Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 11.524
Filter
1.
PLoS One ; 19(6): e0300664, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38829847

ABSTRACT

Acoustic surveys of bat echolocation calls are an important management tool for determining presence and probable absence of threatened and endangered bat species. In the northeastern United States, software programs such as Bat Call Identification (BCID), Kaleidoscope Pro (KPro), and Sonobat can automatically classify ultrasonic detector sound files, yet the programs' accuracy in correctly classifying calls to species has not been independently assessed. We used 1,500 full-spectrum reference calls with known identities for nine northeastern United States bat species to test the accuracy of these programs using calculations of Positive Predictive Value (PPV), Negative Predictive Value (NPV), Sensitivity (SN), Specificity (SP), Overall Accuracy, and No Information Rate. We found that BCID performed less accurately than other programs, likely because it only operates on zero-crossing data and may be less accurate for recordings converted from full-spectrum to zero-crossing. NPV and SP values were high across all species categories for SonoBat and KPro, indicating these programs' success at avoiding false positives. However, PPV and SN values were relatively low, particularly for individual Myotis species, indicating these programs are prone to false negatives. SonoBat and KPro performed better when distinguishing Myotis species from non-Myotis species. We expect less accuracy from these programs for acoustic recordings collected under normal working conditions, and caution that a bat acoustic expert should verify automatically classified files when making species-specific regulatory or conservation decisions.


Subject(s)
Chiroptera , Echolocation , Chiroptera/physiology , Chiroptera/classification , Animals , Echolocation/physiology , New England , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Software , Species Specificity , Acoustics
2.
PLoS One ; 19(5): e0302933, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38701075

ABSTRACT

Animals in urban areas often encounter novel and potentially stressful conditions. It is important to understand how wildlife cope with anthropogenic disturbance. To investigate this specific adaptation we live-trapped squirrels in two study sites in Warsaw: a forest reserve and an urban park and we estimated stress responses at three levels: long-term and medium-term stress (the level of stress hormones, i.e. cortisol and cortisone concentrations, in hair and feces) and acute reaction to human-induced stress (measured during handling with the aid of the three indices: breath rate, struggle rate, and vocalization). According to GLMM models no difference in the stress hormones level was found between the two populations. The only differences in cortisol concentrations clearly depended on the season, i.e. being higher in autumn and winter comparying to other seasons. There was no influence of sex, or reproductive status on stress hormones. Forest squirrels had significantly higher breath rates, suggesting they were more stressed by handling. There was no difference in the struggle rate between study areas, this index was mostly affected by season (i.e. being highest in winter). First-trapped squirrels vocalized less than during the subsequent trappings. Assumingly, during the first, and more stressful trapping, squirrels used 'freezing' and/or little vocalization, while during next captures they used alarm calls to warn conspecifics. Overall, we showed that the two squirrel populations differed only in terms of their breath rate. This suggests that they did not differ in medium-term and long-term stress in general, but they can differ in acute response to handling. This also suggests that both populations were similarly affected by environmental factors. The lack of clear effects may also be due to population heterogeneity. Thus, in order to assess the effects of anthropogenic stressors a broader range of indicators and diverse analytical methods, including behavioral analyses, should be employed.


Subject(s)
Hydrocortisone , Sciuridae , Stress, Physiological , Animals , Sciuridae/physiology , Hydrocortisone/metabolism , Hydrocortisone/analysis , Stress, Physiological/physiology , Humans , Male , Seasons , Female , Cities , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Urban Population
3.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 10536, 2024 05 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38719897

ABSTRACT

Precisely timed and reliably emitted spikes are hypothesized to serve multiple functions, including improving the accuracy and reproducibility of encoding stimuli, memories, or behaviours across trials. When these spikes occur as a repeating sequence, they can be used to encode and decode a potential time series. Here, we show both analytically and in simulations that the error incurred in approximating a time series with precisely timed and reliably emitted spikes decreases linearly with the number of neurons or spikes used in the decoding. This was verified numerically with synthetically generated patterns of spikes. Further, we found that if spikes were imprecise in their timing, or unreliable in their emission, the error incurred in decoding with these spikes would be sub-linear. However, if the spike precision or spike reliability increased with network size, the error incurred in decoding a time-series with sequences of spikes would maintain a linear decrease with network size. The spike precision had to increase linearly with network size, while the probability of spike failure had to decrease with the square-root of the network size. Finally, we identified a candidate circuit to test this scaling relationship: the repeating sequences of spikes with sub-millisecond precision in area HVC (proper name) of the zebra finch. This scaling relationship can be tested using both neural data and song-spectrogram-based recordings while taking advantage of the natural fluctuation in HVC network size due to neurogenesis.


Subject(s)
Action Potentials , Models, Neurological , Neurons , Animals , Action Potentials/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Reproducibility of Results
4.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 3617, 2024 May 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38714699

ABSTRACT

Sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) are highly social mammals that communicate using sequences of clicks called codas. While a subset of codas have been shown to encode information about caller identity, almost everything else about the sperm whale communication system, including its structure and information-carrying capacity, remains unknown. We show that codas exhibit contextual and combinatorial structure. First, we report previously undescribed features of codas that are sensitive to the conversational context in which they occur, and systematically controlled and imitated across whales. We call these rubato and ornamentation. Second, we show that codas form a combinatorial coding system in which rubato and ornamentation combine with two context-independent features we call rhythm and tempo to produce a large inventory of distinguishable codas. Sperm whale vocalisations are more expressive and structured than previously believed, and built from a repertoire comprising nearly an order of magnitude more distinguishable codas. These results show context-sensitive and combinatorial vocalisation can appear in organisms with divergent evolutionary lineage and vocal apparatus.


Subject(s)
Sperm Whale , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Sperm Whale/physiology , Sperm Whale/anatomy & histology , Male , Female , Sound Spectrography
5.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 155(5): 3037-3050, 2024 May 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38717209

ABSTRACT

The progress of fin whale study is hindered by the debate about whether the two typical type-A and type-B calls (characterized by central source frequencies of 17-20 Hz and 20-30 Hz, respectively) originate from a single fin whale or two individual fin whales. Here, hydroacoustic data is employed to study the type, vocal behavior, and temporal evolution of fin whale calls around the Southern Wake Island from 2010 to 2022. It is identified that (1) type-A and type-B calls come from two individuals based on the large source separation of the two calls through high-precision determination of source location; (2) type-A fin whales exhibit vocal influence on type-B fin whales, where type-B fin whales become paired with type-A calls and vocalize regularly when type-A fin whales appear, and type-A fin whales always lead the call sequences; and (3) some type-A fin whales stop calling when another type-A fin whale approaches at a distance of about 1.6 km. During 2010-2022, type-A calls occur every year, whereas type-B calls are prevalent only after November 2018. A culture transmission is proposed from type-A fin whales to type-B fin whales and/or a population increase of type-B fin whales in the region after November 2018.


Subject(s)
Acoustics , Fin Whale , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Fin Whale/physiology , Sound Spectrography , Time Factors , Islands
6.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1904): 20230444, 2024 Jun 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38705172

ABSTRACT

Passive acoustic monitoring (PAM) is a powerful tool for studying ecosystems. However, its effective application in tropical environments, particularly for insects, poses distinct challenges. Neotropical katydids produce complex species-specific calls, spanning mere milliseconds to seconds and spread across broad audible and ultrasonic frequencies. However, subtle differences in inter-pulse intervals or central frequencies are often the only discriminatory traits. These extremities, coupled with low source levels and susceptibility to masking by ambient noise, challenge species identification in PAM recordings. This study aimed to develop a deep learning-based solution to automate the recognition of 31 katydid species of interest in a biodiverse Panamanian forest with over 80 katydid species. Besides the innate challenges, our efforts were also encumbered by a limited and imbalanced initial training dataset comprising domain-mismatched recordings. To overcome these, we applied rigorous data engineering, improving input variance through controlled playback re-recordings and by employing physics-based data augmentation techniques, and tuning signal-processing, model and training parameters to produce a custom well-fit solution. Methods developed here are incorporated into Koogu, an open-source Python-based toolbox for developing deep learning-based bioacoustic analysis solutions. The parametric implementations offer a valuable resource, enhancing the capabilities of PAM for studying insects in tropical ecosystems. This article is part of the theme issue 'Towards a toolkit for global insect biodiversity monitoring'.


