Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Transplant Proc ; 47(2): 511-6, 2015 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25769599

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Early lymphocyte recovery after allogeneic hematopoietic stem cell transplantation (HSCT) is related to the prevention of serious infections and the clearing of residual tumor cells. METHODS: We analyzed the absolute lymphocyte count at 20 (D+20) and 30 (D+30) days after HSCT in 100 patients with malignant hematologic diseases and correlated with the risk of transplant-related mortality, overall survival (OS), disease-free survival (DFS), nonrelapsed mortality (NRM), and risk of infection. RESULTS: Patients presenting with lymphocyte counts of <300 × 103/µL on D+30 have a 3.76 times greater risk of death in <100 days. Over a medium follow-up of 20 months OS, DFS, and NRM were similar between the groups. CONCLUSION: In our group of patients delayed lymphocyte recovery after HSCT was a predictor of early death post-HSCT.


Asunto(s)
Trasplante de Células Madre Hematopoyéticas , Leucemia/sangre , Leucemia/terapia , Recuento de Linfocitos , Síndromes Mielodisplásicos/sangre , Síndromes Mielodisplásicos/terapia , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Supervivencia sin Enfermedad , Femenino , Humanos , Leucemia/mortalidad , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Síndromes Mielodisplásicos/mortalidad , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Estudios Retrospectivos , Tasa de Supervivencia , Factores de Tiempo , Trasplante Homólogo , Resultado del Tratamiento , Adulto Joven
2.
Anim Cogn ; 18(4): 911-9, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25800169

RESUMEN

The spontaneous use of stone tools for cracking nuts by tufted capuchin monkeys, now known to be habitual among wild populations in savanna environments, was first described in a semifree group living in the Tietê Ecological Park (SP, Brazil). Nut-cracking at TEP was first observed by our team in 1995 (Ottoni and Mannu in Int J Primatol 22(3):347-358, 2001), and its ontogeny and associated social dynamics, with inexperienced observers highly interested in the activities of proficient individuals, greatly tolerant to scrounging, support hypotheses about social biases on tool-use learning. Here we further analyze the social learning biases, better characterizing: the social context of nut-cracking in which observation by conspecifics occurs, the quality of the nut-cracking behavior itself and whether scrounging may be the motivation behind this behavior. We confirm that the choice of observational targets is an active one; monkeys do not simply observe those who they are socially close to. We investigate social learning strategies, describing how young capuchins choose to observe older, more proficient and dominant individuals during nut-cracking bouts. Monkeys with higher productivity rates were also more frequently targeted by observers, who were tolerated scroungers, further supporting the scrounging hypothesis. Finally, based on changes of the demographic patterns of tool use and observation, we set to retrace data from 14 years of continuous studies. We argue that we have followed the dissemination of the behavior (Transmission Phase) almost from its beginning, when juveniles were the most frequent nutcrackers, to a more common pattern where adults are the most active tool users (Tradition Phase).


Asunto(s)
Cebus/psicología , Nueces , Aprendizaje Social , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta , Factores de Edad , Animales , Conducta de Elección , Conducta Alimentaria , Solución de Problemas , Predominio Social
3.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 138(2): 210-20, 2009 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18785652

RESUMEN

Wild bearded capuchins (Cebus libidinosus, quadrupedal, medium-sized monkeys) crack nuts using large stones. We examined the kinematics and energetics of the nut-cracking action of two adult males and two adult females. From a bipedal stance, the monkeys raised a heavy hammer stone (1.46 and 1.32 kg, from 33 to 77% of their body weight) to an average height of 0.33 m, 60% of body length. Then, they rapidly lowered the stone by flexing the lower extremities and the trunk until the stone contacted the nut. A hit consisting of an upward phase and a downward phase averaged 0.74 s in duration. The upward phase lasted 69% of hit duration. All subjects added discernable energy to the stone in the downward phase. The monkeys exhibited individualized kinematic strategies, similar to those of human weight lifters. Capuchins illustrate that human-like bipedal stance and large body size are unnecessary to break tough objects from a bipedal position. The phenomenon of bipedal nut-cracking by capuchins provides a new comparative reference point for discussions of percussive tool use and bipedality in primates.


