Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 1.561
Filter
1.
PLoS One ; 19(7): e0306196, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38954709

ABSTRACT

The modified Benjamin-Bona-Mahony (mBBM) model is utilized in the optical illusion field to describe the propagation of long waves in a nonlinear dispersive medium during a visual illusion (Khater 2021). This article investigates the mBBM equation through the utilization of the rational [Formula: see text]-expansion technique to derive new analytical wave solutions. The analytical solutions we have obtained comprise hyperbolic, trigonometric, and rational functions. Some of these exact solutions closely align with previously published results in specific cases, affirming the validity of our other solutions. To provide insights into diverse wave propagation characteristics, we have conducted an in-depth analysis of these solutions using 2D, 3D, and density plots. We also investigated the effects of various parameters on the characteristics of the obtained wave solutions of the model. Moreover, we employed the techniques of linear stability to perform stability analysis of the considered model. Additionally, we have explored the stability of the associated dynamical system through the application of phase plane theory. This study also demonstrates the efficacy and capabilities of the rational [Formula: see text]-expansion approach in analyzing and extracting soliton solutions from nonlinear partial differential equations.


Subject(s)
Models, Theoretical , Humans , Optical Illusions/physiology , Nonlinear Dynamics , Algorithms
2.
Elife ; 122024 Jul 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39023517

ABSTRACT

We reliably judge locations of static objects when we walk despite the retinal images of these objects moving with every step we take. Here, we showed our brains solve this optical illusion by adopting an allocentric spatial reference frame. We measured perceived target location after the observer walked a short distance from the home base. Supporting the allocentric coding scheme, we found the intrinsic bias , which acts as a spatial reference frame for perceiving location of a dimly lit target in the dark, remained grounded at the home base rather than traveled along with the observer. The path-integration mechanism responsible for this can utilize both active and passive (vestibular) translational motion signals, but only along the horizontal direction. This asymmetric path-integration finding in human visual space perception is reminiscent of the asymmetric spatial memory finding in desert ants, pointing to nature's wondrous and logically simple design for terrestrial creatures.


Subject(s)
Distance Perception , Humans , Distance Perception/physiology , Male , Female , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Young Adult , Optical Illusions/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
3.
J Vis ; 24(7): 11, 2024 Jul 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39012639

ABSTRACT

Moving frames produce large displacements in the perceived location of flashed and continuously moving probes. In a series of experiments, we test the contributions of the probe's displacement and the frame's displacement on the strength of the frame's effect. In the first experiment, we find a dramatic position shift of flashed probes whereas the effect on a continuously moving probe is only one-third as strong. In Experiment 2, we show that the absence of an effect for the static probe is a consequence of its perceptual grouping with the static background. As long as the continuously present probe has some motion, it appears to group to some extent with the frame and show an illusory shift of intermediate magnitude. Finally, we informally explored the illusory shifts seen for a continuously moving probe when the frame itself has a more complex path. In this case, the probe appears to group more strongly with the frame. Overall, the effects of the frame on the probe demonstrate the outcome of a competition between the frame and the static background in determining the frame of reference for the probe's perceived position.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception , Photic Stimulation , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Optical Illusions/physiology
4.
Proc Biol Sci ; 291(2025): 20240414, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38889782

ABSTRACT

Visual illusions have been studied in many non-human species, spanning a wide range of biological and methodological variables. While early reviews have proved useful in providing an overview of the field, they have not been accompanied by quantitative analysis to systematically evaluate the contribution of biological and methodological moderators on the proportion of illusory choice. In the current meta-analytical study, we confirm that geometrical visual illusion perception is a general phenomenon among non-human animals. Additionally, we found that studies testing birds report stronger illusion perception compared to other classes, as do those on animals with lateral-positioned eyes compared to animals with forward-facing eyes. In terms of methodological choices, we found a positive correlation between the number of trials during training or testing and the effect sizes, while studies with larger samples report smaller effect sizes. Despite studies that trained animals with artificial stimuli showing larger effect sizes compared with those using spontaneous testing with naturalistic stimuli, like food, we found more recent studies prefer spontaneous choice over training. We discuss the challenges and bottlenecks in this area of study, which, if addressed, could lead to more successful advances in the future.


