Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 14 de 14
Filtrar
1.
J Vis ; 23(7): 7, 2023 07 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37428485

RESUMEN

We actively seek information from the environment through saccadic eye movements, necessitating continual integration of presaccadic and postsaccadic signals, which are displaced on the retina by each saccade. We tested whether trans-saccadic integration may be related to serial dependence (a measure of how perceptual history influences current perception) by measuring how viewing a presaccadic stimulus affects the perceived orientation of a subsequent test stimulus presented around the time of a saccade. Participants reproduced the position, and orientation of a test stimulus presented around a 16° saccade. The reproduced position was mislocalized toward the saccadic target, agreeing with previous work. The reproduced orientation was attracted toward the prior stimulus and regressed to the mean orientation. These results suggest that both short- and long-term past information affects trans-saccadic perception, most strongly when the test stimulus is presented perisaccadically. This study unites the fields of serial dependence and trans-saccadic perception, leading to potential new insights of how information is transferred and accumulated across saccades.


Asunto(s)
Juicio , Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Retina
2.
Psychiatry Investig ; 19(7): 527-537, 2022 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35903055

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Involuntary admission to psychiatric inpatient care can protect both patients with severe mental illnesses and individuals around them. This study analyzed annual healthcare costs per person for involuntary psychiatric admission and examined categories of mental disorders and other factors associated with mortality. METHODS: This retrospective cohort study collected 1 million randomly sampled beneficiaries from the National Health Insurance Database for 2002-2013. It identified and matched 181 patients with involuntary psychiatric admissions (research group) with 724 patients with voluntary psychiatric admissions (control group) through 1:4 propensity-score matching for sex, age, comorbidities, mental disorder category, and index year of diagnosis. RESULTS: Mean life expectancy of patients with involuntary psychiatric admissions was 33.13 years less than the general population. Average annual healthcare costs per person for involuntary psychiatric admissions were 3.94 times higher compared with voluntary admissions. The general linear model demonstrated that average annual medical costs per person per compulsory hospitalization were 5.8 times that of voluntary hospitalization. Survival analysis using the Cox proportional hazards model found no significant association between type of psychiatric admission (involuntary or voluntary) and death. CONCLUSION: This study revealed no significant difference in mortality between involuntary and voluntary psychiatric admissions, indicating involuntary treatment's effectiveness.

3.
Vision Res ; 175: 51-57, 2020 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32707416

RESUMEN

One interesting observation of perceptual learning is the asymmetric transfer between stimuli at different external noise levels: learning at zero/low noise can transfer significantly to the same stimulus at high noise, but not vice versa. The mechanisms underlying this asymmetric transfer have been investigated by psychophysical, neurophysiological, brain imaging, and computational modeling studies. One study (PNAS 113 (2016) 5724-5729) reported that rTMS stimulations of dorsal and ventral areas impair motion direction discrimination of moving dot stimuli at 40% coherent ("noisy") and 100% coherent (zero-noise) levels, respectively. However, after direction training at 100% coherence, only rTMS stimulation of the ventral cortex is effective, disturbing direction discrimination at both coherence levels. These results were interpreted as learning-induced changes of functional specializations of visual areas. We have concerns with the behavioral data of this study. First, contrary to the report of highly location-specific motion direction learning, our replicating experiment showed substantial learning transfer (e.g., transfer/learning ratio = 81.9%. vs 14.8% at 100% coherence). Second and more importantly, we found complete transfer of direction learning from 40% to 100% coherence, a critical baseline that is missing in this study. The transfer effect suggests that similar brain mechanisms underlie motion direction processing at two coherence levels. Therefore, this study's conclusions regarding the roles of dorsal and ventral areas in motion direction processing at two coherence levels, as well as the effects of perceptual learning, are not supported by proper experimental evidence. It remains unexplained why distinct impacts of dorsal and ventral rTMS stimulations on motion direction discrimination were observed.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Encéfalo , Corteza Cerebral , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Humanos , Movimiento (Física) , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología
4.
Vision Res ; 174: 69-76, 2020 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32615458

