position. Next, it highlights the usefulness of the categorical approach by reviewing some of the major advances in the field that have been fostered by a categorical approach. These include the identification of the disorganized attachment group, the development of the concept of conditional behavioral strategies, the creation of systems for coding attachment beyond infancy, and a greater understanding of cross-generational associations. The author then makes several propositions for future research that are necessary for clarifying how best to measure infant attachment.


Subject(s)
Emotions , Infant Behavior , Object Attachment , Personality Assessment/statistics & numerical data , Psychology, Child , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant Behavior/classification , Male , Psychology, Child/classification , Psychology, Child/statistics & numerical data , Psychometrics , Reproducibility of Results , Social Environment
15.
Dev Psychol ; 39(3): 417-22; discussion 423-9, 2003 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12760512

ABSTRACT

Ainsworth's description of attachment patterns in the Strange Situation is one of the best known and most enduring descriptive insights in developmental psychology. Yet attachment theorists have paid little attention to whether ABC classifications represent a true taxonomy or to mechanisms that might produce truly distinct patterns of attachment. This comment focuses on three questions. Does attachment theory require distinct patterns of attachment? How can taxonomic analysis contribute to an understanding of individual differences in attachment security? And are attachment theorists asking the right questions? The authors conclude that attachment theory is indifferent to the structure (taxonomic or dimensional) of individual differences. Nonetheless, taxonomic search methods can make important contributions to attachment study if research is broadened to include secure base behavior in naturalistic settings.


Subject(s)
Infant Behavior , Object Attachment , Personality Assessment/statistics & numerical data , Psychology, Child , Female , Humans , Individuality , Infant , Infant Behavior/classification , Male , Models, Statistical , Psychology, Child/classification , Psychology, Child/statistics & numerical data , Psychometrics , Reproducibility of Results , Social Environment
16.
Dev Psychol ; 39(3): 413-6; discussion 423-9, 2003 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12760511

ABSTRACT

Ainsworth's system of classifying patterns of attachment behavior has served the field well and will not be easily displaced--not because of a commitment by attachment researchers to the taxonomic status of these categories but rather because of difficulties lying in the way of a dimensional approach. Foremost among these is the large number of dimensions used in making classifications and the need to develop reliable scales to tap them. Other problems include the need to capture how behavior changes across age and across the episodes of the Strange Situation. Others will debate R.C. Fraley and S.J. Spieker's (2003) arguments concerning taxonomic status. However, whether taxa or not, Ainsworth's categories at the least have represented well the multidimensional space underlying attachment behavior in conditions of mild stress. Only when an alternative approach is demonstrated to predict (with equal power, in practice) the vast range of outcomes associated with Ainsworth's categories will it be a candidate for substitution.


Subject(s)
Infant Behavior , Object Attachment , Personality Assessment/statistics & numerical data , Psychology, Child , Female , Humans , Individuality , Infant , Infant Behavior/classification , Male , Models, Statistical , Psychology, Child/classification , Psychology, Child/statistics & numerical data , Psychometrics , Reproducibility of Results
17.
Pediatrics ; 108(4): 893-7, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11581441

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To assess the impact of varying definitions of excessive crying and infantile colic on prevalence estimates and to assess to what extent these definitions comprise the same children. METHODS: Parents of 3345 infants aged 1, 3, and 6 months (response: 96.5%) were interviewed on the crying behavior of their infant in a Dutch cross-sectional national population-based study. We computed the prevalence of excessive crying according to 10 published definitions regarding parent-reported duration of infant crying and the parents' experience. We measured concordance between pairs of definitions by Cohen's kappa (agreement adjusted for chance agreement). RESULTS: Overall prevalence rates of excessive crying varied strongly between definitions, from 1.5% to 11.9%. They were always highest in 1-month-old infants. Concordance between definitions was only excellent (kappa > 0.75) if they were closely related, such as crying for >3 hours/day for >3 days/week for the preceding 2 or 3 weeks. Concordance between less closely related definitions was much weaker. Concordance between definitions that were based on duration and on parental experience was mostly poor (kappa: 0.17-0.53 for infants aged 1 and 3 months). CONCLUSIONS: Different definitions of excessive crying lead to the inclusion of very dissimilar groups of infants. We recommend presenting study results using clearly described definitions, preferably concerning both duration of crying and parental distress. This may improve the comparability of studies on the cause and treatment of excessive infant crying. The impact of the method of data collection on this comparability needs additional study.colic, preventive child health care, prevention, infancy.


