Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 11.821
Filter
1.
J Vis ; 24(7): 1, 2024 Jul 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38953861

ABSTRACT

Applications for eye-tracking-particularly in the clinic-are limited by a reliance on dedicated hardware. Here we compare eye-tracking implemented on an Apple iPad Pro 11" (third generation)-using the device's infrared head-tracking and front-facing camera-with a Tobii 4c infrared eye-tracker. We estimated gaze location using both systems while 28 observers performed a variety of tasks. For estimating fixation, gaze position estimates from the iPad were less accurate and precise than the Tobii (mean absolute error of 3.2° ± 2.0° compared with 0.75° ± 0.43°), but fixation stability estimates were correlated across devices (r = 0.44, p < 0.05). For tasks eliciting saccades >1.5°, estimated saccade counts (r = 0.4-0.73, all p < 0.05) were moderately correlated across devices. For tasks eliciting saccades >8° we observed moderate correlations in estimated saccade speed and amplitude (r = 0.4-0.53, all p < 0.05). We did, however, note considerable variation in the vertical component of estimated smooth pursuit speed from the iPad and a catastrophic failure of tracking on the iPad in 5% to 20% of observers (depending on the test). Our findings sound a note of caution to researchers seeking to use iPads for eye-tracking and emphasize the need to properly examine their eye-tracking data to remove artifacts and outliers.


Subject(s)
Eye-Tracking Technology , Fixation, Ocular , Saccades , Humans , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Male , Adult , Female , Young Adult , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Computers, Handheld , Eye Movements/physiology
2.
PLoS One ; 19(7): e0290142, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38959207

ABSTRACT

AIM: This preliminary study investigated the differences in event-related potential and reaction time under two groups (athletes vs. non-athletes). MATERIAL AND METHODS: The P300 was analyzed for Fz, Cz, and Pz electrodes in thirty-one healthy volunteers divided into two groups (volleyball athletes and non-athletes). In addition, the participants performed a saccadic eye movement task to measure reaction time. RESULTS: The EEG analysis showed that the athletes, in comparison to the no-athletes, have differences in the P300 in the frontal area (p = 0.021). In relation to reaction time, the results show lower reaction time for athletes (p = 0.001). CONCLUSIONS: The volleyball athletes may present a greater allocation of attention during the execution of the inhibition task, since they have a lower reaction time for responses when compared to non-athletes.


Subject(s)
Athletes , Electroencephalography , Reaction Time , Saccades , Volleyball , Humans , Reaction Time/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Volleyball/physiology , Male , Female , Young Adult , Adult , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Event-Related Potentials, P300/physiology , Attention/physiology
3.
BMC Ophthalmol ; 24(1): 278, 2024 Jul 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38982388

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To investigate the characteristics of eye movement in children with anisometropic amblyopia, and to compare those characteristics with eye movement in a control group. METHODS: 31 children in the anisometropic amblyopia group (31 amblyopic eyes in group A, 31 contralateral eyes in group B) and 24 children in the control group (48 eyes in group C). Group A was subdivided into groups Aa (severe amblyopia) and Ab (mild-moderate amblyopia). The overall age range was 6-12 years (mean, 7.83 ± 1.79 years). All children underwent ophthalmic examinations; eye movement parameters including saccade latency and amplitude were evaluated using an Eyelink1000 eye tracker. Data Viewer and MATLAB software were used for data analysis. RESULTS: Mean and maximum saccade latencies, as well as mean and maximum saccade amplitudes, were significantly greater in group A than in groups B and C before and after treatment (P < 0.05). Mean and maximum saccade latencies were significantly different among groups Aa, Ab, and C (P < 0.05). Pupil trajectories in two detection modes suggested that binocular fixation was better than monocular fixation. CONCLUSIONS: Eye movement parameters significantly differed between contralateral normal eyes and control eyes. Clinical evaluation of children with anisometropic amblyopia should not focus only on static visual acuity, but also on the assessment of eye movement.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia , Vision, Binocular , Visual Acuity , Humans , Amblyopia/physiopathology , Child , Male , Female , Visual Acuity/physiology , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Anisometropia/physiopathology , Anisometropia/complications , Fixation, Ocular/physiology
4.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(8): 13, 2024 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38975944