Subject(s)
Acoustics , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Panama , Deep Learning , Species Specificity
7.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1904): 20230112, 2024 Jun 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38705178

ABSTRACT

Insects are the most diverse animal taxon on Earth and play a key role in ecosystem functioning. However, they are often neglected by ecological surveys owing to the difficulties involved in monitoring this small and hyper-diverse taxon. With technological advances in biomonitoring and analytical methods, these shortcomings may finally be addressed. Here, we performed passive acoustic monitoring at 141 sites (eight habitats) to investigate insect acoustic activity in the Viruá National Park, Brazil. We first describe the frequency range occupied by three soniferous insect groups (cicadas, crickets and katydids) to calculate the acoustic evenness index (AEI). Then, we assess how AEI varies spatially and temporally among habitat types, and finally we investigate the relationship between vegetation structure variables and AEI for each insect category. Overall, crickets occupied lower and narrower frequency bands than cicadas and katydids. AEI values varied among insect categories and across space and time. The highest acoustic activity occurred before sunrise and the lowest acoustic activity was recorded in pastures. Canopy cover was positively associated with cricket acoustic activity but not with katydids. Our findings contribute to a better understanding of the role of time, habitat and vegetation structure in shaping insect activity within diverse Amazonian ecosystems. This article is part of the theme issue 'Towards a toolkit for global insect biodiversity monitoring'.


Subject(s)
Acoustics , Ecosystem , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Brazil , Gryllidae/physiology , Hemiptera/physiology , Orthoptera/physiology , Insecta/physiology
8.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1904): 20230111, 2024 Jun 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38705186

ABSTRACT

Global pollinator decline urgently requires effective methods to assess their trends, distribution and behaviour. Passive acoustics is a non-invasive and cost-efficient monitoring tool increasingly employed for monitoring animal communities. However, insect sounds remain highly unexplored, hindering the application of this technique for pollinators. To overcome this shortfall and support future developments, we recorded and characterized wingbeat sounds of a variety of Iberian domestic and wild bees and tested their relationship with taxonomic, morphological, behavioural and environmental traits at inter- and intra-specific levels. Using directional microphones and machine learning, we shed light on the acoustic signature of bee wingbeat sounds and their potential to be used for species identification and monitoring. Our results revealed that frequency of wingbeat sounds is negatively related with body size and environmental temperature (between-species analysis), while it is positively related with experimentally induced stress conditions (within-individual analysis). We also found a characteristic acoustic signature in the European honeybee that supported automated classification of this bee from a pool of wild bees, paving the way for passive acoustic monitoring of pollinators. Overall, these findings confirm that insect sounds during flight activity can provide insights on individual and species traits, and hence suggest novel and promising applications for this endangered animal group. This article is part of the theme issue 'Towards a toolkit for global insect biodiversity monitoring'.


Subject(s)
Acoustics , Wings, Animal , Animals , Bees/physiology , Wings, Animal/physiology , Flight, Animal/physiology , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Pollination , Sound
9.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1904): 20230110, 2024 Jun 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38705184

ABSTRACT

Night-time light can have profound ecological effects, even when the source is natural moonlight. The impacts of light can, however, vary substantially by taxon, habitat and geographical region. We used a custom machine learning model built with the Python package Koogu to investigate the in situ effects of moonlight on the calling activity of neotropical forest katydids over multiple years. We prioritised species with calls that were commonly detected in human annotated data, enabling us to evaluate model performance. We focused on eight species of katydids that the model identified with high precision (generally greater than 0.90) and moderate-to-high recall (minimum 0.35), ensuring that detections were generally correct and that many calls were detected. These results suggest that moonlight has modest effects on the amount of calling, with the magnitude and direction of effect varying by species: half of the species showed positive effects of light and half showed negative. These findings emphasize the importance of understanding natural history for anticipating how biological communities respond to moonlight. The methods applied in this project highlight the emerging opportunities for evaluating large quantities of data with machine learning models to address ecological questions over space and time. This article is part of the theme issue 'Towards a toolkit for global insect biodiversity monitoring'.