RESUMEN

Patients with central vision loss depend on peripheral vision for everyday functions. A preferred retinal locus (PRL) on the intact retina is commonly trained as a new "fovea" to help. However, reprogramming the fovea-centered oculomotor control is difficult, so saccades often bring the defunct fovea to block the target. Aligning PRL with distant targets also requires multiple saccades and sometimes head movements. To overcome these problems, we attempted to train normal-sighted observers to form a preferred retinal annulus (PRA) around a simulated scotoma, so that they could rely on the same fovea-centered oculomotor system and make short saccades to align PRA with the target. Observers with an invisible simulated central scotoma (5° radius) practiced making saccades to see a tumbling-E target at 10° eccentricity. The otherwise blurred E target became clear when saccades brought a scotoma-abutting clear window (2° radius) to it. The location of the clear window was either fixed for PRL training, or changing among 12 locations for PRA training. Various cues aided the saccades through training. Practice quickly established a PRL or PRA. Comparing to PRL-trained observers whose first saccade persistently blocked the target with scotoma, PRA-trained observers produced more accurate first saccade. The benefits of more accurate PRA-based saccades also outweighed the costs of slower latency. PRA training may provide an efficient strategy to cope with central vision loss, especially for aging patients who have major difficulties adapting to a PRL.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Sacádicos , Escotoma , Fóvea Central , Humanos , Retina , Percepción Visual
5.
Neuroscience ; 432: 240-246, 2020 04 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32135232

RESUMEN

Protein and miRNA enrichment within extracellular vesicles (EVs) isolated from patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) has been shown to have putative diagnostic value. However, whether a combination of both will be more advantageous is unknown. EVs were enriched from serum samples obtained from patients with sporadic AD (n = 13), mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (n = 10), vascular dementia (VaD) (n = 10), and healthy controls (HC) (n = 10). Expression of protein levels of beta-amyloid peptide (Aß1-42), total tau, P-T181-tau, and P-S396-tau and 18 microRNAs (miRNAs) in the EVs was performed by ELISA and qRT-PCR, respectively. Results were validated in an independent cohort of 18 subjects each by qRT-PCR assays. EV protein expression of Aß1-42, total-tau, P-T181-tau and P-S396-tau, were significantly different among AD, MCI and VaD. Hsa-miR-1306-5p, hsa-miR-342-3p, and hsa-15b-3p were all significantly downregulated in patients with AD compared to HC (P < 0.05), only hsa-miR-1306-5p expression was differentially expressed between AD, MCI, and VaD samples. Similarly, whereas all 14 miRNAs were significantly upregulated in patients with AD compared to HC, only hsa-miR-93-5p, hsa-miR-424-5p, and hsa-miR-3065-5p were differentially expressed when AD samples were compared to MCI and VaD samples. Even though the sample size was small, the results of the current pilot study indicates that hsa-miR-1306-5p, hsa-miR-93-5p, hsa-miR-424-5p, and hsa-miR-3065-5p, and expression of P-S396-tau in EVs might provide a combinatorial protein and miRNA signature to differentiate between HC, patients with MCI or VaD from patient with sporadic AD.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Vesículas Extracelulares , MicroARNs , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/genética , Péptidos beta-Amiloides , Humanos , Proyectos Piloto
6.
J Vis ; 20(2): 5, 2020 02 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32097482