ABSTRACT

Purpose: This study aims at linking subtle changes of fixational eye movements (FEM) in controls and in patients with foveal drusen using adaptive optics retinal imaging in order to find anatomo-functional markers for pre-symptomatic age-related macular degeneration (AMD). Methods: We recruited 7 young controls, 4 older controls, and 16 patients with presymptomatic AMD with foveal drusen from the Silversight Cohort. A high-speed research-grade adaptive optics flood illumination ophthalmoscope (AO-FIO) was used for monocular retinal tracking of fixational eye movements. The system allows for sub-arcminute resolution, and high-speed and distortion-free imaging of the foveal area. Foveal drusen position and size were documented using gaze-dependent imaging on a clinical-grade AO-FIO. Results: FEM were measured with high precision (RMS-S2S = 0.0015 degrees on human eyes) and small foveal drusen (median diameter = 60 µm) were detected with high contrast imaging. Microsaccade amplitude, drift diffusion coefficient, and ISOline area (ISOA) were significantly larger for patients with foveal drusen compared with controls. Among the drusen participants, microsaccade amplitude was correlated to drusen eccentricity from the center of the fovea. Conclusions: A novel high-speed high-precision retinal tracking technique allowed for the characterization of FEM at the microscopic level. Foveal drusen altered fixation stability, resulting in compensatory FEM changes. Particularly, drusen at the foveolar level seemed to have a stronger impact on microsaccade amplitudes and ISOA. The unexpected anatomo-functional link between small foveal drusen and fixation stability opens up a new perspective of detecting oculomotor signatures of eye diseases at the presymptomatic stage.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular , Fovea Centralis , Macular Degeneration , Retinal Drusen , Humans , Female , Retinal Drusen/physiopathology , Retinal Drusen/diagnosis , Male , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Fovea Centralis/diagnostic imaging , Fovea Centralis/physiopathology , Fovea Centralis/pathology , Aged , Middle Aged , Macular Degeneration/physiopathology , Macular Degeneration/diagnosis , Adult , Tomography, Optical Coherence/methods , Ophthalmoscopy/methods , Visual Acuity/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Prodromal Symptoms
5.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(8): 24, 2024 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39012638

ABSTRACT

Purpose: Within the healthy population there is a large variation in the ability to perform smooth pursuit eye movements. Our purpose was to investigate the genetic and physiological bases for this variation. Methods: We carried out a whole-genome association study, recording smooth pursuit movements for 1040 healthy volunteers by infrared oculography. The primary phenotypic measure was root mean square error (RMSE) of eye position relative to target position. Secondary measures were pursuit gain, frequency of catch-up saccades, and frequency of anticipatory saccades. Ten percent of participants, chosen randomly, were tested twice, giving estimates of test-retest reliability. Results: No significant association was found with three genes previously identified as candidate genes for variation in smooth pursuit: DRD3, COMT, NRG1. A strong association (P = 3.55 × 10-11) was found between RMSE and chromosomal region 1q42.2. The most strongly associated marker (rs701232) lies in an intron of KCNK1, which encodes a two-pore-domain potassium ion channel TWIK-1 (or K2P1) that affects cell excitability. Each additional copy of the A allele decreased RMSE by 0.29 standard deviation. When a psychophysical test of visually perceived motion was used as a covariate in the regression analysis, the association with rs701232 did not weaken (P = 5.38 × 10-12). Conclusions: Variation in the sequence or the expression of the pH-dependent ion channel TWIK-1 is a likely source of variance in smooth pursuit. The variance associated with TWIK-1 appears not to arise from sensory mechanisms, because the use of a perceptual covariate left the association intact.


Subject(s)
Genome-Wide Association Study , Potassium Channels, Tandem Pore Domain , Pursuit, Smooth , Humans , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Male , Female , Adult , Potassium Channels, Tandem Pore Domain/genetics , Young Adult , Middle Aged , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide , Hydrogen-Ion Concentration , Healthy Volunteers , Genotype , Saccades/physiology
6.
Bol Med Hosp Infant Mex ; 81(3): 162-169, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38941647

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The cochlear implant (CI) is effective for rehabilitating patients with severe to profound sensorineural hearing loss. However, its placement and use have been associated with various complications, such as those affecting the vestibular system. The objective of this study was to compare vestibular function using the video head impulse test (vHIT) in pediatric patients before and after CI placement. METHODS: A descriptive and retrospective study was conducted. The outcomes of 11 pediatric patients of both sexes with a history of profound hearing loss were evaluated. The results of vestibular-ocular reflex (VOR) gain, saccades, asymmetry, Pérez Rey (PR) index, and VOR/saccade ratio for both ears obtained by the vHIT test before and after CI placement were compared. RESULTS: Of the 11 patients evaluated, the VOR gain showed that 81.8% had normal function, 18.2% had hypofunction, and no patients had hyperfunction before implantation. No statistically significant differences were found when compared with post-implant off and post-implant on conditions (p > 0.05). The extracted variables, asymmetry, PR index, and the VOR/saccades ratio also showed no statistically significant differences between the pre- and post-implant conditions, whether off or on. CONCLUSIONS: The vestibular function of pediatric patients did not show significant changes before and after CI placement. The vHIT test is a valuable tool for assessing vestibular function and could be considered a criterion for surgical and rehabilitation decisions in patients undergoing CI placement.


