Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 11 de 11
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
N Engl J Med ; 344(16): 1179-87, 2001 Apr 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11309632

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: A main indication for the insertion of tympanostomy tubes in infants and young children is persistent otitis media with effusion, reflecting concern that this condition may cause lasting impairments of speech, language, cognitive, and psychosocial development. However, evidence of such relations is inconclusive, and evidence is lacking that the insertion of tympanostomy tubes prevents developmental impairment. METHODS: We enrolled 6350 healthy infants from 2 to 61 days of age and evaluated them regularly for middle-ear effusion. Before the age of three years 429 children with persistent effusion were randomly assigned to have tympanostomy tubes inserted either as soon as possible or up to nine months later if effusion persisted. In 402 of these children we assessed speech, language, cognition, and psychosocial development at the age of three years. RESULTS: By the age of three years, 169 children in the early-treatment group (82 percent) and 66 children in the late-treatment group (34 percent) had received tympanostomy tubes. There were no significant differences between the early-treatment group and the late-treatment group at the age of three years in the mean (+/-SD) scores on the Number of Different Words test, a measure of word diversity (124+/-32 and 126+/-30, respectively); the Percentage of Consonants Correct-Revised test, a measure of speech-sound production (85+/-7 vs. 86+/-7); the General Cognitive Index of McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities (99+/-14 vs. 101+/-13); or on measures of receptive language, sentence length, grammatical complexity, parent-child stress, and behavior. CONCLUSIONS: In children younger than three years of age who have persistent otitis media, prompt insertion of tympanostomy tubes does not measurably improve developmental outcomes at the age of three years.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Ventilación del Oído Medio , Otitis Media con Derrame/cirugía , Audiometría , Conducta Infantil , Lenguaje Infantil , Preescolar , Enfermedad Crónica , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Pruebas de Inteligencia , Masculino , Factores Socioeconómicos , Habla , Factores de Tiempo
2.
Child Dev ; 71(2): 310-22, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10834466

RESUMEN

In a prospective study of child development in relation to early-life otitis media, we administered the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories (CDI) to a large (N = 2,156), sociodemographically diverse sample of 1- and 2-year-old children. As a prerequisite for interpreting the CDI scores, we studied selected measurement properties of the inventories. Scores on the CDI/Words and Gestures (CDI-WG), designed for children 8 to 16 months old, and on the CDI/Words and Sentences (CDI-WS), designed for children 16 to 30 months old, increased significantly with months of age. On several scales of both CDI-WG and CDI-WS, standard deviations approximated or exceeded mean values, reflecting wide variability in results. Statistically significant differences in mean scores were found according to race, maternal education, and health insurance status as an indirect measure of income, but the directionality of differences was not consistent across inventories or across scales of the CDI-WS. Correlations between CDI-WG and CDI-WS ranged from .18 to .39. Our findings suggest that the CDI reflects the progress of language development within the age range 10 to 27 months. However, researchers and clinicians should exercise caution in using results of the CDI to identify individual children at risk for language deficits, to compare groups of children with different sociodemographic profiles, or to evaluate the effects of interventions.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Lenguaje Infantil , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Preescolar , Cognición/fisiología , Femenino , Gestos , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino
3.
Pediatrics ; 105(5): 1119-30, 2000 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10790473

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: As part of a prospective study of possible effects of early-life otitis media on speech, language, cognitive, and psychosocial development, we tested relationships between children's cumulative duration of middle ear effusion (MEE) in their first 3 years of life and their scores on measures of language, speech sound production, and cognition at 3 years of age. METHODS: We enrolled 6350 healthy infants by 2 months of age who presented for primary care at 1 of 2 urban hospitals or 1 of 2 small town/rural and 4 suburban private pediatric practices. We intensively monitored the children's middle ear status by pneumatic otoscopy, supplemented by tympanometry, throughout their first 3 years of life; we monitored the validity of the otoscopic observations on an ongoing basis; and we treated children for otitis media according to specified guidelines. Children who met specified minimum criteria regarding the persistence of MEE became eligible for a clinical trial in which they were assigned randomly to undergo tympanostomy tube placement either promptly or after a defined extended period if MEE remained present. From among those remaining, we selected randomly, within sociodemographic strata, a sample of 241 children who represented a spectrum of MEE experience from having no MEE to having MEE whose cumulative duration fell just short of meeting randomization criteria. In subjects so selected, the estimated duration of MEE ranged from none to 65.6% of the first year of life and 44.8% of the first 3 years of life. In these 241 children we assessed language development, speech sound production, and cognition at 3 years of age, using both formal tests and conversational samples. RESULTS: We found weak to moderate, statistically significant negative correlations between children's cumulative durations of MEE in their first year of life or in age periods that included their first year of life, and their scores on formal tests of receptive vocabulary and verbal aspects of cognition at 3 years of age. However, the percent of variance in these scores explained by time with MEE in the first year of life beyond that explained by sociodemographic variables ranged only from 1.2% to 2.9%, and the negative correlations were concentrated in the subgroup of children whose families had private health insurance (rather than Medicaid). We found no significant correlations in the study population as a whole or in any subgroup between time with MEE during antecedent periods and children's scores on measures of spontaneous expressive language, speech sound production, or other measured aspects of cognition. In contrast, by wide margins, scores on all measures were consistently highest among the most socioeconomically advantaged children and lowest among the most socioeconomically disadvantaged children. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings suggest either that persistent early-life MEE actually causes later small, circumscribed impairments of receptive language and verbal aspects of cognition in certain groups of children or that unidentified, confounding factors predispose children both to early-life otitis media and to certain types of developmental impairment. Findings in the randomized clinical trial component of the larger study should help distinguish between causality and confounding as explanations for our findings.language, speech, cognition, development, otitis media, middle ear effusion.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje Infantil , Otitis Media con Derrame/fisiopatología , Habla , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
4.
Pediatrics ; 104(6): 1264-73, 1999 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10585976

