Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Neuroimage ; 221: 117122, 2020 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32634596

RESUMEN

Structural neural network architecture patterns in the human brain could be related to individual differences in phenotype, behavior, genetic determinants, and clinical outcomes from neuropsychiatric disorders. Recent studies have indicated that a personalized neural (brain) fingerprint can be identified from structural brain connectomes. However, the accuracy, reproducibility and translational potential of personalized fingerprints in terms of cognition is not yet fully determined. In this study, we introduce a dynamic connectome modeling approach to identify a critical set of white matter subnetworks that can be used as a personalized fingerprint. Several individual variable assessments were performed that demonstrate the accuracy and practicality of personalized fingerprint, specifically predicting the identity and IQ of middle age adults, and the developmental quotient in toddlers. Our findings suggest the fingerprint found by our dynamic modeling approach is sufficient for differentiation between individuals, and is also capable of predicting general intellectual ability across human development.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Desarrollo Humano/fisiología , Inteligencia/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Aprendizaje Automático , Red Nerviosa/anatomía & histología , Neuroimagen , Sustancia Blanca/anatomía & histología , Adulto , Anciano , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Red Nerviosa/diagnóstico por imagen , Sustancia Blanca/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
2.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 34(10): 2715-23, 2013 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22522937

RESUMEN

Sentence processing problems form a common consequence of left-hemisphere brain injury, in some patients to such an extent that their pattern of language performance is characterized as "agrammatic". However, the location of left-hemisphere damage that causes such problems remains controversial. It has been suggested that the critical site for syntactic processing is Broca's area of the frontal cortex or, alternatively, that a more widely distributed network is responsible for syntactic processing. The aim of this study was to identify brain regions that are required for successful sentence processing. Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) was used to identify brain regions where injury predicted impaired sentence processing in 50 native speakers of Icelandic with left-hemisphere stroke. Sentence processing was assessed by having individuals identify which picture corresponded to a verbally presented sentence. The VLSM analysis revealed that impaired sentence processing was best predicted by damage to a large left-hemisphere temporo-parieto-occipital area. This is likely due to the multimodal nature of the sentence processing task, which involves auditory and visual analysis, as well as lexical and syntactic processing. Specifically impaired processing of noncanonical sentence types, when compared with canonical sentence processing, was associated with damage to the left-hemisphere anterior superior and middle temporal gyri and the temporal pole. Anterior temporal cortex, therefore, appears to play a crucial role in syntactic processing, and patients with brain damage to this area are more likely to present with receptive agrammatism than patients in which anterior temporal cortex is spared.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Percepción Auditiva/fisiopatología , Mapeo Encefálico , Comprensión/fisiología , Trastornos del Lenguaje/fisiopatología , Lingüística , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Accidente Cerebrovascular/fisiopatología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Trastornos de la Percepción Auditiva/patología , Imagen de Difusión por Resonancia Magnética , Femenino , Humanos , Islandia , Trastornos del Lenguaje/etiología , Trastornos del Lenguaje/patología , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Lóbulo Occipital/patología , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiopatología , Lóbulo Parietal/patología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiopatología , Desempeño Psicomotor , Accidente Cerebrovascular/patología , Accidente Cerebrovascular/psicología , Lóbulo Temporal/patología
3.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 78(3): 286-94, 2007 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17012334

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Structural neuroimaging studies have consistently shown a pattern of extra-hippocampal atrophy in patients with left and right drug-refractory medial temporal lobe epilepsy (MTLE). However, it is not yet completely understood how extra-hippocampal atrophy is related to hippocampal atrophy. Moreover, patients with left MTLE often exhibit more intense cognitive impairment, and subtle brain asymmetries have been reported in patients with left MTLE versus right MTLE but have not been explored in a controlled study. OBJECTIVES: To investigate the association between extra-hippocampal and hippocampal atrophy in patients with MTLE, and the effect of side of hippocampal atrophy on extra-hippocampal atrophy. METHODS: Voxel-based morphometry analyses of magnetic resonance images of the brain were performed to determine the correlation between regional extra-hippocampal grey matter volume and hippocampal grey matter volume. The results from 36 patients with right and left MTLE were compared, and results from the two groups were compared with those from 49 healthy controls. RESULTS: Compared with controls, patients with MTLE showed a more intense correlation between hippocampal grey matter volume and regional grey matter volume in locations such as the contralateral hippocampus, bilateral parahippocampal gyri and frontal and parietal areas. Compared with right MTLE, patients with left MTLE exhibited a wider area of atrophy related to hippocampal grey matter loss, encompassing both the contralateral and ipsilateral hemispheres, particularly affecting the contralateral hippocampus. CONCLUSIONS: Our results suggest that left hippocampal atrophy is associated with a larger degree of extra-hippocampal atrophy. This may help to explain the more intense cognitive impairment usually observed in these patients.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/patología , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/patología , Hipocampo/patología , Adolescente , Adulto , Atrofia , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Trastornos del Conocimiento/etiología , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/complicaciones , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
4.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 75(9): 1356-8, 2004 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15314135