INTRODUCCIÓN: El implante coclear es un dispositivo eficaz para la rehabilitación de pacientes con hipoacusia neurosensorial severa a profunda. Sin embargo, su colocación y uso se ha asociado a diversas complicaciones, entre ellas a nivel del sistema vestibular. El objetivo del presente estudio fue comparar la función vestibular mediante la prueba de videoimpulso cefálico (vHIT) de pacientes pediátricos antes y después de la colocación del implante coclear. MÉTODOS: Se llevó a cabo un estudio descriptivo y retrospectivo. Se evaluaron los resultados de 11 pacientes pediátricos de ambos sexos con antecedente de hipoacusia profunda. Se compararon los resultados de ganancia del VOR, sacadas, asimetría, índice PR así como la relación VOR/sacadas para ambos oídos obtenidos mediante la prueba vHIT antes y después de la colocación del implante coclear. RESULTADOS: De los 11 pacientes evaluados, la ganancia del VOR mostró que el 81.8% tenía normofunción, 18.2% hipofunción y ningún paciente hiperfunción antes del implante. Al compararlo con la ganancia post implante apagado y post implante encendido no se encontraron diferencias estadísticamente significativas (p > 0.05). Las variables sacadas, asimetría, índice PR así como la relación VOR/sacadas tampoco mostraron diferencias estadísticamente significativas entre las condiciones pre y pos implante ya sea apagado o encendido. CONCLUSIONES: La función vestibular de pacientes pediátricos no mostró cambios significativos previo y posterior a la colocación del implante coclear. La prueba vHIT es una herramienta útil que permite evaluar la función vestibular y que podría considerarse como criterio para tomar decisiones quirúrgicas en pacientes que se encuentran en protocolo para implante coclear.


Subject(s)
Cochlear Implantation , Cochlear Implants , Head Impulse Test , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular , Humans , Female , Male , Head Impulse Test/methods , Retrospective Studies , Child , Child, Preschool , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/diagnosis , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/physiopathology , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/rehabilitation , Video Recording , Saccades/physiology , Adolescent , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiopathology
7.
J Neurophysiol ; 132(1): 147-161, 2024 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38836297

ABSTRACT

People usually reach for objects to place them in some position and orientation, but the placement component of this sequence is often ignored. For example, reaches are influenced by gaze position, visual feedback, and memory delays, but their influence on object placement is unclear. Here, we tested these factors in a task where participants placed and oriented a trapezoidal block against two-dimensional (2-D) visual templates displayed on a frontally located computer screen. In experiment 1, participants matched the block to three possible orientations: 0° (horizontal), +45° and -45°, with gaze fixated 10° to the left/right. The hand and template either remained illuminated (closed-loop), or visual feedback was removed (open-loop). Here, hand location consistently overshot the template relative to gaze, especially in the open-loop task; likewise, orientation was influenced by gaze position (depending on template orientation and visual feedback). In experiment 2, a memory delay was added, and participants sometimes performed saccades (toward, away from, or across the template). In this task, the influence of gaze on orientation vanished, but location errors were influenced by both template orientation and final gaze position. Contrary to our expectations, the previous saccade metrics also impacted placement overshoot. Overall, hand orientation was influenced by template orientation in a nonlinear fashion. These results demonstrate interactions between gaze and orientation signals in the planning and execution of hand placement and suggest different neural mechanisms for closed-loop, open-loop, and memory delay placement.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Eye-hand coordination studies usually focus on object acquisition, but placement is equally important. We investigated how gaze position influences object placement toward a 2-D template with different levels of visual feedback. Like reach, placement overestimated goal location relative to gaze and was influenced by previous saccade metrics. Gaze also modulated hand orientation, depending on template orientation and level of visual feedback. Gaze influence was feedback-dependent, with location errors having no significant effect after a memory delay.


