Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 11 de 11
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Nature ; 581(7806): 42-46, 2020 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32376960

RESUMO

State-of-the-art optical clocks1 achieve precisions of 10-18 or better using ensembles of atoms in optical lattices2,3 or individual ions in radio-frequency traps4,5. Promising candidates for use in atomic clocks are highly charged ions6 (HCIs) and nuclear transitions7, which are largely insensitive to external perturbations and reach wavelengths beyond the optical range8 that are accessible to frequency combs9. However, insufficiently accurate atomic structure calculations hinder the identification of suitable transitions in HCIs. Here we report the observation of a long-lived metastable electronic state in an HCI by measuring the mass difference between the ground and excited states in rhenium, providing a non-destructive, direct determination of an electronic excitation energy. The result is in agreement with advanced calculations. We use the high-precision Penning trap mass spectrometer PENTATRAP to measure the cyclotron frequency ratio of the ground state to the metastable state of the ion with a precision of 10-11-an improvement by a factor of ten compared with previous measurements10,11. With a lifetime of about 130 days, the potential soft-X-ray frequency reference at 4.96 × 1016 hertz (corresponding to a transition energy of 202 electronvolts) has a linewidth of only 5 × 10-8 hertz and one of the highest electronic quality factors (1024) measured experimentally so far. The low uncertainty of our method will enable searches for further soft-X-ray clock transitions8,12 in HCIs, which are required for precision studies of fundamental physics6.

2.
Cogn Neurosci ; 6(1): 1-7, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25185800

RESUMO

Gilles de la Tourette syndrome (GTS) is characterized by motor and phonic tics. It is unknown how paying attention to one's own tics might modulate tic frequency. We determined tic frequency in freely ticcing GTS patients while they were being filmed. In Study 1, we investigated 12 patients (1) alone in a room (baseline); (2) alone in front of a mirror. In Study 2, we replicated these conditions in 16 patients and additionally examined how watching a video, in which the individual was shown not ticcing, affected their tic frequency. In both studies, tic frequency was significantly higher when patients watched themselves in a mirror compared to baseline. In contrast, tic frequency was significantly reduced in the video condition. Paying attention to one's own tics increases tic frequency when tics are not suppressed and appears to be specific for attention to tics, rather than attention to the self.


Assuntos
Estimulação Luminosa , Tiques/fisiopatologia , Síndrome de Tourette/fisiopatologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Atenção , Retroalimentação Sensorial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Autoimagem , Adulto Jovem
3.
Cognition ; 134: 50-6, 2015 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25460378

RESUMO

Space perception depends on our motion potentialities and our intended actions are affected by space perception. Research on peripersonal space (the space in reaching distance) shows that we perceive an object as being closer when we (Witt, Proffitt, & Epstein, 2005; Witt & Proffitt, 2008) or another actor (Costantini, Ambrosini, Sinigaglia, & Gallese, 2011; Bloesch, Davoli, Roth, Brockmole, & Abrams, 2012) can interact with it. Similarly, an object only triggers specific movements when it is placed in our peripersonal space (Costantini, Ambrosini, Tieri, Sinigaglia, & Committeri, 2010) or in the other's peripersonal space (Costantini, Committeri, & Sinigaglia, 2011; Cardellicchio, Sinigaglia, & Costantini, 2013). Moreover, also the extrapersonal space (the space outside reaching distance) seems to be perceived in relation to our movement capabilities: the more effort it takes to cover a distance, the greater we perceive the distance to be (Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, & Epstein, 2003; Sugovic & Witt, 2013). However, not much is known about the influence of the other's movement potentialities on our extrapersonal space perception. Three experiments were carried out investigating the categorization of distance in extrapersonal space using human or non-human allocentric reference frames (RF). Subjects were asked to judge the distance ("Near" or "Far") of a target object (a beach umbrella) placed at progressively increasing or decreasing distances until a change from near to far or vice versa was reported. In the first experiment we found a significant "Near space extension" when the allocentric RF was a human virtual agent instead of a static, inanimate object. In the second experiment we tested whether the "Near space extension" depended on the anatomical structure of the RF or its movement potentialities by adding a wooden dummy. The "Near space extension" was only observed for the human agent but not for the dummy. Finally, to rule out the possibility that the effect was simply due to a line-of-sight mechanism (visual perspective taking) we compared the human agent free to move with the same agent tied to a pole with a rope, thus reducing movement potentialities while maintaining equal visual accessibility. The "Near space extension" disappeared when this manipulation was introduced, showing that movement potentialities are the relevant factor for such an effect. Our results demonstrate for the first time that during allocentric distance judgments within extrapersonal space, we implicitly process the movement potentialities of the RF. A target object is perceived as being closer when the allocentric RF is a human with available movement potentialities, suggesting a mechanism of social scaling of extrapersonal space processing.


Assuntos
Percepção de Distância/fisiologia , Movimento , Espaço Pessoal , Percepção Social , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
4.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 7: 265, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23781187

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: This study aimed at developing an experimental paradigm to assess vicarious pain experiences. We further explored the putative moderating role of observer's characteristics such as hypervigilance for pain and dispositional empathy. METHODS: Two experiments are reported using a similar procedure. Undergraduate students were selected based upon whether they reported vicarious pain in daily life, and categorized into a pain responder group or a comparison group. Participants were presented a series of videos showing hands being pricked whilst receiving occasionally pricking (electrocutaneous) stimuli themselves. In congruent trials, pricking and visual stimuli were applied to the same spatial location. In incongruent trials, pricking and visual stimuli were in the opposite spatial location. Participants were required to report on which location they felt a pricking sensation. Of primary interest was the effect of viewing another in pain upon vicarious pain errors, i.e., the number of trials in which an illusionary sensation was reported. Furthermore, we explored the effect of individual differences in hypervigilance to pain, dispositional empathy and the rubber hand illusion (RHI) upon vicarious pain errors. RESULTS: RESULTS of both experiments indicated that the number of vicarious pain errors was overall low. In line with expectations, the number of vicarious pain errors was higher in the pain responder group than in the comparison group. Self-reported hypervigilance for pain lowered the probability of reporting vicarious pain errors in the pain responder group, but dispositional empathy and the RHI did not. CONCLUSION: Our paradigm allows measuring vicarious pain experiences in students. However, the prevalence of vicarious experiences of pain is low, and only a small percentage of participants display the phenomenon. It remains however unknown which variables affect its occurrence.

5.
Neuropsychologia ; 48(2): 619-26, 2010 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19883667

RESUMO

An important aspect of volition is the internal decision whether to act or to withhold an action. We used EEG frequency analysis of sensorimotor rhythms to investigate brain activity when people prepare and then cancel a voluntary action. Participants used a rotating clock-hand to report when they experienced the intention to press a key with their right hand, even on trials where they freely decided to inhibit movement at the last moment. On action trials, we observed the classical pattern of reduced beta-band spectral power prior to movement, followed by beta rebound after movement. On inhibition trials where participants prepared but then cancelled a movement, we found a left frontal increase in spectral power (event-related synchronisation: ERS) peaking 12 ms before the perceived intention to move. This neural correlate of intentional inhibition was significantly different from the activity at the corresponding moment in action trials. The results are discussed in the context of a recent model of voluntary action (WWW model; Brass & Haggard, 2008). Planned actions can be subjected to a final predictive check which either commits actions for execution or suspends and withholds them. The neural mechanism of intentional inhibition may play an important role in self-control.


Assuntos
Fenômenos Eletrofisiológicos/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Intenção , Movimento/fisiologia , Volição/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Análise Espectral , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 98(24): 13995-9, 2001 Nov 20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11717457

RESUMO

Imitation is a complex phenomenon, the neural mechanisms of which are still largely unknown. When individuals imitate an action that already is present in their motor repertoire, a mechanism matching the observed action onto an internal motor representation of that action should suffice for the purpose. When one has to copy a new action, however, or to adjust an action present in one's motor repertoire to a different observed action, an additional mechanism is needed that allows the observer to compare the action made by another individual with the sensory consequences of the same action made by himself. Previous experiments have shown that a mechanism that directly matches observed actions on their motor counterparts exists in the premotor cortex of monkeys and humans. Here we report the results of functional magnetic resonance experiments, suggesting that in the superior temporal sulcus, a higher order visual region, there is a sector that becomes active both during hand action observation and during imitation even in the absence of direct vision of the imitator's hand. The motor-related activity is greater during imitation than during control motor tasks. This newly identified region has all the requisites for being the region at which the observed actions, and the reafferent motor-related copies of actions made by the imitator, interact.


Assuntos
Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Feminino , Dedos/fisiologia , Mãos/fisiologia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Radiografia , Lobo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagem
7.
Neuroimage ; 14(6): 1416-23, 2001 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11707097

RESUMO

Recent neurocognitive studies show that perception and execution of actions are intimately linked. The mere observation of an action seems to evoke a tendency to execute that action. Since such imitative response tendencies are not adaptive in many everyday situations imitative response tendencies usually have to be inhibited. These inhibitory processes have never been investigated using brain imaging techniques. Former work on response inhibition and interference control has focused on paradigms such as the Stroop task or the go/no-go task. We have carried out an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study in order to investigate the cortical mechanisms underlying the inhibition of imitative responses. The experiment employs a simple response task in which subjects were instructed to execute predefined finger movements (tapping or lifting of the index finger) in response to an observed congruent or incongruent finger movement (tapping or lifting). A comparison of brain activation in incongruent and congruent trials revealed strong activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (middle frontal gyrus) and activation in the right frontopolar cortex and the right anterior parietal cortex, as well as in the precuneus. These results support the assumption of prefrontal involvement in response inhibition and extend this assumption to a "new" class of prepotent responses, namely, to imitative actions.


Assuntos
Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Imageamento Tridimensional , Comportamento Imitativo/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Atenção/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Imagem Ecoplanar , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Masculino , Valores de Referência
8.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 106(1-2): 3-22, 2001 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11256338

RESUMO

The present study was designed to examine the hypothesis that stimulus-response arrangements with high ideomotor compatibility lead to substantial compatibility effects even in simple response tasks. In Experiment 1, participants executed pre-instructed finger movements in response to compatible and incompatible finger movements. A pronounced reaction time advantage was found for compatible as compared to incompatible trials. Experiment 2 revealed a much smaller compatibility effect for less ideomotor-compatible object movements compared to finger movements. Experiment 3 presented normal stimuli (hand upright) and flipped stimuli (hand upside-down). Two components were found to contribute to the compatibility effect, a dynamic spatial compatibility component (related to movement directions) and an ideomotor component (related to movement types). The implications of these results for theories about stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) as well as for theories about imitation are discussed.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento , Movimento/fisiologia , Percepção Visual , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Teoria Psicológica , Tempo de Reação
9.
Brain Cogn ; 44(2): 124-43, 2000 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11041986

RESUMO

Intuitively, one can assume that imitating a movement is an easier task than responding to a symbolic stimulus like a verbal instruction. Support for this suggestion can be found in neuropsychological research as well as in research on stimulus-response compatibility. However controlled experimental evidence for this assumption is still lacking. We used a stimulus-response compatibility paradigm to test the assumption. In a series of experiments, it was tested whether observed finger movements have a stronger influence on finger movement execution than a symbolic or spatial cue. In the first experiment, we compared symbolic cues with observed finger movements using an interference paradigm. Observing finger movements strongly influenced movement execution, irrespective of whether the finger movement was the relevant or the irrelevant stimulus dimension. In the second experiment, effects of observed finger movements and spatial finger cues were compared. The observed finger movement dominated the spatial finger cue. A reduction in the similarity of observed and executed action in the third experiment led to a decrease of the influence of observed finger movement, which demonstrates the crucial role of the imitative relation of observed and executed action for the described effects. The results are discussed in relation to recent models of stimulus-response compatibility. Neurocognitive support for the strong relationship between movement observation and movement execution is reported.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Dedos/fisiologia , Comportamento Imitativo/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Simbolismo , Retroalimentação , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação
10.
Science ; 286(5449): 2526-8, 1999 Dec 24.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10617472

RESUMO

How does imitation occur? How can the motor plans necessary for imitating an action derive from the observation of that action? Imitation may be based on a mechanism directly matching the observed action onto an internal motor representation of that action ("direct matching hypothesis"). To test this hypothesis, normal human participants were asked to observe and imitate a finger movement and to perform the same movement after spatial or symbolic cues. Brain activity was measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging. If the direct matching hypothesis is correct, there should be areas that become active during finger movement, regardless of how it is evoked, and their activation should increase when the same movement is elicited by the observation of an identical movement made by another individual. Two areas with these properties were found in the left inferior frontal cortex (opercular region) and the rostral-most region of the right superior parietal lobule.


Assuntos
Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Comportamento Imitativo/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Dedos/fisiologia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Movimento , Neurônios/fisiologia
11.
J Intensive Care Med ; 6(1): 36-44, 1991.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10149577

RESUMO

Transcranial Doppler sonography noninvasively measures flow velocities within the basal cerebral arteries. It has been used for the management of patients with ischemic cerebrovascular disease and subarachnoid hemorrhage, as well as in the determination of brain death. Its role and technical limitations in the intensive care unit are reviewed.


Assuntos
Artérias Cerebrais/diagnóstico por imagem , Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/diagnóstico por imagem , Velocidade do Fluxo Sanguíneo , Morte Encefálica/diagnóstico por imagem , Circulação Cerebrovascular/fisiologia , Humanos , Unidades de Terapia Intensiva , Monitorização Fisiológica , Hemorragia Subaracnóidea/diagnóstico por imagem , Ultrassonografia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...