RESUMEN

Perceptual learning, which improves stimulus discrimination, typically results from training with a single stimulus condition. Two major learning mechanisms, early cortical neural plasticity and response reweighting, have been proposed. Here we report a new format of perceptual learning that by design may have bypassed these mechanisms. Instead, it is more likely based on abstracted stimulus evidence from multiple stimulus conditions. Specifically, we had observers practice orientation discrimination with Gabors or symmetric dot patterns at up to 47 random or rotating location × orientation conditions. Although each condition received sparse trials (12 trials/session), the practice produced significant orientation learning. Learning also transferred to a Gabor at a single untrained condition with two- to three-times lower orientation thresholds. Moreover, practicing a single stimulus condition with matched trial frequency (12 trials/session) failed to produce significant learning. These results suggest that learning with multiple stimulus conditions may not come from early cortical plasticity or response reweighting with each particular condition. Rather, it may materialize through a new format of perceptual learning, in which orientation evidence invariant to particular orientations and locations is first abstracted from multiple stimulus conditions and then reweighted by later learning mechanisms. The coarse-to-fine transfer of orientation learning from multiple Gabors or symmetric dot patterns to a single Gabor also suggest the involvement of orientation concept learning by the learning mechanisms.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Orientación Espacial , Adulto Joven
7.
Nat Prod Res ; 34(21): 3066-3072, 2020 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31076005

RESUMEN

One new α-ditetralonyl glucoside (1), was isolated from the green walnut husk of Juglans mandshurica (Juglandaceae), together with twelve known compounds (2-13). The structure of the new compound was determined as (2R,4S,10S,12S)-2-[7-(12,13,16-trihydroxy-α-tetralonyl-13-O-ß-D-glucopyranoside)]- 4,8-dihydroxy-α-tetralone-4-O-ß-D-glucopyranoside (1), on the basis of detailed spectroscopic analyses, and acidic hydrolysis. Compounds 6, 7 and 11 were isolated from the genus Juglans for the first time. Compound 1-13 showed weak cytotoxic against A549 and HeLa cell lines.


Asunto(s)
Antineoplásicos Fitogénicos/química , Antineoplásicos Fitogénicos/farmacología , Juglans/química , Células A549 , Ensayos de Selección de Medicamentos Antitumorales , Glucósidos/química , Células HeLa , Humanos , Espectroscopía de Resonancia Magnética , Estructura Molecular , Nueces/química , Extractos Vegetales/química , Espectrometría de Masa por Ionización de Electrospray
8.
Vision Res ; 156: 39-45, 2019 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30684500

RESUMEN

Perceptual learning is often interpreted as learning of fine stimulus templates. However, we have proposed that perceptual learning is more than template learning, in that more abstract statistical rules may have been learned, so that learning can transfer to stimuli at different precisions. Here we provide new evidence to support this view: Perceptual learning of Vernier discrimination at high noise, which has thresholds approximately 10 times as much as those at zero noise, is initially non-transferrable to zero noise. However, additional exposure to a noise-free Vernier-forming Gabor, which is ineffective alone, not only maximizes zero-noise fine Vernier discrimination, but also further enhances high-noise Vernier performance. Such high-threshold coarse Vernier training cannot impact the fine stimulus template directly. One plausible explanation is that the observers have learned the statistical rules that can apply to standardized input distributions to improve discrimination, regardless of the original precision of these distributions.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Ruido , Psicofísica , Umbral Sensorial , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología , Adulto Joven
9.
Vision Res ; 152: 3-9, 2018 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29262303

RESUMEN

Location specific perceptual learning can transfer to a new location if the new location is trained with a secondary task that by itself does not impact the performance of the primary learning task (double training). Learning may also transfer to other locations when double training is performed at the same location. Here we investigated the mechanisms underlying double-training enabled learning and transfer with an external noise paradigm. Specifically, we measured the Vernier thresholds at various external noise contrasts before and after double training. Double training mainly vertically downshifts the TvC functions at the training and transfer locations, which may be interpreted as improved sampling efficiency in a linear amplifier model or a combination of internal noise reduction and external noise exclusion in a perceptual template model at both locations. The change of the TvC functions appears to be a high-level process that can be remapped from a training location to a new location after double training.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Ruido , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Condicionamiento Psicológico , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicofísica , Umbral Sensorial , Adulto Joven
10.
Bioresour Technol ; 238: 39-47, 2017 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28433916

RESUMEN

Low temperature climate presented a technical challenge to start-up composting in northern region of China. This study investigated if the cold-adapted microbial consortium (CAMC) could promote composting start-up at low temperature. In this work, the CAMC was inoculated when food waste was composted at 10°C. The results showed that inoculating CAMC accelerated the piles temperature effectively, the piles passed through the start-up period within 37h. Moreover, the inoculants could enhance the abundances of dominant strains related to organic matters degradation rate. Redundancy analysis (RDA) indicated that the relationships among indigenous bacteria, organic substrates degradation and temperature evolution were influenced by the inoculants. Furthermore, the heat generation value and degradation rate of the hydrolysable carbohydrate, lipids and protein were significantly enhanced with CAMC inoculated. This work demonstrated that inoculating CAMC was beneficial to composting self-heating, it provided a novel biotechnology support to ensure the normal start-up of winter composting.