Subject(s)
Feedback, Sensory , Fixation, Ocular , Hand , Memory , Psychomotor Performance , Humans , Male , Female , Hand/physiology , Adult , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Biomechanical Phenomena/physiology , Feedback, Sensory/physiology , Memory/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Young Adult , Visual Perception/physiology , Saccades/physiology
8.
J Integr Neurosci ; 23(6): 108, 2024 May 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38940093

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: In this study, we used electroencephalogram (EEG) to investigate the activity pattern of the cerebral cortex related to visual pursuit and saccade strategies to predict the arrival position of a visual target. In addition, we clarified the differences in the EEG of those who could predict the arrival position well using the saccade strategy compared to those who were not proficient. METHODS: Sixteen participants performed two tasks: the "Pursuit Strategy Task (PST)" and the "Saccade Strategy Task (SST)" while undergoing EEG. For the PST, the participants were instructed to follow the target with their eyes throughout its trajectory and indicate when it reached the final point. For the SST, the participants were instructed to shift their gaze to the end point of arrival once they had predicted it. RESULTS: Low beta EEG activity at the Oz, Cz, and CP2 electrodes was significantly higher during the SST than during the PST. In addition, low beta EEG activity at P7 electrode was significantly higher in the group showing a small position error (PE) than in the group showing a large PE at response. CONCLUSIONS: EEG activity at the Oz, Cz, and CP2 electrodes during the SST may reflect visuospatial attention to the moving target, the tracking of moving targets, and the focus on the final destination position. In addition, EEG activity at P7 electrode may more accurately detect the speed and direction of the moving target by the small PE group at response.


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography , Saccades , Humans , Saccades/physiology , Male , Female , Young Adult , Adult , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Attention/physiology , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology
9.
J Vis ; 24(6): 16, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38913016

ABSTRACT

Humans saccade to faces in their periphery faster than to other types of objects. Previous research has highlighted the potential importance of the upper face region in this phenomenon, but it remains unclear whether this is driven by the eye region. Similarly, it remains unclear whether such rapid saccades are exclusive to faces or generalize to other semantically salient stimuli. Furthermore, it is unknown whether individuals differ in their face-specific saccadic reaction times and, if so, whether such differences could be linked to differences in face fixations during free viewing. To explore these open questions, we invited 77 participants to perform a saccadic choice task in which we contrasted faces as well as other salient objects, particularly isolated face features and text, with cars. Additionally, participants freely viewed 700 images of complex natural scenes in a separate session, which allowed us to determine the individual proportion of first fixations falling on faces. For the saccadic choice task, we found advantages for all categories of interest over cars. However, this effect was most pronounced for images of full faces. Full faces also elicited faster saccades compared with eyes, showing that isolated eye regions are not sufficient to elicit face-like responses. Additionally, we found consistent individual differences in saccadic reaction times toward faces that weakly correlated with face salience during free viewing. Our results suggest a link between semantic salience and rapid detection, but underscore the unique status of faces. Further research is needed to resolve the mechanisms underlying rapid face saccades.


Subject(s)
Facial Recognition , Individuality , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time , Saccades , Humans , Saccades/physiology , Male , Female , Reaction Time/physiology , Adult , Young Adult , Facial Recognition/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Adolescent
10.
Otol Neurotol ; 45(7): e509-e516, 2024 Aug 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38918071

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The purpose of this study was to evaluate the value of asymmetry values, gain, and pathological saccades of the video head impulse test (vHIT) in sudden sensorineural hearing loss (SSNHL). STUDY DESIGN: Retrospective study. SETTING: Tertiary referral center. PATIENTS: A total of 226 individuals diagnosed with unilateral definite SSNHL were hospitalized. The assessment included a comprehensive evaluation of medical history, pure-tone test, acoustic impedance, positional test, video nystagmography (VNG), vHIT, vestibular evoked myogenic potentials (VEMPs) and magnetic resonance. INTERVENTIONS: vHIT, VNG, cVEMP, oVEMP. Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS version 22.0 for Windows. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: The asymmetry values, gain, and pathological saccades of the vHIT. RESULTS: The abnormal gain of vHIT in anterior, horizontal, and posterior canal in SSNHL patients with vertigo were revealed in 20 of 112 (17.9%), 24 of 112 (21.4%), and 60 of 112 (53.6%), respectively. The vHIT pathological saccades (overt + covert) of anterior, horizontal, and posterior canal in SSNHL patients with vertigo were observed in 5 of 112 (4.6%), 52 of 112 (46.4%), and 58 of 112 (51.8%), respectively. Multivariate analysis indicated that the prognosis of patients with vertigo was correlated with vHIT gain of posterior canal, pathological saccade in horizontal canal, asymmetric ratio of horizontal canal gain, asymmetric ratio of posterior canal gain, Canal paresis (%) on caloric test and spontaneous nystagmus. CONCLUSION: In the vHIT of patients with SSNHL with vertigo, the posterior canal is most easily affected. Reduced gain of posterior canal, pathological saccade of horizontal canal, and larger asymmetric gain of posterior canal and horizontal canal may be negative prognostic factors.