Subject(s)
Visual Perception , Animals , Optical Illusions/physiology , Illusions , Birds/physiology
5.
J Vis ; 24(6): 15, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38913017

ABSTRACT

The watercolor effect (WCE) is a striking visual illusion elicited by a bichromatic double contour, such as a light orange and a dark purple, hugging each other on a white background. Color assimilation, emanating from the lighter contour, spreads onto the enclosed surface area, thereby tinting it with a chromatic veil, not unlike a weak but real color. Map makers in the 17th century utilized the WCE to better demarcate the shape of adjoining states, while 20th-century artist Bridget Riley created illusory watercolor as part of her op-art. Today's visual scientists study the WCE for its filling-in properties and strong figure-ground segregation. This review emphasizes the superior strength of the WCE for grouping and figure-ground organization vis-à-vis the classical Gestalt factors of Max Wertheimer (1923), thereby inspiring a notion of form from induced color. It also demonstrates that a thin chromatic line, flanking the inside of a black Mondrian-type pattern, induces the WCE across a large white surface area. Phenomenological, psychophysical, and neurophysiological approaches are reviewed.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Humans , Color Perception/physiology , History, 20th Century , Optical Illusions/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , History, 17th Century , History, 19th Century , History, 18th Century
6.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 78(2): 88-99, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38722576

ABSTRACT

Localisation of simple stimuli such as angle vertices may contribute to a plethora of illusory effects. We focus on the Müller-Lyer illusion in an attempt to measure and characterise a more elementary effect that may contribute to the magnitude of said illusion. Perceived location error of angle vertices (a single set of Müller-Lyer fins) and arcs in a 2D plane was measured with the aim to provide clarification of ambiguous results from studies of angle localisation and expand the results to other types of stimuli. In three experiments, we utilised the method of constant stimuli in order to determine perceived locations of angle vertices (Experiments 1 and 2) as well as circular and elliptical arcs (Experiment 3). The results show significant distortions of perceived compared to objective vertex locations (all effect sizes d > 1.01, p < .001). Experiment 2 revealed strong effects of angle size and fin length on localisation error. Mislocalization was larger for more acute angles and longer angle fins (both ηp² = .43, p < .001). In Experiment 3, localisation errors were larger for longer arcs (ηp² = .19, p = .001) irrespective of shape (circular or elliptical). We discuss the effect in the context of modern trends in research of the Müller-Lyer illusion as well as the widely popular centroid theory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Optical Illusions , Space Perception , Humans , Adult , Female , Male , Optical Illusions/physiology , Young Adult , Space Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Form Perception/physiology
7.
Vision Res ; 219: 108393, 2024 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38579405

ABSTRACT

Recent studies have revealed that pupillary response changes depend on perceptual factors such as subjective brightness caused by optical illusions and luminance. However, the manner in which the perceptual factor that is derived from the glossiness perception of object surfaces affects the pupillary response remains unclear. We investigated the relationship between the glossiness perception and pupillary response through a glossiness rating experiment that included recording the pupil diameter. We prepared general object images (original) and randomized images (shuffled) that comprised the same images with randomized small square regions as stimuli. The image features were controlled by matching the luminance histogram. The observers were asked to rate the perceived glossiness of the stimuli presented for 3,000 ms and the changes in their pupil diameters were recorded. Images with higher glossiness ratings constricted the pupil size more than those with lower glossiness ratings at the peak constriction of the pupillary responses during the stimulus duration. The linear mixed-effects model demonstrated that the glossiness rating, image category (original/shuffled), variance of the luminance histogram, and stimulus area were most effective in predicting the pupillary responses. These results suggest that the illusory brightness obtained by the image regions of high-glossiness objects, such as specular highlights, induce pupil constriction.


