Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 32
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Primates ; 63(5): 535-546, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35838928

RESUMEN

Gaze aversion is a behavior adopted by several mammalian and non-mammalian species in response to eye contact, and is usually interpreted as a reaction to a perceived threat. Unlike many other primate species, common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) are thought to have a high tolerance for direct gaze, barely exhibiting gaze avoidance towards conspecifics and humans. Here we show that this does not hold for marmosets interacting with a familiar experimenter who suddenly establishes eye contact in a playful interaction (peekaboo). Video footage synchronously recorded from the perspective of the marmoset and the experimenter showed that the monkeys consistently alternated between eye contact and head-gaze aversion, and that these responses were often preceded by head-cocking. We hypothesize that this behavioral strategy helps marmosets to temporarily disengage from emotionally overwhelming social stimulation due to sight of another individual's face, in order to prepare for a new round of affiliative face-to-face interactions.


Asunto(s)
Callithrix , Juego e Implementos de Juego , Animales , Callithrix/fisiología , Humanos , Mamíferos
3.
PLoS Biol ; 19(9): e3001400, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34529650

RESUMEN

Purkinje cell (PC) discharge, the only output of cerebellar cortex, involves 2 types of action potentials, high-frequency simple spikes (SSs) and low-frequency complex spikes (CSs). While there is consensus that SSs convey information needed to optimize movement kinematics, the function of CSs, determined by the PC's climbing fiber input, remains controversial. While initially thought to be specialized in reporting information on motor error for the subsequent amendment of behavior, CSs seem to contribute to other aspects of motor behavior as well. When faced with the bewildering diversity of findings and views unraveled by highly specific tasks, one may wonder if there is just one true function with all the other attributions wrong? Or is the diversity of findings a reflection of distinct pools of PCs, each processing specific streams of information conveyed by climbing fibers? With these questions in mind, we recorded CSs from the monkey oculomotor vermis deploying a repetitive saccade task that entailed sizable motor errors as well as small amplitude saccades, correcting them. We demonstrate that, in addition to carrying error-related information, CSs carry information on the metrics of both primary and small corrective saccades in a time-specific manner, with changes in CS firing probability coupled with changes in CS duration. Furthermore, we also found CS activity that seemed to predict the upcoming events. Hence PCs receive a multiplexed climbing fiber input that merges complementary streams of information on the behavior, separable by the recipient PC because they are staggered in time.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Movimiento
4.
eNeuro ; 7(4)2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32513660

RESUMEN

Research on social perception in monkeys may benefit from standardized, controllable, and ethologically valid renditions of conspecifics offered by monkey avatars. However, previous work has cautioned that monkeys, like humans, show an adverse reaction toward realistic synthetic stimuli, known as the "uncanny valley" effect. We developed an improved naturalistic rhesus monkey face avatar capable of producing facial expressions (fear grin, lip smack and threat), animated by motion capture data of real monkeys. For validation, we additionally created decreasingly naturalistic avatar variants. Eight rhesus macaques were tested on the various videos and avoided looking at less naturalistic avatar variants, but not at the most naturalistic or the most unnaturalistic avatar, indicating an uncanny valley effect for the less naturalistic avatar versions. The avoidance was deepened by motion and accompanied by physiological arousal. Only the most naturalistic avatar evoked facial expressions comparable to those toward the real monkey videos. Hence, our findings demonstrate that the uncanny valley reaction in monkeys can be overcome by a highly naturalistic avatar.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Expresión Facial , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Movimiento (Física) , Percepción Social
5.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 15292, 2019 10 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31653910

RESUMEN

The ability to extract the direction of the other's gaze allows us to shift our attention to an object of interest to the other and to establish joint attention. By mapping one's own intentions on the object of joint attention, humans develop a Theory of (the other's) Mind (TOM), a functional sequence possibly disrupted in autism. Gaze following of both humans and old world monkeys is orchestrated by very similar cortical architectures, strongly suggesting homology. Also new world monkeys, a primate suborder that split from the old world monkey line about 35 million years ago, have complex social structures and one member of this group, the common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) are known to follow human head-gaze. However, the question is if they use gaze following to establish joint attention with conspecifics. Here we show that this is indeed the case. In a free choice task, head-restrained marmosets prefer objects gazed at by a conspecific and, moreover, they exhibit considerably shorter choice reaction times for the same objects. These findings support the assumption of an evolutionarily old domain specific faculty shared within the primate order and they underline the potential value of marmosets in studies of normal and disturbed joint attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Callithrix/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Reflejo/fisiología , Animales , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
6.
PLoS Biol ; 16(8): e2004344, 2018 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30067764