Subject(s)
Forests , Machine Learning , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Light
10.
Commun Biol ; 7(1): 575, 2024 May 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38750083

ABSTRACT

Despite extensive research on avian vocal learning, we still lack a general understanding of how and when this ability evolved in birds. As the closest living relatives of the earliest Passeriformes, the New Zealand wrens (Acanthisitti) hold a key phylogenetic position for furthering our understanding of the evolution of vocal learning because they share a common ancestor with two vocal learners: oscines and parrots. However, the vocal learning abilities of New Zealand wrens remain unexplored. Here, we test for the presence of prerequisite behaviors for vocal learning in one of the two extant species of New Zealand wrens, the rifleman (Acanthisitta chloris). We detect the presence of unique individual vocal signatures and show how these signatures are shaped by social proximity, as demonstrated by group vocal signatures and strong acoustic similarities among distantly related individuals in close social proximity. Further, we reveal that rifleman calls share similar phenotypic variance ratios to those previously reported in the learned vocalizations of the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata. Together these findings provide strong evidence that riflemen vocally converge, and though the mechanism still remains to be determined, they may also suggest that this vocal convergence is the result of rudimentary vocal learning abilities.


Subject(s)
Songbirds , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Songbirds/physiology , Social Behavior , New Zealand , Male , Learning , Female , Biological Evolution
11.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 11158, 2024 05 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38750135

ABSTRACT

Examples of symbiotic relationships often include cleaning mutualisms, typically involving interactions between cleaner fish and other fish, called the clients. While these cleaners can cooperate by removing ectoparasites from their clients, they can also deceive by feeding on client mucus, a behavior usually referred to as "cheating behavior" that often leads to a discernible jolt from the client fish. Despite extensive studies of these interactions, most research has focused on the visual aspects of the communication. In this study, we aimed to explore the role of acoustic communication in the mutualistic relationship between cleaner fishes and nine holocentrid client species across four regions of the Indo-Pacific Ocean: French Polynesia, Guam, Seychelles, and the Philippines. Video cameras coupled with hydrophones were positioned at various locations on reefs housing Holocentridae fish to observe their acoustic behaviors during interactions. Our results indicate that all nine species of holocentrids can use acoustic signals to communicate to cleaner fish their refusal of the symbiotic interaction or their desire to terminate the cooperation. These sounds were predominantly observed during agonistic behavior and seem to support visual cues from the client. This study provides a novel example of acoustic communication during a symbiotic relationship in teleosts. Interestingly, these vocalizations often lacked a distinct pattern or structure. This contrasts with numerous other interspecific communication systems where clear and distinguishable signals are essential. This absence of a clear acoustic pattern may be because they are used in interspecific interactions to support visual behavior with no selective pressure for developing specific calls required in conspecific recognition. The different sound types produced could also be correlated with the severity of the client response. There is a need for further research into the effects of acoustic behaviors on the quality and dynamics of these mutualistic interactions.


Subject(s)
Symbiosis , Animals , Symbiosis/physiology , Fishes/physiology , Sound , Acoustics , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Animal Communication , Coral Reefs , Pacific Ocean , Polynesia , Perciformes/physiology
12.
Proc Biol Sci ; 291(2023): 20232849, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38775542

ABSTRACT

Recent experiments have demonstrated that carnivores and ungulates in Africa, Asia, Europe and North America fear the human 'super predator' far more than other predators. Australian mammals have been a focus of research on predator naiveté because it is suspected they show atypical antipredator responses. To experimentally test if mammals in Australia also most fear humans, we quantified the responses of four native marsupials (eastern grey kangaroo, Bennett's wallaby, Tasmanian pademelon, common brushtail possum) and introduced fallow deer to playbacks of predator (human, dog, Tasmanian devil, wolf) or non-predator control (sheep) vocalizations. Native marsupials most feared the human 'super predator', fleeing humans 2.4 times more often than the next most frightening predator (dogs), and being most, and significantly, vigilant to humans. These results demonstrate that native marsupials are not naïve to the peril humans pose, substantially expanding the taxonomic and geographic scope of the growing experimental evidence that wildlife worldwide generally perceive humans as the planet's most frightening predator. Introduced fallow deer fled humans, but not more than other predators, which we suggest may result from their being introduced. Our results point to both challenges concerning marsupial conservation and opportunities for exploiting fear of humans as a wildlife management tool.