Subject(s)
Photic Stimulation , Pupil , Humans , Pupil/physiology , Male , Female , Photic Stimulation/methods , Young Adult , Adult , Visual Perception/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology
8.
Psychol Sci ; 35(5): 504-516, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38564652

ABSTRACT

Motion silencing is a striking and unexplained visual illusion wherein changes that are otherwise salient become difficult to perceive when the changing elements also move. We develop a new method for quantifying illusion strength (Experiments 1a and 1b), and we demonstrate a privileged role for rotational motion on illusion strength compared with highly controlled stimuli that lack rotation (Experiments 2a to 3b). These contrasts make it difficult to explain the illusion in terms of lower-level detection limits. Instead, we explain the illusion as a failure to attribute changes to locations. Rotation exacerbates the illusion because its perception relies upon structured object representations. This aggravates the difficulty of attributing changes by demanding that locations are referenced relative to both an object-internal frame and an external frame. Two final experiments (4a and 4b) add support to this account by employing a synchronously rotating external frame of reference that diminishes otherwise strong motion silencing. All participants were Johns Hopkins University undergraduates.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Male , Young Adult , Optical Illusions/physiology , Rotation
9.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 3141, 2024 Apr 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38653975

ABSTRACT

Brightness illusions are a powerful tool in studying vision, yet their neural correlates are poorly understood. Based on a human paradigm, we presented illusory drifting gratings to mice. Primary visual cortex (V1) neurons responded to illusory gratings, matching their direction selectivity for real gratings, and they tracked the spatial phase offset between illusory and real gratings. Illusion responses were delayed compared to real gratings, in line with the theory that processing illusions requires feedback from higher visual areas (HVAs). We provide support for this theory by showing a reduced V1 response to illusions, but not real gratings, following HVAs optogenetic inhibition. Finally, we used the pupil response (PR) as an indirect perceptual report and showed that the mouse PR matches the human PR to perceived luminance changes. Our findings resolve debates over whether V1 neurons are involved in processing illusions and highlight the involvement of feedback from HVAs.


Subject(s)
Neurons , Optogenetics , Photic Stimulation , Primary Visual Cortex , Animals , Neurons/physiology , Primary Visual Cortex/physiology , Mice , Male , Humans , Female , Visual Perception/physiology , Illusions/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Pupil/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Cortex/cytology
10.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(6): 605-625, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38573695

ABSTRACT

Object-based warping is a visual illusion in which dots appear farther apart from each other when superimposed on an object. Previous research found that the illusion's strength varies with the perceived objecthood of the display. We tested whether objecthood alone determines the strength of the visual illusion or if low-level factors separable from objecthood also play a role. In Experiments 1-2, we varied low-level features to assess their impact on the warping illusion. We found that the warping illusion is equally strong for a variety of shapes but varies with the elements by which shape is defined. Shapes composed of continuous edges produced larger warping effects than shapes defined by disconnected elements. In Experiment 3, we varied a display's objecthood while holding low-level features constant. Displays with matched low-level features produced warping effects of the same size even when the perceived unity of the elements in the display varied. In Experiments 4-6, we tested whether displays with low-level features predicted to be important in spatial warping produced the visual illusion even when the display weakly configured into a single object. Results showed that the presence of low-level features like contour solidity and convexity determined warping effect sizes over and above what could be accounted for by the display's perceived objecthood. Our findings challenge the view that the spatial warping illusion is solely object-based. Other factors like the solidity of contours and contours' position relative to reference dots appear to play separate and important roles in determining warping effect sizes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Optical Illusions , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Adult , Young Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Male , Female , Space Perception/physiology
11.
BMC Psychol ; 12(1): 108, 2024 Mar 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38429795