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: As part of a long-term study of possible effects of early-life otitis media on speech, language, cognitive, and psychosocial development, we tested relationships between parents' ratings of parent-child stress at ages 1, 2, and 3 years, and of their children's behavior problems at ages 2 and 3 years, and the children's cumulative duration of middle-ear effusion (MEE) in their first 3 years of life. METHODS: We enrolled healthy infants by age 2 months who presented for primary care at 1 of 2 urban hospitals or 1 of 2 small-town/rural and 4 suburban private pediatric practices. We obtained standardized baseline measures of parental stress; we intensively monitored the children's middle-ear status by pneumatic otoscopy, supplemented by tympanometry, throughout their first 3 years of life; we monitored the validity of the otoscopic observations on an ongoing basis; and we treated children for otitis media according to specified guidelines. We obtained parent ratings of parental stress using the Parenting Stress Index/Short Form when the children reached ages 1, 2, and 3 years, and parent ratings of children's behavior using the Child Behavior Checklist when the children reached ages 2 and 3 years. RESULTS: In 2278 children we found no substantial relationships between parents' ratings of parent-child stress when the children reached ages 1, 2, and 3 years, or of their children's behavior problems at ages 2 and 3 years, and the cumulative duration of the children's MEE during antecedent periods. On the other hand, ratings both of parent-child stress and of behavior problems were consistently highest among the most socioeconomically disadvantaged children and lowest among the most socioeconomically advantaged children. Ratings also tended to be highest among children whose parents' baseline stress scores were highest. CONCLUSIONS: Parent-child stress and children's behavior problems in the first 3 years of life, as rated by parents, bear little or no relationship to the children's previous cumulative duration of MEE.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil/psicología , Otitis Media/psicología , Padres/psicología , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Adulto , Antibacterianos/uso terapéutico , Preescolar , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Ventilación del Oído Medio , Otitis Media/terapia , Pennsylvania , Estudios Prospectivos , Factores Socioeconómicos , Factores de Tiempo
5.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 42(6): 1432-43, 1999 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10599625

RESUMEN

The present study was designed to determine whether 4 measures of children's spontaneous speech and language differed according to the educational level of the children's mothers. Spontaneous language samples from 240 three-year-old children were analyzed to determine mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLUm), number of different words (NDW), total number of words (TNW), and percentage of consonants correct (PCC). A norm-referenced, knowledge-dependent measure of language comprehension, the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R), was also included for purposes of comparison with the spontaneous measures. Three levels of maternal education were compared: less than high school graduate, high school graduate, and college graduate. Trend analyses showed statistically significant linear trends across educational levels for MLUm, NDW, TNW, and PPVT-R; the trend for PCC was not significant. The relationship of maternal education and other sociodemographic variables to measures of children's language should be examined before using such measures to identify children with language disorders.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje Infantil , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Madres/psicología , Habla/fisiología , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Preescolar , Escolaridad , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Trastornos del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Estudios Prospectivos , Población Rural , Población Urbana
6.
Pediatrics ; 104(4): e52, 1999 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10506277

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: As part of a study of possible effects of early life otitis media on children's development, we attempted to determine whether levels of language and communication skills at 1 and 2 years of age are associated with the cumulative duration of middle ear effusion (MEE) in the first 2 years of life. METHODS: Subjects (N = 2156) were followed at one of eight study sites in the Pittsburgh area. Middle ear status was monitored closely throughout the first 2 years of life. For each child, the cumulative percentage of days with MEE was estimated based on diagnoses at visits and interpolations for intervals between visits. For each child also, 1 or both parents completed the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory-Words and Gestures (CDI-WG) when the child was 1 year of age and the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory-Words and Sentences (CDI-WS) when the child was 2 years of age. RESULTS: Unadjusted correlations between scores on the CDI-WG and percentage of days with MEE in the first year of life were close to zero, and there were no statistically significant negative correlations. Unadjusted correlations between scores on the CDI-WS and the cumulative percentage of days with MEE in year 2 and in years 1 and 2 combined were generally negative and statistically significant, but the magnitudes of those correlations were no higher than 0.09. After adjustment for sociodemographic variables, only the Vocabulary Production Scale of the CDI-WS remained correlated significantly with the percentage of days with MEE, and the percentage of days with MEE accounted for only a negligible percentage of the variance in scores on this scale. CONCLUSIONS: In this diverse sample of children, parent-reported levels of language skills at 1 and 2 years of age were correlated negligibly with the cumulative percentage of days with MEE in the children's first and second years of life. otitis media, otitis media with effusion, language, communication.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje Infantil , Otitis Media con Derrame/complicaciones , Comunicación , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/etiología , Masculino , Estudios Prospectivos
7.
J Speech Hear Res ; 38(4): 864-75, 1995 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7474979