Subject(s)
Head Impulse Test , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural , Hearing Loss, Sudden , Saccades , Vestibular Evoked Myogenic Potentials , Humans , Head Impulse Test/methods , Male , Female , Middle Aged , Adult , Saccades/physiology , Retrospective Studies , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/physiopathology , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/diagnosis , Aged , Hearing Loss, Sudden/physiopathology , Hearing Loss, Sudden/diagnosis , Vestibular Evoked Myogenic Potentials/physiology , Adolescent , Vertigo/physiopathology , Vertigo/diagnosis , Young Adult , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Aged, 80 and over
11.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(27): e2316608121, 2024 Jul 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38941277

ABSTRACT

Coordination of goal-directed behavior depends on the brain's ability to recover the locations of relevant objects in the world. In humans, the visual system encodes the spatial organization of sensory inputs, but neurons in early visual areas map objects according to their retinal positions, rather than where they are in the world. How the brain computes world-referenced spatial information across eye movements has been widely researched and debated. Here, we tested whether shifts of covert attention are sufficiently precise in space and time to track an object's real-world location across eye movements. We found that observers' attentional selectivity is remarkably precise and is barely perturbed by the execution of saccades. Inspired by recent neurophysiological discoveries, we developed an observer model that rapidly estimates the real-world locations of objects and allocates attention within this reference frame. The model recapitulates the human data and provides a parsimonious explanation for previously reported phenomena in which observers allocate attention to task-irrelevant locations across eye movements. Our findings reveal that visual attention operates in real-world coordinates, which can be computed rapidly at the earliest stages of cortical processing.


Subject(s)
Attention , Saccades , Humans , Attention/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Male , Female , Visual Perception/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Models, Neurological , Photic Stimulation/methods
12.
J Vis ; 24(6): 11, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38869372

ABSTRACT

Microsaccades-tiny fixational eye movements-improve discriminability in high-acuity tasks in the foveola. To investigate whether they help compensate for low discriminability at the perifovea, we examined microsaccade characteristics relative to the adult visual performance field, which is characterized by two perceptual asymmetries: horizontal-vertical anisotropy (better discrimination along the horizontal than vertical meridian) and vertical meridian asymmetry (better discrimination along the lower than upper vertical meridian). We investigated whether and to what extent microsaccade directionality varies when stimuli are at isoeccentric locations along the cardinals under conditions of heterogeneous discriminability (Experiment 1) and homogeneous discriminability, equated by adjusting stimulus contrast (Experiment 2). Participants performed a two-alternative forced-choice orientation discrimination task. In both experiments, performance was better on trials without microsaccades between ready signal onset and stimulus offset than on trials with microsaccades. Across the trial sequence, the microsaccade rate and directional pattern were similar across locations. Our results indicate that microsaccades were similar regardless of stimulus discriminability and target location, except during the response period-once the stimuli were no longer present and target location no longer uncertain-when microsaccades were biased toward the target location. Thus, this study reveals that microsaccades do not flexibly adapt as a function of varying discriminability in a basic visual task around the visual field.


Subject(s)
Photic Stimulation , Saccades , Visual Fields , Humans , Saccades/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Male , Adult , Female , Young Adult , Photic Stimulation/methods , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Fovea Centralis/physiology
13.
Elife ; 132024 Jun 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38913073

ABSTRACT

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) presents a range of challenges, including heightened sensory sensitivities. Here, we examine the idea that sensory overload in ASD may be linked to issues with efference copy mechanisms, which predict the sensory outcomes of self-generated actions, such as eye movements. Efference copies play a vital role in maintaining visual and motor stability. Disrupted efference copies hinder precise predictions, leading to increased reliance on actual feedback and potential distortions in perceptions across eye movements. In our first experiment, we tested how well healthy individuals with varying levels of autistic traits updated their mental map after making eye movements. We found that those with more autistic traits had difficulty using information from their eye movements to update the spatial representation of their mental map, resulting in significant errors in object localization. In the second experiment, we looked at how participants perceived an object displacement after making eye movements. Using a trans-saccadic spatial updating task, we found that those with higher autism scores exhibited a greater bias, indicating under-compensation of eye movements and a failure to maintain spatial stability during saccades. Overall, our study underscores efference copy's vital role in visuo-motor stability, aligning with Bayesian theories of autism, potentially informing interventions for improved action-perception integration in autism.


