Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 11 de 11
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
3.
Am J Hum Genet ; 105(1): 189-197, 2019 07 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31256875

ABSTRACT

Women are under-represented in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). Despite the recent emphasis on diversity in STEM, our understanding of what drives differences between women and men scientists remains limited. This, in turn, limits our ability to intervene to level the playing field. To quantify the representation and participation of women and men at academic meetings in human genetics, we developed high-throughput and crowd-sourced approaches focused on question-asking behavior. Question asking is one voluntary and self-initiated scientific activity we can measure. Here we report that women ask fewer questions than expected regardless of their representation in talk audiences. We present evidence that external barriers affect the representation of women in STEM. However, differences in question-asking behavior suggest that internal factors also impact women's participation. We then examine the effects of specific interventions and show that wide public discussion of the relative under-participation of women in question-and-answer sessions alters question-asking behavior. We suggest that engaging the community in such projects promotes visibility of diversity issues at academic meetings and allows for efficient data collection that can be used to further explore and understand differences in conference participation.


Subject(s)
Communication , Congresses as Topic/statistics & numerical data , Natural Science Disciplines/standards , Public Opinion , Research Personnel/psychology , Societies, Scientific/statistics & numerical data , Congresses as Topic/organization & administration , Female , Humans , Male , Research Personnel/statistics & numerical data , Sex Factors , Societies, Scientific/organization & administration
5.
PLoS One ; 10(3): e0118747, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25790025

ABSTRACT

Why are some scientific disciplines, such as sociology and psychology, more fragmented into conflicting schools of thought than other fields, such as physics and biology? Furthermore, why does high fragmentation tend to coincide with limited scientific progress? We analyzed a formal model where scientists seek to identify the correct answer to a research question. Each scientist is influenced by three forces: (i) signals received from the correct answer to the question; (ii) peer influence; and (iii) noise. We observed the emergence of different macroscopic patterns of collective exploration, and studied how the three forces affect the degree to which disciplines fall apart into divergent fragments, or so-called "schools of thought". We conducted two simulation experiments where we tested (A) whether the three forces foster or hamper progress, and (B) whether disciplinary fragmentation causally affects scientific progress and vice versa. We found that fragmentation critically limits scientific progress. Strikingly, there is no effect in the opposite causal direction. What is more, our results shows that at the heart of the mechanisms driving scientific progress we find (i) social interactions, and (ii) peer disagreement. In fact, fragmentation is increased and progress limited if the simulated scientists are open to influence only by peers with very similar views, or when within-school diversity is lost. Finally, disciplines where the scientists received strong signals from the correct answer were less fragmented and experienced faster progress. We discuss model's implications for the design of social institutions fostering interdisciplinarity and participation in science.


Subject(s)
Models, Theoretical , Natural Science Disciplines/methods , Natural Science Disciplines/standards , Science/methods , Science/standards , Social Behavior , Computer Simulation , Humans
6.
An. R. Acad. Farm ; 80(3): 578-599, jul.-sept. 2014.
Article in Spanish | IBECS | ID: ibc-129516

ABSTRACT

Análisis de las relaciones de los hermanos Alexander y Wilhelm von Humboldt con la cultura y la ciencia españolas, con las que entraron en contacto en sus estudios científicos. El tema, escasamente valorado, ayuda a comprender su visión sobre España, los resultados de dichos estudios, en ocasiones discutibles, y la apreciación de los mismos desde la mentalidad española


Analysis of the relationship of Alexander and Wilhelm von Humboldt with Spanish culture and science. The theme, poorly valued, help to understand their vision of the Spain culture. The results of these studies, sometimes controversial, help to understand the appreciation of their researches in the Spanish cultural tradition


Subject(s)
Humans , Male , Female , Science/history , Science/trends , Natural Science Disciplines/history , Natural Science Disciplines/methods , Natural Science Disciplines/trends , Linguistics/history , Linguistics/methods , Linguistics/trends , Natural Science Disciplines , Natural Science Disciplines/organization & administration , Natural Science Disciplines/standards
9.
PLoS One ; 5(4): e10068, 2010 Apr 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20383332