Subject(s)
Deer , Fear , Marsupialia , Predatory Behavior , Animals , Deer/physiology , Humans , Marsupialia/physiology , Australia , Introduced Species , Wolves/physiology , Dogs , Vocalization, Animal
13.
Science ; 384(6698): 874-877, 2024 May 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38781375

ABSTRACT

Producing a specific number of vocalizations with purpose requires a sophisticated combination of numerical abilities and vocal control. Whether this capacity exists in animals other than humans is yet unknown. We show that crows can flexibly produce variable numbers of one to four vocalizations in response to arbitrary cues associated with numerical values. The acoustic features of the first vocalization of a sequence were predictive of the total number of vocalizations, indicating a planning process. Moreover, the acoustic features of vocal units predicted their order in the sequence and could be used to read out counting errors during vocal production.


Subject(s)
Crows , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Crows/physiology , Cues , Male , Female , Acoustics
14.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 10715, 2024 05 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38782960

ABSTRACT

The large nose adorned by adult male proboscis monkeys is hypothesised to serve as an audiovisual signal of sexual selection. It serves as a visual signal of male quality and social status, and as an acoustic signal, through the expression of loud, low-formant nasalised calls in dense rainforests, where visibility is poor. However, it is unclear how the male proboscis monkey nasal complex, including the internal structure of the nose, plays a role in visual or acoustic signalling. Here, we use cranionasal data to assess whether large noses found in male proboscis monkeys serve visual and/or acoustic signalling functions. Our findings support a visual signalling function for male nasal enlargement through a relatively high degree of nasal aperture sexual size dimorphism, the craniofacial region to which nasal soft tissue attaches. We additionally find nasal aperture size increases beyond dental maturity among male proboscis monkeys, consistent with the visual signalling hypothesis. We show that the cranionasal region has an acoustic signalling role through pronounced nasal cavity sexual shape dimorphism, wherein male nasal cavity shape allows the expression of loud, low-formant nasalised calls. Our findings provide robust support for the male proboscis monkey nasal complex serving both visual and acoustic functions.


Subject(s)
Sex Characteristics , Animals , Male , Nasal Cavity/anatomy & histology , Nasal Cavity/physiology , Nose/anatomy & histology , Animal Communication , Acoustics , Skull/anatomy & histology , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Female
16.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1905): 20230183, 2024 Jul 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38768197

ABSTRACT

Because of the diverging needs of individuals, group life can lead to disputes and competition, but it also has many advantages, such as reduced predation risk, information sharing and increased hunting success. Social animals have to maintain group cohesion and need to synchronize activities, such as foraging, resting, social interactions and movements, in order to thrive in groups. Acoustic signals are highly relevant for social dynamics, some because they are long-ranging and others because they are short-ranging, which may serve important within-group functions. However, although there has been an increase in studies concentrating on acoustic communication within groups in the past decade, many aspects of how vocalizations relate to group dynamics are still poorly understood. The aim of this review is to present an overview of our current knowledge on the role of vocalizations in regulating social group dynamics, identify knowledge gaps and recommend potential future research directions. We review the role that vocalizations play in (i) collective movement, (ii) separation risk and cohesion maintenance, (iii) fission-fusion dynamics, and (iv) social networks. We recommend that future studies aim to increase the diversity of studied species and strengthen the integration of state-of-the-art tools to study social dynamics and acoustic signals. This article is part of the theme issue 'The power of sound: unravelling how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics'.


Subject(s)
Social Behavior , Vocalization, Animal , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Animals , Group Dynamics
17.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1905): 20230182, 2024 Jul 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38768200

ABSTRACT

Acoustic signalling is a key mode of communication owing to its instantaneousness and rapid turnover, its saliency and flexibility and its ability to function strategically in both short- and long-range contexts. Acoustic communication is closely intertwined with both collective behaviour and social network structure, as it can facilitate the coordination of collective decisions and behaviour, and play an important role in establishing, maintaining and modifying social relationships. These research topics have each been studied separately and represent three well-established research areas. Yet, despite the close connection of acoustic communication with collective behaviour and social networks in natural systems, only few studies have focused on their interaction. The aim of this theme issue is therefore to build a foundation for understanding how acoustic communication is linked to collective behaviour, on the one hand, and social network structure on the other, in non-human animals. Through the building of such a foundation, our hope is that new questions in new avenues of research will arise. Understanding the links between acoustic communication and social behaviour seems crucial for gaining a comprehensive understanding of sociality and social evolution. This article is part of the theme issue 'The power of sound: unravelling how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics'.