Asunto(s)
Cebus/fisiología , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Nueces , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta/fisiología , Animales , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Brasil , Femenino , Masculino , Grabación en Video
4.
Am J Primatol ; 70(9): 884-91, 2008 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18618602

RESUMEN

Habitually, capuchin monkeys access encased hard foods by using their canines and premolars and/or by pounding the food on hard surfaces. Instead, the wild bearded capuchins (Cebus libidinosus) of Boa Vista (Brazil) routinely crack palm fruits with tools. We measured size, weight, structure, and peak-force-at-failure of the four palm fruit species most frequently processed with tools by wild capuchin monkeys living in Boa Vista. Moreover, for each nut species we identify whether peak-force-at-failure was consistently associated with greater weight/volume, endocarp thickness, and structural complexity. The goals of this study were (a) to investigate whether these palm fruits are difficult, or impossible, to access other than with tools and (b) to collect data on the physical properties of palm fruits that are comparable to those available for the nuts cracked open with tools by wild chimpanzees. Results showed that the four nut species differ in terms of peak-force-at-failure and that peak-force-at-failure is positively associated with greater weight (and consequently volume) and apparently with structural complexity (i.e. more kernels and thus more partitions); finally for three out of four nut species shell thickness is also positively associated with greater volume. The finding that the nuts exploited by capuchins with tools have very high resistance values support the idea that tool use is indeed mandatory to crack them open. Finally, the peak-force-at-failure of the piassava nuts is similar to that reported for the very tough panda nuts cracked open by wild chimpanzees; this highlights the ecological importance of tool use for exploiting high resistance foods in this capuchin species.


Asunto(s)
Arecaceae/fisiología , Cebus/fisiología , Frutas/fisiología , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Masculino
5.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 132(3): 426-44, 2007 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17177182

RESUMEN

Capuchins living in Boa Vista (Piauì, Brazil) crack open hard palm nuts on hard, level surfaces (anvils) using stones (hammers) as percussive tools. This activity leaves diagnostic physical remains: distinctive shallow depressions (pits) on the surface of the anvil, cracked shells, and stone hammers on the anvil. To initiate comparison of percussive stone tool use and interpretation of the artifacts it produces across capuchins, chimpanzees, and hominins, we describe a sample of the anvils and hammer stones used by capuchin monkeys at our site. Anvils (boulders and logs) were located predominantly in the transition zone between the flat open woodland and ridges, in locations that offered some overhead coverage, and with a tree nearby, but not necessarily near palm trees. Anvils contained shallow, hemispherical pits with smooth interiors. Hammers represent a diverse assemblage of ancient rocks that are much harder than the prevailing sedimentary rock out of which they eroded. Hard stones large enough to serve as hammers were more abundant on the anvils than in the surrounding area, indicating that capuchins transport them to the anvils. Capuchins use hammers weighing on average more than 1 kg, a weight that is equivalent to 25-40% of the average body weight for adult males and females. Our findings indicate that capuchins select stones to use as hammers and transport stones and nuts to anvil sites. Wild capuchins provide a new reference point for interpreting early percussive stone tool use in hominins, and a point of comparison with chimpanzees cracking nuts.


Asunto(s)
Cebus/fisiología , Conducta Alimentaria , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta , Animales , Brasil , Nueces
6.
Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput ; 32(3): 446-9, 2000 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11029818

RESUMEN

EthoLog is a tool that aids in the transcription and timing of behavior observation sessions--experimental or naturalistic, from video/audio tapes or registering real time. It was created with Visual Basic and runs on Windows (3.x/9x). The user types the key codes for the predefined behavioral categories, and EthoLog registers their sequence and timing and saves the resulting data in ASCII output files. A sequential analysis matrix can be generated from the sequential data. The output files may be edited, converted to plain text files for printing, or exported to a spreadsheet program, such as MS Excel, for further analyses.


Asunto(s)
Conducta , Sistemas de Registros Médicos Computarizados , Observación , Psicología/métodos , Humanos , Factores de Tiempo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...