RESUMEN

The cerebellum allows us to rapidly adjust motor behavior to the needs of the situation. It is commonly assumed that cerebellum-based motor learning is guided by the difference between the desired and the actual behavior, i.e., by error information. Not only immediate but also future behavior will benefit from an error because it induces lasting changes of parallel fiber synapses on Purkinje cells (PCs), whose output mediates the behavioral adjustments. Olivary climbing fibers, likewise connecting with PCs, are thought to transport information on instant errors needed for the synaptic modification yet not to contribute to error memory. Here, we report work on monkeys tested in a saccadic learning paradigm that challenges this concept. We demonstrate not only a clear complex spikes (CS) signature of the error at the time of its occurrence but also a reverberation of this signature much later, before a new manifestation of the behavior, suitable to improve it.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Cerebelo/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Animales , Axones/fisiología , Cerebelo/anatomía & histología , Cerebelo/citología , Electrodos Implantados , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Células de Purkinje/citología , Técnicas Estereotáxicas , Sinapsis/fisiología
7.
J Vis ; 17(9): 19, 2017 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28837965

RESUMEN

Scrutiny of the visual environment requires saccades that shift gaze to objects of interest. In case the object should be moving, smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) try to keep the image of the object within the confines of the fovea in order to ensure sufficient time for its analysis. Both saccades and SPEM can be adaptively changed by the experience of insufficiencies, compromising the precision of saccades or the minimization of object image slip in the case of SPEM. As both forms of adaptation rely on the cerebellar oculomotor vermis (OMV), most probably deploying a shared neuronal machinery, one might expect that the adaptation of one type of eye movement should affect the kinematics of the other. In order to test this expectation, we subjected two monkeys to a standard saccadic adaption paradigm with SPEM test trials at the end and, alternatively, the same two monkeys plus a third one to a random saccadic adaptation paradigm with interleaved trials of SPEM. In contrast to our expectation, we observed at best marginal transfer which, moreover, had little consistency across experiments and subjects. The lack of consistent transfer of saccadic adaptation decisively constrains models of the implementation of oculomotor learning in the OMV, suggesting an extensive separation of saccade- and SPEM-related synapses on P-cell dendritic trees.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Modelos Animales , Probabilidad
8.
eNeuro ; 4(2)2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28374010

RESUMEN

Humans follow another person's eye gaze to objects of interest to the other, thereby establishing joint attention, a first step toward developing a theory of the other's mind. Previous functional MRI studies agree that a "gaze-following patch" (GFP) of cortex close to the posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS) is specifically implicated in eye gaze-following. The location of the GFP is in the vicinity of the posterior members of the core face-processing system that consists of distinct patches in ventral visual cortex, the STS, and frontal cortex, also involved in processing information on the eyes. To test whether the GFP might correspond to one of the posterior face patches, we compared the pattern of blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) imaging contrasts reflecting the passive vision of static faces with the one evoked by shifts of attention guided by the eye gaze of others. The viewing of static faces revealed the face patch system. On the other hand, eye gaze-following activated a cortical patch (the GFP) with its activation maximum separated by more than 24 mm in the right and 19 mm in the left hemisphere from the nearest face patch, the STS face area (FA). This segregation supports a distinct function of the GFP, different from the elementary processing of facial information.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Fijación Ocular , Percepción Social , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Circulación Cerebrovascular/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Oxígeno/sangre , Estimulación Luminosa , Lóbulo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
9.
Sci Rep ; 7: 40613, 2017 01 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28091557

RESUMEN

Saccades and smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) are two types of goal-directed eye movements whose kinematics differ profoundly, a fact that may have contributed to the notion that the underlying cerebellar substrates are separated. However, it is suggested that some Purkinje cells (PCs) in the oculomotor vermis (OMV) of monkey cerebellum may be involved in both saccades and SPEM, a puzzling finding in view of the different kinematic demands of the two types of eye movements. Such 'dual' OMV PCs might be oddities with little if any functional relevance. On the other hand, they might be representatives of a generic mechanism serving as common ground for saccades and SPEM. In our present study, we found that both saccade- and SPEM-related responses of individual PCs could be predicted well by linear combinations of eye acceleration, velocity and position. The relative weights of the contributions that these three kinematic parameters made depended on the type of eye movement. Whereas in the case of saccades eye position was the most important independent variable, it was velocity in the case of SPEM. This dissociation is in accordance with standard models of saccades and SPEM control which emphasize eye position and velocity respectively as the relevant controlled state variables.