Subject(s)
Social Behavior , Animals , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Acoustics , Sound , Group Dynamics
18.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1905): 20230193, 2024 Jul 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38768201

ABSTRACT

Vocalizations coordinate social interactions between conspecifics by conveying information concerning the individual or group identity of the sender. Social accommodation is a form of vocal learning where social affinity is signalled by converging or diverging vocalizations with those of conspecifics. To investigate whether social accommodation is linked to the social lifestyle of the sender, we investigated sex-specific differences in social accommodation in a dispersed living primate, the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus), where females form stable sleeping groups whereas males live solitarily. We used 482 trill calls of 36 individuals from our captive breeding colony to compare acoustic dissimilarity between individuals with genetic relatedness, social contact time and body weight. Our results showed that female trills become more similar the more time females spend with each other, independent of genetic relationship, suggesting vocal convergence. In contrast, male trills were affected more by genetic than social factors. However, focusing only on socialized males, increasing time as cage partners caused greater divergence in males' trills. Thus, grey mouse lemurs show the capacity for social accommodation, with females converging their trills to signal social closeness to sleeping group partners, whereas males do not adapt or diverge their trills to signal individual distinctiveness. This article is part of the theme issue 'The power of sound: unravelling how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics'.


Subject(s)
Cheirogaleidae , Social Behavior , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Cheirogaleidae/physiology , Male , Female , Sex Characteristics , Sex Factors
19.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1905): 20230194, 2024 Jul 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38768196

ABSTRACT

Vocal communication is an emblematic feature of group-living animals, used to share information and strengthen social bonds. Vocalizations are also used to coordinate group-level behaviours in many taxa, but little is known of the factors that may influence vocal behaviour during cooperative acts. Allied male Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) use the 'pop' vocalization as a coercive signal when working together to herd single oestrous females. Using long-term association and acoustic data, we examined the influence of social and non-social factors on pop use by allied male dolphins in this context. Neither pop rate nor pop bout duration were influenced by any of the factors examined. However, allied males with stronger social bonds engaged in higher rates of vocal synchrony; whereby they actively matched the timing of their pop production. Hence, social bond strength influenced pop use in a cooperative context, suggesting dual functions of pop use: to induce the female to remain close, and to promote social bond maintenance and cooperation among males. This article is part of the theme issue 'The power of sound: unravelling how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics'.


Subject(s)
Bottle-Nosed Dolphin , Sexual Behavior, Animal , Social Behavior , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Bottle-Nosed Dolphin/physiology , Bottle-Nosed Dolphin/psychology , Male , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Female , Sexual Behavior, Animal/physiology , Cooperative Behavior
20.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1905): 20230190, 2024 Jul 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38768202

ABSTRACT

Animal communication is frequently studied with conventional network representations that link pairs of individuals who interact, for example, through vocalization. However, acoustic signals often have multiple simultaneous receivers, or receivers integrate information from multiple signallers, meaning these interactions are not dyadic. Additionally, non-dyadic social structures often shape an individual's behavioural response to vocal communication. Recently, major advances have been made in the study of these non-dyadic, higher-order networks (e.g. hypergraphs and simplicial complexes). Here, we show how these approaches can provide new insights into vocal communication through three case studies that illustrate how higher-order network models can: (i) alter predictions made about the outcome of vocally coordinated group departures; (ii) generate different patterns of song synchronization from models that only include dyadic interactions; and (iii) inform models of cultural evolution of vocal communication. Together, our examples highlight the potential power of higher-order networks to study animal vocal communication. We then build on our case studies to identify key challenges in applying higher-order network approaches in this context and outline important research questions that these techniques could help answer. This article is part of the theme issue 'The power of sound: unravelling how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics'.


Subject(s)
Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Social Behavior , Animal Communication , Models, Biological
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...