Asunto(s)
Frío , Consorcios Microbianos , Eliminación de Residuos , China , Suelo , Temperatura
11.
Infect Dis Poverty ; 5(1): 75, 2016 Aug 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27507163

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Implementing effective interventions remain a lot of difficulties along all border regions. The emergence of artemisinin resistance of Plasmodium falciparum strains in the Greater Mekong Subregion is a matter of great concern. China has effectively controlled cross-border transmission of malaria and artemisinin resistance of P. falciparum along the China-Myanmar border. METHODS: A combined quantitative and qualitative study was used to collect data, and then an integrated impact evaluation was conducted to malaria control along the China-Myanmar border during 2007-2013. RESULTS: The parasite prevalence rate (PPR) in the five special regions of Myanmar was decreased from 13.6 % in March 2008 to 1.5 % in November 2013. Compared with the baseline (PPR in March 2008), the risk ratio was only 0.11 [95 % confidence interval (CI), 0.09-0. 14) in November 2013, which is equal to an 89 % reduction in the malaria burden. Annual parasite incidence (API) across 19 Chinese border counties was reduced from 19.6 per 10 000 person-years in 2006 to 0.9 per 10 000 person-years in 2013. Compared with the baseline (API in 2006), the API rate ratio was only 0.05(95 % CI, 0.04-0.05) in 2013, which equates to a reduction of the malaria burden by 95.0 %. Meanwhile, the health service system was strengthened and health inequity of marginalized populations reduced along the international border. CONCLUSION: The effective collaboration between China, Myanmar and the international non-governmental organization promptly carried out the core interventions through simplified processes. The integrated approaches dramatically decreased malaria burden of Chinese-Myanmar border.


Asunto(s)
Malaria Falciparum/prevención & control , Antimaláricos/farmacología , Antimaláricos/uso terapéutico , China/epidemiología , Humanos , Incidencia , Cooperación Internacional , Malaria Falciparum/tratamiento farmacológico , Malaria Falciparum/epidemiología , Malaria Falciparum/parasitología , Mianmar/epidemiología , Plasmodium falciparum/efectos de los fármacos , Prevalencia
12.
J Neurosci ; 36(7): 2238-46, 2016 Feb 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26888933

RESUMEN

Humans can learn to abstract and conceptualize the shared visual features defining an object category in object learning. Therefore, learning is generalizable to transformations of familiar objects and even to new objects that differ in other physical properties. In contrast, visual perceptual learning (VPL), improvement in discriminating fine differences of a basic visual feature through training, is commonly regarded as specific and low-level learning because the improvement often disappears when the trained stimulus is simply relocated or rotated in the visual field. Such location and orientation specificity is taken as evidence for neural plasticity in primary visual cortex (V1) or improved readout of V1 signals. However, new training methods have shown complete VPL transfer across stimulus locations and orientations, suggesting the involvement of high-level cognitive processes. Here we report that VPL bears similar properties of object learning. Specifically, we found that orientation discrimination learning is completely transferrable between luminance gratings initially encoded in V1 and bilaterally symmetric dot patterns encoded in higher visual cortex. Similarly, motion direction discrimination learning is transferable between first- and second-order motion signals. These results suggest that VPL can take place at a conceptual level and generalize to stimuli with different physical properties. Our findings thus reconcile perceptual and object learning into a unified framework. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: Training in object recognition can produce a learning effect that is applicable to new viewing conditions or even to new objects with different physical properties. However, perceptual learning has long been regarded as a low-level form of learning because of its specificity to the trained stimulus conditions. Here we demonstrate with new training tactics that visual perceptual learning is completely transferrable between distinct physical stimuli. This finding indicates that perceptual learning also operates at a conceptual level in a stimulus-invariant manner.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Percepción/fisiología , Adulto , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Aprendizaje Espacial , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
13.
Vision Res ; 119: 9-15, 2016 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26709811