Subject(s)
Autism Spectrum Disorder , Humans , Male , Female , Autism Spectrum Disorder/physiopathology , Autism Spectrum Disorder/psychology , Adult , Young Adult , Eye Movements/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Saccades/physiology , Autistic Disorder/physiopathology , Autistic Disorder/psychology
14.
Adv Exp Med Biol ; 1455: 95-116, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38918348

ABSTRACT

Temporal information processing in the range of a few hundred milliseconds to seconds involves the cerebellum and basal ganglia. In this chapter, we present recent studies on nonhuman primates. In the studies presented in the first half of the chapter, monkeys were trained to make eye movements when a certain amount of time had elapsed since the onset of the visual cue (time production task). The animals had to report time lapses ranging from several hundred milliseconds to a few seconds based on the color of the fixation point. In this task, the saccade latency varied with the time length to be measured and showed stochastic variability from one trial to the other. Trial-to-trial variability under the same conditions correlated well with pupil diameter and the preparatory activity in the deep cerebellar nuclei and the motor thalamus. Inactivation of these brain regions delayed saccades when asked to report subsecond intervals. These results suggest that the internal state, which changes with each trial, may cause fluctuations in cerebellar neuronal activity, thereby producing variations in self-timing. When measuring different time intervals, the preparatory activity in the cerebellum always begins approximately 500 ms before movements, regardless of the length of the time interval being measured. However, the preparatory activity in the striatum persists throughout the mandatory delay period, which can be up to 2 s, with different rate of increasing activity. Furthermore, in the striatum, the visual response and low-frequency oscillatory activity immediately before time measurement were altered by the length of the intended time interval. These results indicate that the state of the network, including the striatum, changes with the intended timing, which lead to different time courses of preparatory activity. Thus, the basal ganglia appear to be responsible for measuring time in the range of several hundred milliseconds to seconds, whereas the cerebellum is responsible for regulating self-timing variability in the subsecond range. The second half of this chapter presents studies related to periodic timing. During eye movements synchronized with alternating targets at regular intervals, different neurons in the cerebellar nuclei exhibit activity related to movement timing, predicted stimulus timing, and the temporal error of synchronization. Among these, the activity associated with target appearance is particularly enhanced during synchronized movements and may represent an internal model of the temporal structure of stimulus sequence. We also considered neural mechanism underlying the perception of periodic timing in the absence of movement. During perception of rhythm, we predict the timing of the next stimulus and focus our attention on that moment. In the missing oddball paradigm, the subjects had to detect the omission of a regularly repeated stimulus. When employed in humans, the results show that the fastest temporal limit for predicting each stimulus timing is about 0.25 s (4 Hz). In monkeys performing this task, neurons in the cerebellar nuclei, striatum, and motor thalamus exhibit periodic activity, with different time courses depending on the brain region. Since electrical stimulation or inactivation of recording sites changes the reaction time to stimulus omission, these neuronal activities must be involved in periodic temporal processing. Future research is needed to elucidate the mechanism of rhythm perception, which appears to be processed by both cortico-cerebellar and cortico-basal ganglia pathways.


Subject(s)
Basal Ganglia , Cerebellum , Time Perception , Animals , Cerebellum/physiology , Basal Ganglia/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Time Factors , Humans
15.
Exp Brain Res ; 242(7): 1797-1806, 2024 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38839617

ABSTRACT

People with multiple sclerosis (PwMS) who report dizziness often have gaze instability due to vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) deficiencies and compensatory saccade (CS) abnormalities. Herein, we aimed to describe and compare the gaze stabilization mechanisms for yaw and pitch head movements in PwMS. Thirty-seven PwMS (27 female, mean ± SD age = 53.4 ± 12.4 years old, median [IQR] Expanded Disability Status Scale Score = 3.5, [1.0]. We analyzed video head impulse test results for VOR gain, CS frequency, CS latency, gaze position error (GPE) at impulse end, and GPE at 400 ms after impulse start. Discrepancies were found for median [IQR] VOR gain in yaw (0.92 [0.14]) versus pitch-up (0.71 [0.44], p < 0.001) and pitch-down (0.81 [0.44], p = 0.014]), CS latency in yaw (258.13 [76.8]) ms versus pitch-up (208.78 [65.97]) ms, p = 0.001] and pitch-down (132.17 [97.56] ms, p = 0.006), GPE at impulse end in yaw (1.15 [1.85] degs versus pitch-up (2.71 [3.9] degs, p < 0.001), and GPE at 400 ms in yaw (-0.25 [0.98] degs) versus pitch-up (1.53 [1.07] degs, p < 0.001) and pitch-down (1.12 [1.82] degs, p = 0.001). Compared with yaw (0.91 [0.75]), CS frequency was similar for pitch-up (1.03 [0.93], p = 0.999) but lower for pitch-down (0.65 [0.64], p = 0.023). GPE at 400 ms was similar for yaw and pitch-down (1.88 [2.76] degs, p = 0.400). We postulate that MS may have preferentially damaged the vertical VOR and saccade pathways in this cohort.