Asunto(s)
Músculos Oculomotores/citología , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiología , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Animales , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Neuronas/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme
10.
Eur J Neurosci ; 44(8): 2531-2542, 2016 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27255776

RESUMEN

Recent studies have suggested that microsaccades, the small amplitude saccades made during fixation, are precisely controlled. Two lines of evidence suggest that the cerebellum plays a key role not only in improving the accuracy of macrosaccades but also of microsaccades. First, lesions of the fastigial oculomotor regions (FOR) cause horizontal dysmetria of both micro- and macrosaccades. Secondly, our previous work on Purkinje cell simple spikes in the oculomotor vermis (OV) has established qualitatively similar response preferences for these two groups of saccades. In this work, we investigated the control signals for micro- and macrosaccades in the FOR, the target of OV Purkinje cell axons. We found that the same FOR neurons discharged for micro- and macrosaccades. For both groups of saccades, FOR neurons exhibited very similar dependencies of their discharge strength on direction and amplitude and very similar burst onset time differences for ipsi- and contraversive saccades and, in both, response duration reflected saccade duration, at least at the population level. An intriguing characteristic of microsaccade-related responses is that immediate pre-saccadic firing rates decreased with distance to the target center, a pattern that strikingly parallels the eye position dependency of both microsaccade metrics and frequency, which may suggest a potential neural mechanism underlying the role of FOR in fixation. Irrespective of this specific consideration, our study supports the view that microsaccades and macrosaccades share the same cerebellar circuitry and, in general, further strengthens the notion of a microsaccade-macrosaccade continuum.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Axones/fisiología , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Cerebelo/fisiología , Macaca mulatta
11.
Proc Biol Sci ; 282(1816): 20151020, 2015 Oct 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26446808

RESUMEN

Human eye-gaze is a powerful stimulus, drawing the observer's attention to places and objects of interest to someone else ('eye-gaze following'). The largely homogeneous eyes of monkeys, compromising the assessment of eye-gaze by conspecifics from larger distances, explain the absence of comparable eye-gaze following in these animals. Yet, monkeys are able to use peer head orientation to shift attention ('head-gaze following'). How similar are monkeys' head-gaze and human eye-gaze following? To address this question, we trained rhesus monkeys to make saccades to targets, either identified by the head-gaze of demonstrator monkeys or, alternatively, identified by learned associations between the demonstrators' facial identities and the targets (gaze versus identity following). In a variant of this task that occurred at random, the instruction to follow head-gaze or identity was replaced in the course of a trial by the new rule to detect a change of luminance of one of the saccade targets. Although this change-of-rule rendered the demonstrator portraits irrelevant, they nevertheless influenced performance, reflecting a precise redistribution of spatial attention. The specific features depended on whether the initial rule was head-gaze or identity following: head-gaze caused an insuppressible shift of attention to the target gazed at by the demonstrator, whereas identity matching prompted much later shifts of attention, however, only if the initial rule had been identity following. Furthermore, shifts of attention prompted by head-gaze were spatially precise. Automaticity and swiftness, spatial precision and limited executive control characterizing monkeys' head-gaze following are key features of human eye-gaze following. This similarity supports the notion that both may rely on the same conserved neural circuitry.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Aprendizaje , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Cara , Cabeza , Visión Ocular
12.
Vision Res ; 112: 68-82, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25982719

RESUMEN

Despite the ecological importance of gaze following, little is known about the underlying neuronal processes, which allow us to extract gaze direction from the geometric features of the eye and head of a conspecific. In order to understand the neuronal mechanisms underlying this ability, a careful description of the capacity and the limitations of gaze following at the behavioral level is needed. Previous studies of gaze following, which relied on naturalistic settings have the disadvantage of allowing only very limited control of potentially relevant visual features guiding gaze following, such as the contrast of iris and sclera, the shape of the eyelids and--in the case of photographs--they lack depth. Hence, in order to get full control of potentially relevant features we decided to study gaze following of human observers guided by the gaze of a human avatar seen stereoscopically. To this end we established a stereoscopic 3D virtual reality setup, in which we tested human subjects' abilities to detect at which target a human avatar was looking at. Following the gaze of the avatar showed all the features of the gaze following of a natural person, namely a substantial degree of precision associated with a consistent pattern of systematic deviations from the target. Poor stereo vision affected performance surprisingly little (only in certain experimental conditions). Only gaze following guided by targets at larger downward eccentricities exhibited a differential effect of the presence or absence of accompanying movements of the avatar's eyelids and eyebrows.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Simulación por Computador , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Cejas , Párpados , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
13.
J Neurosci ; 35(8): 3403-11, 2015 Feb 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25716840