ABSTRACT

Humans are typically inept at evaluating their abilities and predispositions. People dismiss such a lack of metacognitive insight into their capacities while even enhancing (albeit illusorily) self-evaluation such that they should have more desirable traits than an average peer. This superiority illusion helps maintain a healthy mental state. However, the scope and range of its influence on broader human behavior, especially perceptual tasks, remain elusive. As belief shapes the way people perceive and recognize, the illusory self-superiority belief potentially regulates our perceptual and metacognitive performance. In this study, we used hierarchical Bayesian estimation and machine learning of signal detection theoretic measures to understand how the superiority illusion influences visual perception and metacognition for the Ponzo illusion. Our results demonstrated that the superiority illusion correlated with the Ponzo illusion magnitude and metacognitive performance. Next, we combined principal component analysis and cross-validated regularized regression (relaxed elastic net) to identify which superiority components contributed to the correlations. We revealed that the "extraversion" superiority dimension tapped into the Ponzo illusion magnitude and metacognitive ability. In contrast, the "honesty-humility" and "neuroticism" dimensions only predicted Ponzo illusion magnitude and metacognitive ability, respectively. These results suggest common and distinct influences of superiority features on perceptual sensitivity and metacognition. Our findings contribute to the accumulating body of evidence indicating that the leverage of superiority illusion is far-reaching, even to visual perception.


Subject(s)
Metacognition , Optical Illusions , Humans , Optical Illusions/physiology , Bayes Theorem , Visual Perception , Diagnostic Self Evaluation
12.
Exp Psychol ; 70(4): 249-256, 2023 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38105748

ABSTRACT

An object appears to move at higher speed than another equally fast object when brief nonspatial tones coincide with its changes in motion direction. We refer to this phenomenon as the beep-speed illusion (Meyerhoff et al., 2022, Cognition, 219, 104978). The origin of this illusion is unclear; however, attentional explanations and potential biases in the response behavior appear to be plausible candidates. In this report, we test a simple bias explanation that emerges from the way the dependent variable is assessed. As the participants have to indicate the faster of the two objects, participants possibly always indicate the audio-visually synchronized object in situations of perceptual uncertainty. Such a response behavior potentially could explain the observed shift in perceived speed. We therefore probed the magnitude of the beep-speed illusion when the participants indicated either the object that appeared to move faster or the object that appeared to move slower. If a simple selection bias would explain the beep-speed illusion, the response pattern should be inverted with the instruction to indicate the slower object. However, contrary to this bias hypothesis, illusion emerged indistinguishably under both instructions. Therefore, simple selection biases cannot explain the beep-speed illusion.


Subject(s)
Illusions , Motion Perception , Optical Illusions , Humans , Optical Illusions/physiology , Selection Bias , Motion Perception/physiology , Attention , Cognition
13.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 85(4): 1304-1316, 2023 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37002462

ABSTRACT

Partial replications of experiments reported by Cai et al. (Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(4), 1217-1226, 2017) on the so-called Horizontal-vertical illusion confirmed that dissecting L-figures into two separate lines yields greater overestimation of (near-)verticals than do intact Ls. However, contrary to Cai et al.'s findings, which had been obtained with a staircase procedure, with the method of constant stimuli, the amount of illusion was much smaller. This divergence is explained by the self-reinforcing nature of adjustment procedures. Another finding, already reported by Cormack and Cormack (Perception & Psychophysics, 16(2), 208-212, 1974), that obtuse angles between an L's lines yield greater bias than acute angles, was also replicated in one experiment but tended to be reversed in another. Mixing dissected, upright and top-down inverted Ls and laterally oriented Ts, both with tilted lines, within one experiment confirmed that the bias for Ts is opposite to the one for Ls: For Ts, the effect of (virtual) bisection dominates, yielding an overestimation of the length of the undivided line, whereas for Ls, the horizontal-vertical anisotropy dominates, yielding an overestimation of the length of the vertical line. The differential gap effects can possibly be explained by interactions within the neural substrate between orientation-sensitive and end-inhibited neurons, and the method effects by perceptual learning.