Subject(s)
Multiple Sclerosis , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular , Humans , Female , Male , Middle Aged , Multiple Sclerosis/physiopathology , Multiple Sclerosis/complications , Adult , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology , Aged , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Head Impulse Test/methods
16.
Brain Cogn ; 179: 106182, 2024 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38824809

ABSTRACT

A single bout of exercise improves executive function (EF) and is a benefit - in part -attributed to an exercise-mediated increase in cerebral blood flow enhancing neural efficiency. Limited work has used an event-related protocol to examine postexercise changes in preparatory phase cerebral hemodynamics for an EF task. This is salient given the neural efficiency hypothesis' assertion that improved EF is related to decreased brain activity. Here, event-related transcranial Doppler ultrasound was used to measure pro- (saccade to target) and antisaccades (saccade mirror-symmetrical target) preparatory phase middle cerebral artery velocity (MCAv) prior to and immediately after 15-min of aerobic exercise. Antisaccades produced longer reaction times (RT) and an increased preparatory phase MCAv than prosaccades - a result attributed to greater EF neural activity for antisaccades. Antisaccades selectively produced a postexercise RT reduction (ps < 0.01); however, antisaccade preparatory phase MCAv did not vary from pre- to postexercise (p=0.53) and did not correlate with the antisaccade RT benefit (p = 0.31). Accordingly, results provide no evidence that improved neural efficiency indexed via functional hyperemia is linked to a postexercise EF behavioural benefit. Instead, results support an evolving view that an EF benefit represents the additive interplay between interdependent exercise-mediated neurophysiological changes.


Subject(s)
Cerebrovascular Circulation , Executive Function , Exercise , Saccades , Ultrasonography, Doppler, Transcranial , Humans , Saccades/physiology , Exercise/physiology , Male , Female , Young Adult , Adult , Cerebrovascular Circulation/physiology , Ultrasonography, Doppler, Transcranial/methods , Executive Function/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Hemodynamics/physiology , Inhibition, Psychological , Middle Cerebral Artery/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
17.
PLoS One ; 19(6): e0303596, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38905269

ABSTRACT

Eye-tracking techniques have gained widespread application in various fields including research on the visual system, neurosciences, psychology, and human-computer interaction, with emerging clinical implications. In this preliminary phase of our study, we introduce a pilot test of innovative virtual reality technology designed for tracking head and eye movements among healthy individuals. This tool was developed to assess the presence of mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI), given the frequent association of oculomotor function deficits with such injuries. Alongside eye-tracking, we also integrated fMRI due to the complementary nature of these techniques, offering insights into both neural activation patterns and behavioural responses, thereby providing a comprehensive understanding of oculomotor function. We used fMRI with tasks evaluating oculomotor functions: Smooth Pursuit (SP), Saccades, Anti-Saccades, and Optokinetic Nystagmus (OKN). Prior to the scanning, the testing with a system of VR goggles with integrated eye and head tracking was used where subjects performed the same tasks as those used in fMRI. 31 healthy adult controls (HCs) were tested with the purpose of identifying brain regions associated with these tasks and collecting preliminary norms for later comparison with concussed subjects. HCs' fMRI results showed following peak activation regions: SP-cuneus, superior parietal lobule, paracentral lobule, inferior parietal lobule (IPL), cerebellartonsil (CT); Saccades-middle frontal gyrus (MFG), postcentral gyrus, medial frontal gyrus; Anti-saccades-precuneus, IPL, MFG; OKN-middle temporal gyrus, ACC, postcentral gyrus, MFG, CT. These results demonstrated brain regions associated with the performance on oculomotor tasks in healthy controls and most of the highlighted areas are corresponding with those affected in concussion. This suggests that the involvement of brain areas susceptible to mTBI in implementing oculomotor evaluation, taken together with commonly reported oculomotor difficulties post-concussion, may lead to finding objective biomarkers using eye-tracking tasks.