RESUMEN

Visual neglect is a relatively common deficit after brain damage, particularly strokes. Cancellation tests provide standard clinical measures of neglect severity and deficits in daily life. A recent single-case study introduced a new variation on standard cancellation. Instead of making a visible mark on each target found, the patient made invisible marks (recorded with carbon paper underneath, for later scoring). Such invisible cancellation was found to reveal more neglect than cancellation with visible marks. Here we test the generality of this. Twenty three successive cases with suspected neglect each performed cancellation with visible or invisible marks. Neglect of contralesional targets was more pronounced with invisible marks. Indeed, about half of the patients only showed neglect in this version. For cases showing more neglect with invisible marks, stronger neglect of contralesional targets correlated with more revisits to ipsilesional targets for making additional invisible marks upon them. These results indicate that cancellation with invisible marks can reveal more neglect than standard cancellation with visible marks, while still providing a practical bedside test. Our observations may be consistent with recent proposals that demands on spatial working memory (required to keep track of previously found items only when marked invisibly) can exacerbate spatial neglect.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Memoria , Trastornos de la Percepción/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Percepción/fisiopatología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Psicometría , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Percepción Visual
5.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 74(12): 1627-30, 2003 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14638879

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The objective of this study was to assess the volumes of medial temporal lobe structures using high resolution magnetic resonance images from patients with chronic refractory medial temporal lobe epilepsy (MTLE). METHODS: We studied 30 healthy subjects, and 25 patients with drug refractory MTLE and unilateral hippocampal atrophy (HA). We used T1 magnetic resonance images with 1 mm isotropic voxels, and applied a field non-homogeneity correction and a linear stereotaxic transformation into a standard space. The structures of interest are the entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex, parahippocampal cortex, temporopolar cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala. Structures were identified by visual examination of the coronal, sagittal, and axial planes. The threshold of statistical significance was set to p<0.05. RESULTS: Patients with right and left MTLE showed a reduction in volume of the entorhinal (p<0.001) and perirhinal (p<0.01) cortices ipsilateral to the HA, compared with normal controls. Patients with right MTLE exhibited a significant asymmetry of all studied structures; the right hemisphere structures had smaller volume than their left side counterparts. We did not observe linear correlations between the volumes of different structures of the medial temporal lobe in patients with MTLE. CONCLUSION: Patients with refractory MTLE have damage in the temporal lobe that extends beyond the hippocampus, and affects the regions with close anatomical and functional connections to the hippocampus.


Asunto(s)
Amígdala del Cerebelo/patología , Corteza Entorrinal/patología , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/patología , Hipocampo/patología , Lóbulo Temporal/patología , Adulto , Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiopatología , Atrofia/complicaciones , Atrofia/patología , Atrofia/fisiopatología , Enfermedad Crónica , Corteza Entorrinal/fisiopatología , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/complicaciones , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología , Femenino , Hipocampo/fisiopatología , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología
6.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 15(6): 785-99, 2003 Aug 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14511532

RESUMEN

We investigated the extent to which a common neural mechanism is involved in task set-switching and response withholding, factors that are frequently confounded in task-switching and go/no-go paradigms. Subjects' brain activity was measured using event-related electrical potentials (ERPs) and event-related functional MRI (fMRI) neuroimaging in separate studies using the same cognitive paradigm. Subjects made compatible left/right keypress responses to left/right arrow stimuli of 1000 msec duration; they switched every two trials between responding at stimulus onset (GO task-green arrows) and stimulus offset (WAIT task-red arrows). With-holding an immediate response (WAIT vs. GO) elicited an enhancement of the frontal N2 ERP and lateral PFC activation of the right hemisphere, both previously associated with the "no-go" response, but only on switch trials. Task-switching (switch vs. nonswitch) was associated with frontal N2 amplification and right hemisphere ventrolateral PFC activation, but only for the WAIT task. The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) was the only brain region to be activated for both types of task switch, but this activation was located more rostrally for the WAIT than for the GO switch trials. We conclude that the frontal N2 ERP and lateral PFC activation are not markers for withholding an immediate response or switching tasks per se, but are associated with switching into a response-suppression mode. Different regions within the ACC may be involved in two processes integral to task-switching: processing response conflict (rostral ACC) and overcoming prior response suppression (caudal ACC).