RESUMEN

Recent studies reported significantly less location specificity in motion direction learning than in previous classical studies. The latter performed training with the method of constant stimuli containing a single level of direction difference. In contrast the former used staircase methods that varied the direction difference trial by trial. We suspect that extensive practice with a single direction difference could allow an observer to use some subtle local cues for direction discrimination. Such local cues may be unavailable at a new stimulus location, leading to higher location specificity. To test this hypothesis, we jittered slightly the directions of a stimulus pair by the same amount while keeping the direction difference constant, so as to disturb the potential local cues. We observed significantly more transfer of learning to untrained locations. The local cue effects may also explain the recent controversies regarding the finding that foveal motion direction learning becomes significantly more transferrable to a new direction with TPE (training-plus-exposure) training. One specific study by Zili Liu and collaborators that challenges this finding also used a single-level direction difference for training. We first replicated their results. But we found that if the directions of the stimulus pair were again jittered while the direction difference was kept constant, motion direction learning transferred significantly more to an orthogonal direction with TPE training. Our results thus demonstrate the importance of using appropriate psychophysical methods in training to reduce local-cue related specificity in perceptual learning.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica
14.
Guang Pu Xue Yu Guang Pu Fen Xi ; 35(10): 2940-5, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26904847

RESUMEN

The characteristics of fluorescence spectra of dissolved organic matter (DOM) derived from composting is one of the key ways to assess the compost maturity. However, the existing methods mainly focus on the qualitative description for the humification degree of compost. In this paper, projection pursuit classification (PPC) was conducted to quantitative assess the grades of compost maturity, based on the characteristics of fluorescence spectra of DOM. Eight organic wastes (chicken manure, swine manure, kitchen waste, lawn waste, fruits and vegetables waste, straw, green waste, and municipal solid waste) composting were conducted, the germination percentage (GI) and fluorescence spectra of DOM were measured during composting. Statistic analysis with all fluorescence parameters of DOM indicated that I436/I383 (a ratio between the fluorescence intensities at 436 and 383 nm in excitation spectra), FLR (an area ratio between fulvic-like region from 308 to 363 nm and total region in emission spectra), P(HA/Pro) (a regional integration ratio between humic acid-like region to protein-like region in excitation emission matrix (EEM) spectra), A4/A1 (an area ratio of the last quarter to the first quarter in emission spectra), r(A,C) (a ratio between the fluorescence intensities of peak A and peak C in EEM spectra) were correlated with each other (p < 0.01), suggesting that this fluorescence parameters could be considered as comprehensive evaluation index system of PPC. Subsequently, the four degrades of compost maturity included the best degree of maturity (I, GI > 80%), better degree of compost maturity (II, 60% < GI < 80%), maturity (III, 50% < GI < 60%), and immaturity (IV, GI < 50%) were divided according the GI value during composting. The corresponding fluorescence parameter values were calculated at each degrade of compost maturity. Then the projection values were calculated based on PPC considering the above fluorescence parameter values. The projection value was 2.01 - 2.22 for I grade, 1.21 - 2.0 for II grade, 0.57 - 1.2 for III grade, and 0.10 - 0.56 for IV grade. Model validation was then carried out with composts samples, the results indicated that the simulated values were agreed with the observed values, and the accuracy of PPC was 75% for four grades of maturity, and 100% for maturity and immaturity, suggesting that PPC could meet the need of the assessment of compost maturity.


Asunto(s)
Fluorescencia , Espectrometría de Fluorescencia , Animales , Sustancias Húmicas , Estiércol , Suelo , Residuos Sólidos , Porcinos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...