RESUMEN

Microsaccades, the small saccades made when we try to keep the eyes still, were once believed to be inconsequential for vision, but recent studies suggest that they can precisely relocate gaze to tiny visual targets. Because the cerebellum is necessary for motor precision, we investigated whether microsaccades may exploit this neural machinery in monkeys. Almost all vermal Purkinje cells, which provide the eye-related output of the cerebellar cortex, were found to increase or decrease their simple spike firing rate during microsaccades. At both the single-cell and population level, microsaccade-related activity was highly similar to macrosaccade-related activity and we observed a continuous representation of saccade amplitude that spanned both the macrosaccade and microsaccade domains. Our results suggest that the cerebellum's role in fine-tuning eye movements extends even to the oculomotor system's smallest saccades and add to a growing list of observations that call into question the classical categorical distinction between microsaccades and macrosaccades.


Asunto(s)
Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino
14.
Elife ; 32014 Jul 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25024428

RESUMEN

Primates use gaze cues to follow peer gaze to an object of joint attention. Gaze following of monkeys is largely determined by head or face orientation. We used fMRI in rhesus monkeys to identify brain regions underlying head gaze following and to assess their relationship to the 'face patch' system, the latter being the likely source of information on face orientation. We trained monkeys to locate targets by either following head gaze or using a learned association of face identity with the same targets. Head gaze following activated a distinct region in the posterior STS, close to-albeit not overlapping with-the medial face patch delineated by passive viewing of faces. This 'gaze following patch' may be the substrate of the geometrical calculations needed to translate information on head orientation from the face patches into precise shifts of attention, taking the spatial relationship of the two interacting agents into account.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Lóbulo Temporal/anatomía & histología , Percepción Visual , Animales , Atención , Conducta Animal , Simulación por Computador , Señales (Psicología) , Fijación Ocular , Cabeza , Macaca mulatta , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Orientación , Reconocimiento de Normas Patrones Automatizadas , Estimulación Luminosa , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Visión Ocular
15.
Front Syst Neurosci ; 7: 3, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23494070

RESUMEN

Smooth pursuit adaptation (SPA) is an example of cerebellum-dependent motor learning that depends on the integrity of the oculomotor vermis (OMV). In an attempt to unveil the neuronal basis of the role of the OMV in SPA, we recorded Purkinje cell simple spikes (PC SS) of trained monkeys. Individual PC SS exhibited specific changes of their discharge patterns during the course of SPA. However, these individual changes did not provide a reliable explanation of the behavioral changes. On the other hand, the population response of PC SS perfectly reflected the changes resulting from adaptation. Population vector was calculated using all cells recorded independent of their location. A population code conveying the behavioral changes is in full accordance with the anatomical convergence of PC axons on target neurons in the cerebellar nuclei. Its computational advantage is the ease with which it can be adjusted to the needs of the behavior by changing the contribution of individual PC SS based on error feedback.

16.
Cereb Cortex ; 22(2): 345-62, 2012 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21670098

RESUMEN

The pontine nuclei (PN) are the major intermediary elements in the corticopontocerebellar pathway. Here we asked if the PN may help to adapt the spatial reference frames used by cerebrocortical neurons involved in the sensory guidance of movement to a format potentially more appropriate for the cerebellum. To this end, we studied movement-related neurons in the dorsal PN (DPN) of monkeys, most probably projecting to the cerebellum, executing fixed vector saccades or, alternatively, fixed vector hand reaches from different starting positions. The 83 task-related neurons considered fired movement-related bursts before saccades (saccade-related) or before hand movements (hand movement-related). About 40% of the SR neurons were "oculocentric," whereas the others were modulated by eye starting position. A third of the HMR neurons encoded hand reaches in hand-centered coordinates, whereas the remainder exhibited different types of dependencies on starting positions, reminiscent in general of cortical responses. All in all, pontine reference frames for the sensory guidance of movement seem to be very similar to those in cortex. Specifically, the frequency of orbital position gain fields of SR neurons is identical in the DPN and in one of their major cortical inputs, lateral intraparietal area (LIP).