Subject(s)
Colic/diagnosis , Crying/psychology , Infant Behavior/classification , Colic/epidemiology , Colic/psychology , Cross-Sectional Studies , Humans , Infant , Infant Behavior/psychology , Infant, Newborn , Netherlands/epidemiology , Parents/psychology , Population Surveillance , Prevalence , Sampling Studies , Terminology as Topic , Time Factors
18.
J Child Health Care ; 5(3): 111-6, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11811440

ABSTRACT

Excessive infantile crying is a widespread problem which exerts a serious impact on families' lives. The prevalence, cause, and treatment of the problem have been addressed in the literature for many years but with varying degrees of success. Variation in definition of excessive crying leads to difficulty when comparing results between studies. Flawed method and inadequate sampling characterise many studies in this field, often seriously limiting the usefulness of the findings. Progress has been made in some aspects of excessive crying, and other approaches hold promise of success, but wide gaps remain with regard to cause and treatment.


Subject(s)
Crying/physiology , Crying/psychology , Infant Behavior/classification , Colic/epidemiology , Colic/physiopathology , Colic/psychology , Humans , Infant , Infant Behavior/physiology , Infant Behavior/psychology , Infant, Newborn , Parents/psychology , Prevalence
19.
Can J Occup Ther ; 67(2): 110-9, 2000 Apr.
Article in French | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10829558

ABSTRACT

Occupational therapists evaluate the capacities of infants at younger ages than ever before. The variability of the infants' state has an inevitable influence on the expression of their behaviours. In order to counter the effect of this instability, Grenier (1985) suggests inducing the "liberated state," which optimizes the infant's level of attention. The aim of this study was to identify the behaviours that discriminate this liberated state from the alert state (state 4) as described by Brazelton (1973). Some of the parameters that were defined in the Synactive Theory of Development Model (Als, 1982) were used in order to document the behaviours of the infants observed in the two different states. The average age of the infants, at the time of observation, was 62 hours. The duration of the observation was 5 minutes. Among the 54 newborns that were evaluated, 21 were observed in the two states under study; 33 were evaluated in one state only. The results confirm a greater stability of the "liberated state". Stress and self-regulation behaviours, and the need for external regulation are less frequent in this state; these differences are statistically significant (p < 0.05). Goal-directed movements are more frequent in the "liberated state" (p < 0.001). The authors suggest the use of the "liberated state" to further document the early capacities of infants.


Subject(s)
Attention , Infant Behavior/classification , Occupational Therapy , Female , Goals , Humans , Infant, Newborn , Male , Stress, Psychological
20.
Child Dev ; 69(4): 1107-28, 1998 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9768489

ABSTRACT

The research explores the antecedents and consequences of attachment disorganization from a prospective longitudinal perspective. The relations of attachment disorganization/disorientation to endogenous (e.g., maternal medical history, infant temperament) and environmental (e.g., maternal caregiving quality, infant history of abuse) antecedents and to behavioral consequences from 24 months to 19 years are examined. For the 157 participants in the longitudinal study, attachment disorganization was correlated significantly with environmental antecedents (e.g., maternal relationship and risk status, caregiving quality, and infant history of maltreatment), but not with available endogenous antecedents. Infant history of attachment disorganization was correlated with consequent variables related to mother-child relationship quality at 24 and 42 months, child behavior problems in preschool, elementary school and high school, and psychopathology and dissociation in adolescence. Structural models suggest that disorganization may mediate the relations between early experience and later psychopathology and dissociation. The findings are considered within a developmental view of psychopathology, that is, pathology defined in terms of process, as a pattern of adaptation constructed by individuals in their environments.


Subject(s)
Dissociative Disorders/etiology , Mother-Child Relations , Object Attachment , Social Adjustment , Socialization , Adolescent , Adult , Chi-Square Distribution , Child , Child Abuse/psychology , Child Behavior Disorders/etiology , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant Behavior/classification , Male , Maternal Behavior , Models, Psychological , Personality Development , Prospective Studies , Regression Analysis , Temperament
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...