Subject(s)
Eye-Tracking Technology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Pilot Projects , Adult , Male , Female , Eye Movements/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Young Adult , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiopathology , Brain/physiology , Middle Aged , Nystagmus, Optokinetic/physiology
18.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 12852, 2024 06 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38834578

ABSTRACT

The dorsal pulvinar has been implicated in visuospatial attentional and perceptual confidence processing. Pulvinar lesions in humans and monkeys lead to spatial neglect symptoms, including an overt spatial saccade bias during free choices. However, it remains unclear whether disrupting the dorsal pulvinar during target selection that relies on a perceptual decision leads to a perceptual impairment or a more general spatial orienting and choice deficit. To address this question, we reversibly inactivated the unilateral dorsal pulvinar by injecting GABA-A agonist THIP while two macaque monkeys performed a color discrimination saccade task with varying perceptual difficulty. We used Signal Detection Theory and simulations to dissociate perceptual sensitivity (d-prime) and spatial selection bias (response criterion) effects. We expected a decrease in d-prime if dorsal pulvinar affects perceptual discrimination and a shift in response criterion if dorsal pulvinar is mainly involved in spatial orienting. After the inactivation, we observed response criterion shifts away from contralesional stimuli, especially when two competing stimuli in opposite hemifields were present. Notably, the d-prime and overall accuracy remained largely unaffected. Our results underline the critical contribution of the dorsal pulvinar to spatial orienting and action selection while showing it to be less important for visual perceptual discrimination.


Subject(s)
Pulvinar , Saccades , Animals , Pulvinar/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Male , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Macaca mulatta , Attention/physiology
19.
J Vis ; 24(6): 4, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38842836

ABSTRACT

The interception (or avoidance) of moving objects is a common component of various daily living tasks; however, it remains unclear whether precise alignment of foveal vision with a target is important for motor performance. Furthermore, there has also been little examination of individual differences in visual tracking strategy and the use of anticipatory gaze adjustments. We examined the importance of in-flight tracking and predictive visual behaviors using a virtual reality environment that required participants (n = 41) to intercept tennis balls projected from one of two possible locations. Here, we explored whether different tracking strategies spontaneously arose during the task, and which were most effective. Although indices of closer in-flight tracking (pursuit gain, tracking coherence, tracking lag, and saccades) were predictive of better interception performance, these relationships were rather weak. Anticipatory gaze shifts toward the correct release location of the ball provided no benefit for subsequent interception. Nonetheless, two interceptive strategies were evident: 1) early anticipation of the ball's onset location followed by attempts to closely track the ball in flight (i.e., predictive strategy); or 2) positioning gaze between possible onset locations and then using peripheral vision to locate the moving ball (i.e., a visual pivot strategy). Despite showing much poorer in-flight foveal tracking of the ball, participants adopting a visual pivot strategy performed slightly better in the task. Overall, these results indicate that precise alignment of the fovea with the target may not be critical for interception tasks, but that observers can adopt quite varied visual guidance approaches.


Subject(s)
Individuality , Motion Perception , Humans , Male , Female , Young Adult , Motion Perception/physiology , Adult , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Virtual Reality , Saccades/physiology , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology
20.
J Vis ; 24(6): 8, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38856982

ABSTRACT

When interacting with the environment, humans typically shift their gaze to where information is to be found that is useful for the upcoming action. With increasing age, people become slower both in processing sensory information and in performing their movements. One way to compensate for this slowing down could be to rely more on predictive strategies. To examine whether we could find evidence for this, we asked younger (19-29 years) and older (55-72 years) healthy adults to perform a reaching task wherein they hit a visual target that appeared at one of two possible locations. In separate blocks of trials, the target could appear always at the same location (predictable), mainly at one of the locations (biased), or at either location randomly (unpredictable). As one might expect, saccades toward predictable targets had shorter latencies than those toward less predictable targets, irrespective of age. Older adults took longer to initiate saccades toward the target location than younger adults, even when the likely target location could be deduced. Thus we found no evidence of them relying more on predictive gaze. Moreover, both younger and older participants performed more saccades when the target location was less predictable, but again no age-related differences were found. Thus we found no tendency for older adults to rely more on prediction.


Subject(s)
Aging , Fixation, Ocular , Saccades , Humans , Aged , Middle Aged , Adult , Male , Female , Saccades/physiology , Aging/physiology , Young Adult , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Age Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...