Asunto(s)
Movimiento/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Puente/citología , Puente/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Animales , Fijación Ocular , Mano , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
17.
Cereb Cortex ; 22(4): 877-91, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21725035

RESUMEN

Lesion studies suggest that the oculomotor vermis (OMV) is critical for the initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements (SPEMs); yet, its specific role has remained elusive. In this study, we tested the hypothesis that vermal Purkinje cells (PCs) may be needed to fine-tune the kinematic description of SPEM initiation. Recording from identified PCs from the monkey OMV, we observed that SPEM-related PCs were characterized by a formidable diversity of response profiles with typically only modest reflection of eye movement kinematics. In contrast, the PC population discharge could be perfectly predicted based on a linear combination of eye acceleration, velocity, and position. This finding is in full accord with a role of the OMV in shaping eye movement kinematics. It, moreover, supports the notion that this shaping action is based on a population code, whose anatomic basis is the convergence of PCs on target neurons in the cerebellar nuclei.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Cerebelo/citología , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Animales , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , Modelos Lineales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción Espacial/fisiología
18.
Neuroimage ; 54(2): 1643-53, 2011 Jan 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20832481

RESUMEN

Previous fMRI experiments showed an involvement of the STS in the processing of eye-gaze direction in joint attention. Since head-gaze direction can also be used for the assessment of another person's attentional focus, we compared the mechanisms underlying the processing of head- and eye-gaze direction using a combined psychophysical and fMRI approach. Subjects actively followed the head- or eye-gaze direction of a person in a photograph towards one of seven possible targets by moving their eyes. We showed that the right posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS) as well as the right fusiform gyrus (FSG) were involved in both processing of head- as well as eye-gaze direction. Another finding was a bilateral deactivation of a distinct area in the middle STS (mSTS) as well as the left anterior STS (aSTS), that was stronger when subjects followed eye-gaze direction than when they followed head-gaze direction. We assume that this deactivation is based on an active suppression of information arising from the distracting other directional cue, i.e. head-gaze direction in the eye-gaze direction task and eye-gaze direction in the head-gaze direction task. These results further support the hypothesis that the human equivalent of the gaze sensitive area in monkeys lies in more anterior parts of the STS than previously thought.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Ojo , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Cabeza , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
19.
J Neurosci ; 30(47): 15834-42, 2010 Nov 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21106822

RESUMEN

The physical properties of our bodies are subject to change (due to fatigue, heavy equipment, injury or aging) as we move around in the surrounding environment. The traditional definition of motor adaptation dictates that a mechanism in our brain needs to compensate for such alterations by appropriately modifying neural motor commands, if the vitally important accuracy of executed movements is to be preserved. In this article we describe how a repetitive eye movement task brings about changes in eye saccade kinematics that compromise accurate motor performance in the absence of a proper compensatory response. Surgical lesions in animals and human patient studies have previously demonstrated that an intact cerebellum is necessary for the compensation to arise and prevent the occurrence of hypometric movements. Here we identified the dynamic properties of the eye plant by recording from abducens motoneurons responsible for the required movement and measured the muscle response to microstimulation of the abducens nucleus in rhesus monkeys. The ensuing results demonstrate that the muscular periphery remains intact during the fatiguing eye movement task, while internal sources of noise (drowsiness, attentional modulation, neuronal fatigue etc.) must be responsible for a diminished oculomotor performance. This finding leads to the important realization that while supervising the accuracy of our movements, the nervous system takes additionally into account and adapts to any disruptive processes within the brain itself, clearly unrelated to the dynamical behavior of muscles or the environment. The existence of this supplementary mechanism forces a reassessment of traditional views of cerebellum-dependent motor adaptation.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fatiga Muscular/fisiología , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiología , Animales , Sistema Nervioso Central/fisiología , Macaca mulatta , Músculos Oculomotores/inervación , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
20.
J Vis ; 9(9): 5.1-13, 2009 Aug 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19761338

RESUMEN

During pursuit eye movements, the world around us remains perceptually stable despite the retinal-image slip induced by the eye movement. It is commonly held that this perceptual invariance is achieved by subtracting an internal reference signal, reflecting the eye movement, from the retinal motion signal. However, if the reference signal is too small or too large, a false eye-movement-induced motion of the external world, the Filehne illusion (FI), will be perceived. A reference signal of inadequate size can be simulated experimentally by asking human subjects to pursue a target across backgrounds with externally added motion that are perceived as moving. In the present study we asked if non-human primates respond to such manipulation in a way comparable to humans. Using psychophysical methods, we demonstrate that Rhesus monkeys do indeed experience a percept of pursuit-induced background motion. In this study we show that an FI can be predictably induced in Rhesus monkeys. The monkey FI shows dependencies on the size and direction of background movement, which is very similar to the ones characterizing the human FI. This congruence suggests that the perception of self-induced visual motion is based on similar inferential mechanisms in non-human and human primates.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones/fisiología , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Psicofísica , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Animales , Humanos , Masculino , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Retina/fisiología , Especificidad de la Especie
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...