Subject(s)
Optical Illusions , Humans , Optical Illusions/physiology , Orientation , Psychophysics , Discrimination Learning , Learning
14.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(10): 6345-6353, 2023 05 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36562991

ABSTRACT

Converging evidence has found that the perceived visual size illusions are heritable, raising the possibility that visual size illusions might be predicted by intrinsic brain activity without external stimuli. Here we measured resting-state brain activity and 2 classic visual size illusions (i.e. the Ebbinghaus and the Ponzo illusions) in succession, and conducted spectral dynamic causal modeling analysis among relevant cortical regions. Results revealed that forward connection from right V1 to superior parietal lobule (SPL) was predictive of the Ebbinghaus illusion, and self-connection in the right SPL predicted the Ponzo illusion. Moreover, disruption of intrinsic activity in the right SPL by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) temporally increased the Ebbinghaus rather than the Ponzo illusion. These findings provide a better mechanistic understanding of visual size illusions by showing the causal and distinct contributions of right parietal cortex to them, and suggest that spontaneous fluctuations in intrinsic brain activity are relevant to individual difference in behavior.


Subject(s)
Illusions , Optical Illusions , Humans , Optical Illusions/physiology , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , Parietal Lobe , Human Rights , Visual Perception
15.
Vision Res ; 202: 108143, 2023 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36347085

ABSTRACT

One of the original Ponzo illusion figures, which consists of two converging lines between which two parallel lines of similar length have been inserted orthogonal to the figure's axis of mirror symmetry, was itself mirror-reflected so that the overall shape of the figure became "< >" or "> <", and one line at a time was inserted into each half. The usual illusion - the overestimation of the length of a line that is nearer to a vertex than a farther-away comparison line - occurred. Experiments 1 and 2 used different distances of target and comparison lines to the vertices, but identical distances of these lines from the converging lines, and so, as a tandem, deconfounded the two variables. Experiments 3 and 4 changed the symmetries of the modified Ponzo figure by reducing opposing half-angles of the converging lines or by tilting target and comparison lines concordantly or discordantly. The first measure, which created unequal distances of the endpoints of the target and comparison lines from the converging lines, hardly affected the amount of illusion. The second measure often attenuated the illusion - equally so for concordant and discordant tilts - suggesting that global and local symmetries of the stimuli, and their accordance, were less important than the vertical versus oblique orientation of target and comparison lines. Descriptively, the main cause of the Ponzo illusion seems to be the size of the gap between target and converging lines. The neural substrate of the effect may be interactions between orientation-sensitive and end-inhibited neurons.


Subject(s)
Illusions , Optical Illusions , Humans , Optical Illusions/physiology , Neurons
16.
Perception ; 51(7): 496-504, 2022 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35570743

ABSTRACT

In order to investigate interrelations between the Oppel-Kundt- and the T-illusion, T-type figures, comprised of one dotted and one empty line (demarcated by its endpoints), separated by a gap of variable size, and rotated to oblique orientations, were judged with regard to the lengths of the two extents. The T-illusion (overestimation of the length of the undivided line) was greater for a T with a dotted undivided line and a small gap. When the divided line was dotted, the illusion vanished at a small gap and reversed at a larger one. Findings are interpreted to mirror activities of a neural T-schema as well as orientation- and density-sensitive neurons.


Subject(s)
Illusions , Optical Illusions , Humans , Illusions/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology
17.
J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn ; 48(2): 123-134, 2022 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35533105

ABSTRACT

A Müller-Lyer figure consists only of a line and arrowheads located at both ends of the line. Many comparative studies have reported that animals perceive Müller-Lyer illusion as humans, but few have used appropriate experimental designs to verify whether animal subjects actually respond to line length alone. The present study investigated whether budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) can perceive the Müller-Lyer illusion by using a method that addresses this problem. Four budgerigars were trained to select a long or short line (counterbalanced across subjects) from two horizontal lines. Next, the same task was conducted using two lines, one of which was situated between arrowheads pointing either right (>>) or left (<<). In the final training phase, the arrowheads were replaced with those pointing inward (><) or outward (<>). The performance of each subject toward each stimulus set of these trainings suggested that they did not determine the length of the line by including the arrowheads. In the test phase, response tendencies to the four figures were compared. Results suggested that budgerigars perceive the Müller-Lyer illusion in the same direction as humans; however, its magnitude is larger than that of humans. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Melopsittacus , Optical Illusions , Animals , Humans , Optical Illusions/physiology
18.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 17(9): e1009344, 2021 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34478441