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención , Mapeo Encefálico , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Electroencefalografía/instrumentación , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Prefrontal/anatomía & histología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción Visual
7.
Exp Brain Res ; 143(3): 350-8, 2002 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11889513

RESUMEN

Different sensory systems (e.g. proprioception and vision) have a combined influence on the perception of body orientation, but the timescale over which they can be integrated remains unknown. Here we examined how visual information and neck proprioception interact in perception of the "subjective straight ahead" (SSA), as a function of time since initial stimulation. In complete darkness, healthy subjects directed a laser spot to the point felt subjectively to be exactly straight ahead of the trunk. As previously observed, left neck muscle vibration led to a disparity between subjective perception and objective position of the body midline, with SSA misplaced to the left. We found that this displacement was sustained throughout 28 min of continuous proprioceptive stimulation, provided there was no visual input. Moreover, prolonged vibration of neck muscles leads to a continuing disparity between subjective and objective body orientation even after offset of the vibration; the longer the preceding vibration, the more persistent the illusory deviation of body orientation. To examine the role of vision, one group of subjects fixated a central visual target at the start of each block of continuous neck vibration, with SSA then measured at successive intervals in darkness. The illusory deviation of SSA was eliminated whenever visual input was provided, but returned as a linear function of time when visual information was eliminated. These results reveal: the persistent effects of neck proprioception on the SSA, both during and after vibration; the influence of vision; and integration between incoming proprioceptive information and working memory traces of visual information.


Asunto(s)
Vías Aferentes/fisiología , Sistema Nervioso Central/fisiología , Músculos del Cuello/inervación , Orientación/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Ilusiones/fisiología , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Músculos del Cuello/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Estimulación Física , Factores de Tiempo , Vibración/efectos adversos
8.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 13(7): 920-9, 2001 Oct 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11595095

RESUMEN

We examined the effects of chronic unilateral lesions to either the inferior parietal lobe, or to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex including the frontal eye fields (FEFs), upon human visual perception and saccades in temporal-order-judgment (TOJ) tasks. Two visual events were presented on each trial, one in each hemifield at various stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). In the saccade task, patients moved their eyes to whichever stimulus attracted gaze first. In the perceptual-manual task, they pressed a button to indicate which stimulus was perceived first. Frontal patients showed appropriate TOJs for visual targets in both tasks. Parietal patients showed appropriate TOJs in the perceptual-manual but not the saccade task; their saccades tended to be ipsilesional unless the contralesional target led substantially. This reveals a bias in saccade choice after parietal damage that cannot be attributed to deficient visual perception. These results challenge previous claims that only anterior lesions produce motoric spatial biases in humans. However, they are in accord with recent neurophysiological evidence for parietal involvement in saccade generation, and also with suggestions that visuomotor transformations in the parietal lobe serving direct spatial motor responses can dissociate from conscious perception as indicated by indirect arbitrary responses.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Afasia/psicología , Encefalopatías/psicología , Femenino , Hemiplejía/psicología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiología , Paresia/psicología , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología
9.
Neuroimage ; 14(2): 486-500, 2001 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11467921

RESUMEN

In studies of patients with focal brain lesions, it is often useful to coregister an image of the patient's brain to that of another subject or a standard template. We refer to this process as spatial normalization. Spatial normalization can improve the presentation and analysis of lesion location in neuropsychological studies; it can also allow other data, for example from functional imaging, to be compared to data from other patients or normal controls. In functional imaging, the standard procedure for spatial normalization is to use an automated algorithm, which minimizes a measure of difference between image and template, based on image intensity values. These algorithms usually optimize both linear (translations, rotations, zooms, and shears) and nonlinear transforms. In the presence of a focal lesion, automated algorithms attempt to reduce image mismatch between template and image at the site of the lesion. This can lead to significant inappropriate image distortion, especially when nonlinear transforms are used. One solution is to use cost-function masking-masking the areas used in the calculation of image difference-to exclude the area of the lesion, so that the lesion does not bias the transformations. We introduce and evaluate this technique using normalizations of a selection of brains with focal lesions and normal brains with simulated lesions. Our results suggest that cost-function masking is superior to the standard approach to this problem, which is affine-only normalization; we propose that cost-function masking should be used routinely for normalizations of brains with focal lesions.


Asunto(s)
Algoritmos , Encefalopatías/diagnóstico , Corteza Cerebral/patología , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Adulto , Anciano , Artefactos , Neoplasias Encefálicas/diagnóstico , Neoplasias Encefálicas/secundario , Infarto Cerebral/diagnóstico , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Aneurisma Intracraneal/diagnóstico , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Valores de Referencia
10.
Exp Brain Res ; 137(3-4): 487-96, 2001 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11355393

RESUMEN

Research on visual attention has demonstrated that covert attention can be focused on particular locations within one hemifield, but that a specific "meridian" cost may also be found for shifting attention between hemifields. These issues have received less consideration for audition, even though reliable behavioral measures for the effects of spatial attention on hearing are now available. We examined the spatial distribution of covert attention in an auditory task following spatially non-predictive peripheral auditory cues (which should induce exogenous attention shifts), or following symbolic central cues that predicted the likely location for the auditory target (to induce endogenous attention shifts). In both cases, we found that attention can be focused not only on one hemifield versus another, but also within one hemifield in an auditory task. However, there was no unequivocal evidence for a meridian effect in audition.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA