Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Explore (NY) ; 17(3): 197-202, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33262082

RESUMO

Meditation has been deemed a miracle cure for a wide range of neurological disorders. However, it is unclear whether meditation practice would be beneficial for patients suffering from epilepsy. Here we report on the comparison of the effects of focused-attention meditation and a control task on electroencephalographic (EEG) activity in a patient undergoing stereoencephalographic (SEEG) investigation for drug-resistant epilepsy. The patient routinely practiced focused-attention meditation and reported that she found it beneficial. During the SEEG investigation, intracerebral EEG data were recorded during meditation as well as during mind-wandering task. The EEG data were analyzed for type of electrical activity (labeled) by two expert epileptologists. We found that the proportion of EEG segments containing activity classified as interictal epileptiform discharges (IEDs; abnormal electrical activity that occurs between seizures) increased significantly during meditation practice. Although the finding was surprising, this increase in IEDs may not correlate with an increase in seizure frequency, and the patient might still benefit from practicing meditation. The finding does, however, warrant further studies on the influence of meditation on epileptic activity.


Assuntos
Epilepsia do Lobo Temporal , Epilepsia , Meditação , Eletroencefalografia , Epilepsia do Lobo Temporal/terapia , Feminino , Humanos , Convulsões/terapia
2.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30104426

RESUMO

Behavioural and cognitive processes play important roles in mediating an individual's interactions with its environment. Yet, while there is a vast literature on repeatable individual differences in behaviour, relatively little is known about the repeatability of cognitive performance. To further our understanding of the evolution of cognition, we gathered 44 studies on individual performance of 25 species across six animal classes and used meta-analysis to assess whether cognitive performance is repeatable. We compared repeatability (R) in performance (1) on the same task presented at different times (temporal repeatability), and (2) on different tasks that measured the same putative cognitive ability (contextual repeatability). We also addressed whether R estimates were influenced by seven extrinsic factors (moderators): type of cognitive performance measurement, type of cognitive task, delay between tests, origin of the subjects, experimental context, taxonomic class and publication status. We found support for both temporal and contextual repeatability of cognitive performance, with mean R estimates ranging between 0.15 and 0.28. Repeatability estimates were mostly influenced by the type of cognitive performance measures and publication status. Our findings highlight the widespread occurrence of consistent inter-individual variation in cognition across a range of taxa which, like behaviour, may be associated with fitness outcomes.This article is part of the theme issue 'Causes and consequences of individual differences in cognitive abilities'.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Variação Biológica Individual , Cognição , Animais
3.
Cognition ; 158: 33-43, 2017 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27776224

RESUMO

Verifying that a face is from a target person (e.g. finding someone in the crowd) is a critical ability of the human face processing system. Yet how fast this can be performed is unknown. The 'entry-level shift due to expertise' hypothesis suggests that - since humans are face experts - processing faces should be as fast - or even faster - at the individual than at superordinate levels. In contrast, the 'superordinate advantage' hypothesis suggests that faces are processed from coarse to fine, so that the opposite pattern should be observed. To clarify this debate, three different face processing levels were compared: (1) a superordinate face categorization level (i.e. detecting human faces among animal faces), (2) a face familiarity level (i.e. recognizing famous faces among unfamiliar ones) and (3) verifying that a face is from a target person, our condition of interest. The minimal speed at which faces can be categorized (∼260ms) or recognized as familiar (∼360ms) has largely been documented in previous studies, and thus provides boundaries to compare our condition of interest to. Twenty-seven participants were included. The recent Speed and Accuracy Boosting procedure paradigm (SAB) was used since it constrains participants to use their fastest strategy. Stimuli were presented either upright or inverted. Results revealed that verifying that a face is from a target person (minimal RT at ∼260ms) was remarkably fast but longer than the face categorization level (∼240ms) and was more sensitive to face inversion. In contrast, it was much faster than recognizing a face as familiar (∼380ms), a level severely affected by face inversion. Face recognition corresponding to finding a specific person in a crowd thus appears achievable in only a quarter of a second. In favor of the 'superordinate advantage' hypothesis or coarse-to-fine account of the face visual hierarchy, these results suggest a graded engagement of the face processing system across processing levels as reflected by the face inversion effects. Furthermore, they underline how verifying that a face is from a target person and detecting a face as familiar - both often referred to as "Face Recognition" - in fact differs.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Facial , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
4.
Neuropsychologia ; 75: 390-401, 2015 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26100560

RESUMO

Rapidly recognizing familiar people from their faces appears critical for social interactions (e.g., to differentiate friend from foe). However, the actual speed at which the human brain can distinguish familiar from unknown faces still remains debated. In particular, it is not clear whether familiarity can be extracted from rapid face individualization or if it requires additional time consuming processing. We recorded scalp EEG activity in 28 subjects performing a go/no-go, famous/non-famous, unrepeated, face recognition task. Speed constraints were used to encourage subjects to use the earliest familiarity information available. Event related potential (ERP) analyses show that both the N170 and the N250 components were modulated by familiarity. The N170 modulation was related to behaviour: subjects presenting the strongest N170 modulation were also faster but less accurate than those who only showed weak N170 modulation. A complementary Multi-Variate Pattern Analysis (MVPA) confirmed ERP results and provided some more insights into the dynamics of face recognition as the N170 differential effect appeared to be related to a first transitory phase (transitory bump of decoding power) starting at around 140 ms, which returned to baseline afterwards. This bump of activity was henceforth followed by an increase of decoding power starting around 200 ms after stimulus onset. Overall, our results suggest that rather than a simple single-process, familiarity for faces may rely on a cascade of neural processes, including a coarse and fast stage starting at 140 ms and a more refined but slower stage occurring after 200 ms.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados Visuais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
5.
Seizure ; 23(2): 122-8, 2014 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24252808

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The Wada test is still the gold standard procedure to predict language and memory deficits before temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. As amobarbital was no longer available, our aim was to validate propofol as an alternative. METHOD: We retrospectively studied 47 patients who underwent a bilateral intracarotid procedure, performed with amobarbital (18), or propofol (29), between 2000 and 2010 during the preoperative evaluation of temporal lobe epilepsy. RESULTS: The number of patients experiencing an adverse event (mostly transient disturbance of consciousness or benign ocular symptoms) during both injections did not differ significantly between amobarbital and propofol. Hemispheric dominance was successfully determined in 96.5% patients with propofol vs. 94.4% with amobarbital for language, and in 72.4% under propofol vs. 77.7% under amobarbital for memory with no significant difference between groups. CONCLUSION: Propofol can be used for the Wada test with an efficacy and safety comparable to amobarbital.


Assuntos
Amobarbital , Anestésicos , Epilepsia do Lobo Temporal/fisiopatologia , Epilepsia do Lobo Temporal/cirurgia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Propofol , Adolescente , Adulto , Amobarbital/efeitos adversos , Anestésicos/efeitos adversos , Encéfalo/efeitos dos fármacos , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Estado de Consciência/efeitos dos fármacos , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Memória/efeitos dos fármacos , Memória/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Cuidados Pré-Operatórios/efeitos adversos , Cuidados Pré-Operatórios/métodos , Propofol/efeitos adversos , Estudos Retrospectivos , Percepção Visual/efeitos dos fármacos , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...