ABSTRACT

The hypothesis of a Hierarchy of the Sciences with physical sciences at the top, social sciences at the bottom, and biological sciences in-between is nearly 200 years old. This order is intuitive and reflected in many features of academic life, but whether it reflects the "hardness" of scientific research--i.e., the extent to which research questions and results are determined by data and theories as opposed to non-cognitive factors--is controversial. This study analysed 2434 papers published in all disciplines and that declared to have tested a hypothesis. It was determined how many papers reported a "positive" (full or partial) or "negative" support for the tested hypothesis. If the hierarchy hypothesis is correct, then researchers in "softer" sciences should have fewer constraints to their conscious and unconscious biases, and therefore report more positive outcomes. Results confirmed the predictions at all levels considered: discipline, domain and methodology broadly defined. Controlling for observed differences between pure and applied disciplines, and between papers testing one or several hypotheses, the odds of reporting a positive result were around 5 times higher among papers in the disciplines of Psychology and Psychiatry and Economics and Business compared to Space Science, 2.3 times higher in the domain of social sciences compared to the physical sciences, and 3.4 times higher in studies applying behavioural and social methodologies on people compared to physical and chemical studies on non-biological material. In all comparisons, biological studies had intermediate values. These results suggest that the nature of hypotheses tested and the logical and methodological rigour employed to test them vary systematically across disciplines and fields, depending on the complexity of the subject matter and possibly other factors (e.g., a field's level of historical and/or intellectual development). On the other hand, these results support the scientific status of the social sciences against claims that they are completely subjective, by showing that, when they adopt a scientific approach to discovery, they differ from the natural sciences only by a matter of degree.


Subject(s)
Science/standards , Biological Science Disciplines/standards , Methods , Natural Science Disciplines/standards , Publications/standards , Publications/trends , Research/standards , Research/trends , Science/classification , Science/methods , Social Sciences/standards
11.
Sci Eng Ethics ; 6(1): 109-21, 2000 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11273428

ABSTRACT

Scientific misconduct includes the fabrication, falsification, and plagiarism (FFP) of concepts, data or ideas; some institutions in the United States have expanded this concept to include "other serious deviations (OSD) from accepted research practice." It is the absence of this OSD clause that distinguishes scientific misconduct policies of the past from the "research misconduct" policies that should be the basis of future federal policy in this area. This paper introduces a standard for judging whether an action should be considered research misconduct as distinguished from scientific misconduct: by this standard, research misconduct must involve activities unique to the practice of science and must have the potential to negatively affect the scientific record. Although the number of cases of scientific misconduct is uncertain (only the NIH and the NSF keep formal records), the costs are high in terms of the integrity of the scientific record, diversions from research to investigate allegations, ruined careers of those eventually exonerated, and erosion of public confidence in science. Existing scientific misconduct policies vary from institution to institution and from government agency to government agency; some have highly developed guidelines that include OSD, others have no guidelines at all. One result has been that the federal False Claims Act has been used to pursue allegations of scientific misconduct. As a consequence, such allegations have been adjudicated in federal courts, rather than judged by scientific peers. The federal government is now establishing a first-ever research misconduct policy that would apply to all research funded by the federal government regardless of which agency funded the research or whether the research was carried out in a government, industrial or university laboratory. Physical scientists, who up to now have only infrequently been the subject of scientific misconduct allegations, must nonetheless become active in the debate over research misconduct policies and how they are implemented since they will now be explicitly covered by this new federal wide policy.


Subject(s)
Natural Science Disciplines , Scientific Misconduct/legislation & jurisprudence , Costs and Cost Analysis , Government Agencies , National Institutes of Health (U.S.) , Natural Science Disciplines/legislation & jurisprudence , Natural Science Disciplines/standards , Research , United States
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...