ABSTRACT

We show how anomalous time reversal of stimuli and their associated responses can exist in very small connectionist models. These networks are built from dynamical toy model neurons which adhere to a minimal set of biologically plausible properties. The appearance of a "ghost" response, temporally and spatially located in between responses caused by actual stimuli, as in the phi phenomenon, is demonstrated in a similar small network, where it is caused by priming and long-distance feedforward paths. We then demonstrate that the color phi phenomenon can be present in an echo state network, a recurrent neural network, without explicitly training for the presence of the effect, such that it emerges as an artifact of the dynamical processing. Our results suggest that the color phi phenomenon might simply be a feature of the inherent dynamical and nonlinear sensory processing in the brain and in and of itself is not related to consciousness.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Models, Neurological , Optical Illusions/physiology , Action Potentials/physiology , Computational Biology , Computer Simulation , Consciousness/physiology , Humans , Illusions/physiology , Illusions/psychology , Models, Psychological , Nerve Net/physiology , Neural Networks, Computer , Neurons/physiology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Photic Stimulation
19.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 494, 2021 01 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33436801

ABSTRACT

There is growing interest in how human observers perceive social scenes containing multiple people. Interpersonal distance is a critical feature when appraising these scenes; proxemic cues are used by observers to infer whether two people are interacting, the nature of their relationship, and the valence of their current interaction. Presently, however, remarkably little is known about how interpersonal distance is encoded within the human visual system. Here we show that the perception of interpersonal distance is distorted by the Müller-Lyer illusion. Participants perceived the distance between two target points to be compressed or expanded depending on whether face pairs were positioned inside or outside the to-be-judged interval. This illusory bias was found to be unaffected by manipulations of face direction. These findings aid our understanding of how human observers perceive interpersonal distance and may inform theoretical accounts of the Müller-Lyer illusion.


Subject(s)
Distance Perception/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Size Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
20.
J Hum Genet ; 66(3): 261-271, 2021 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32939015

ABSTRACT

The Ebbinghaus illusion (EI) is an optical illusion of relative size perception that reflects the contextual integration ability in the visual modality. The current study investigated the genetic basis of two subtypes of EI, EI overestimation, and EI underestimation in humans, using quantitative genomic analyses. A total of 2825 Chinese adults were tested on their magnitudes of EI overestimation and underestimation using the method of adjustment, a standard psychophysical protocol. Heritability estimation based on common single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) revealed a moderate heritability (34.3%) of EI overestimation but a nonsignificant heritability of EI underestimation. A meta-analysis of two phases (phase 1: n = 1986, phase 2: n = 839) of genome-wide association study (GWAS) discovered 1969 and 58 SNPs reaching genome-wide significance for EI overestimation and EI underestimation, respectively. Among these SNPs, 55 linkage-disequilibrium-independent SNPs were associated with EI overestimation in phase 1 with genome-wide significance and their associations could be confirmed in phase 2 cohort. Gene-based analyses found seven genes to be associated with EI overestimation at the genome-wide level, two from meta-analysis, and five from classical two-stage analysis. Overall, this study provided consistent evidence for a substantial genetic basis of the Ebbinghaus illusion.


Subject(s)
Genome-Wide Association Study , Optical Illusions/physiology , Size Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Asian People/genetics , Ethnicity/genetics , Female , Genotype , Humans , Individuality , Linkage Disequilibrium , Male , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide , Visual Cortex/anatomy & histology , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...