RESUMEN

Two studies were conducted to examine speaking rate following traumatic brain injury (TBI) in childhood and adolescence. Study 1 focused on longitudinal changes in speaking rate in 9 subjects with severe TBI and their age-matched control subjects. Physical measurements of speaking rate (in syllables/sec) were made from spontaneous speech samples obtained from each subject during three sampling sessions over a 13-month period. Although the average speaking rate of the group with TBI was slower than that of the control group at all three sampling sessions, an examination of the data from individual subject pairs revealed markedly slower speaking rates in only 5 of the 9 subjects with TBI at the final sampling session. The perceptual significance of slowed speaking rates in these 5 subjects was confirmed through subjective ratings by naive listeners. In Study 2, the contributions of two potential causes of slowed speaking rate were explored: reduced articulatory speed and increased pausing believed to be associated with linguistic processing difficulties. It is hypothesized that articulatory speed and linguistic processing speed may contribute independently to slowed speaking rates more than 1 year after TBI.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Trastornos del Lenguaje/etiología , Adolescente , Lesiones Encefálicas/fisiopatología , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Trastornos del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Medición de la Producción del Habla
8.
J Child Lang ; 21(2): 257-71, 1994 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7929681

RESUMEN

Charles-Luce & Luce (1990) found smaller phonological similarity neighbourhoods in five- and seven-year-old children's expressive lexicons than in an adult receptive lexicon, a finding they interpreted as evidence that children need not employ fine-grained auditory perceptual analyses in lexical processing. In the present investigation, neighbourhood sizes were calculated for an expressive lexicon derived from two vocabulary lists representative of children aged 1;0 to 3;0 (Rescorla, 1989; Reznick & Goldsmith, 1989). Over 80% of the words in these early lexicons had at least one phonological neighbour; nearly 20% had six or more phonological neighbours. Very young children must have access to reasonably detailed phonological information in order to create and distinguish among such phonologically similar lexical entries.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Fonética , Vocabulario , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Psicolingüística , Percepción del Habla
9.
J Speech Hear Disord ; 55(3): 567-81, 1990 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2381198

RESUMEN

The spontaneous expressive language abilities of 9 severely brain-injured children and adolescents and their age-matched normal controls were examined seven times over a 12-month period following injury. Analysis of conversational language samples revealed a relatively stable pattern of language performance for the normal subjects over this time interval. The brain-injured subjects, as a group, demonstrated improvement on the majority of measures, but only a few reached the level of their control subjects and interindividual variability was considerable. Results suggest that the prognosis for clinically significant improvement in severely brain-injured subjects is good; however, deficits in expressive skills remain apparent up to at least 12 months following injury.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Trastornos del Lenguaje/rehabilitación , Lenguaje , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Trastornos del Lenguaje/etiología , Masculino
10.
J Speech Hear Disord ; 55(3): 582-90, 1990 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2381199

RESUMEN

Video narration is a context for sampling spontaneous expressive language in which the subject produces an on-line description of the events he or she observes on videotape. Video narration offers a means of reducing the variability among language samples from different speakers, or from the same speaker over time, because the number and complexity of events to be coded linguistically is known and constant. This increased consistency facilitates comparisons among samples, as well as enabling certain analyses requiring a transparent relationship between utterances and events. Advantages and limitations of video narration as an adjunct to conversational sampling are described, and comparisons between longitudinal video narration and conversational samples obtained from brain-injured children and their matched normal controls are presented.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Pruebas del Lenguaje/métodos , Grabación de Cinta de Video , Adolescente , Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Trastornos del Lenguaje/etiología
11.
J Speech Hear Disord ; 52(3): 218-22, 1987 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3455444

RESUMEN

In this study, the fast mapping skills of a group of 11 normal children (ages 4:0-5:6) were compared to those of a group of 11 language-impaired children (ages 4:1-5:4) exhibiting expressive syntactic deficits. Fast mapping is a hypothesized process enabling children to create lexical representations for new words after as little as a single exposure. Subjects encountered a nonsense word and its novel object referent. Subsequent tasks probed the amount and kinds of information about the new word that the subjects had entered into memory. Normal and language-impaired subjects did not differ in their ability to infer a connection between the novel word and referent, to comprehend the novel word after a single exposure, and to recall some nonlinguistic information associated with the referent. However, the language-impaired subjects were less successful than the normal subjects in producing the new word, recalling significantly fewer of its three phonemes.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Lenguaje/psicología , Memoria , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lingüística , Masculino , Fonética
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA