Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 128
Filtrar
1.
Dalton Trans ; 49(24): 8238-8246, 2020 Jun 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32510087

RESUMO

The lanthanide complexes EuL3, GdL3, YbL3 and LuL3 of the N,N'-bis(2-hydroxy-di-3,5-tert-butylphenyl)amine were prepared. The X-Ray crystal structures of GdL3 and LuL3 demonstrated a nine-coordinate sphere with three ligand molecules under their anionic diamagnetic form (Cat-N-BQ)-. The complexes showed three oxidation events (Eox11/2 = 0.15-0.16 V, E1/22 = 0.51-55 V, and E1/23 = 0.75-0.78 V vs. Fc+/Fc) via cyclic voltammetry, corresponding to the successive oxidation of the aminophenolate moeities to iminosemiquinone species. The complexes GdL3 and YbL3 were characterized by EPR spectroscopy, allowing for the determination of the zero field splitting (ZFS) parameters in the first case. The monocations (LnL3)+ and monoanions (LnL3)- were electrochemically generated (Ln = Eu, Gd, Yb, Lu), as well as the dications YbL32+ and LuL32+. The spins are antiferromagnetically exchange coupled in the diradical species LuL32+ (|D| = 260 MHz, E = 0). All the complexes (incl. neutral) possess a strong absorption band in the NIR region (730-840 nm, ε > 19 mM-1 cm-1) corresponding to ligand-based transitions.

3.
Reproduction ; 126(3): 365-70, 2003 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12968944

RESUMO

Direct observation of 387 embryos in the early stages of development was combined with observations on breeding behaviour and reproductive biology obtained from the published literature, to estimate the timing of births, oestrus, ovulation and implantation, and to derive estimates of the duration of pregnancy, embryonic diapause and active gestation for crabeater seals (Lobodon carcinophagus). The total duration of pregnancy (conception to birth) is estimated to be 11.3 months (344 days). It is estimated that the pupping season extends from late September to early November, with peak births in mid-October. The estimated mean duration of lactation is approximately 17 days; the mean date of weaning is 31 October (14 October to 17 November); and the mean date of conception is 4 November (18 October to 21 November). Oestrus, ovulation and conception occur approximately 4 days after weaning. Estimates of times of weaning and conception were made assuming that the preimplantation period is the same in all individuals. The mean date of implantation of the embryo is 24 January+/-17 days; the duration of embryonic diapause is 2.7 months (81 days); and the duration of active gestation (implantation to parturition) is 8.8 months (264 days).


Assuntos
Implantação Tardia do Embrião/fisiologia , Prenhez/fisiologia , Focas Verdadeiras/fisiologia , Estações do Ano , Animais , Estro/fisiologia , Feminino , Lactação/fisiologia , Ovulação/fisiologia , Gravidez , Desmame
4.
Child Neuropsychol ; 7(3): 153-61, 2001 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12187472

RESUMO

This study assessed both left- and right-hemisphere functions simultaneously when two-syllable words differing only in the initial stop consonant and spoken in different emotional tones were paired dichotically. Seventy-two right-handed normally achieving children, 12 boys and 12 girls at each of grades 1, 3, and 5, were instructed to detect either the presence of a specific word or of a specific emotion. In addition, 30 right-handed learning disabled (LD) children (age-matched to the normal controls) were assessed to determine whether LD children distribute verbal and nonverbal functions to different hemispheres. Results indicated that although both control and LD children demonstrated an overall REA for word stimuli and an LEA for emotional stimuli, and that emotional stimuli were easier to process than word stimuli, LD children were less accurate in processing both types of stimuli than their control counterparts. 'Complementary specialization,' as assessed through distribution of laterality effects, was found to be greater for control children than for LD children. However, the lack of consistency in complementary specialization found among the three developmental grade levels may be indicative that independent brain mechanisms underlying verbal and emotional processing have yet to be fully established in children. Further, in contrast to adult findings, a larger LEA was obtained for the emotion 'happy' than for the emotion 'sad.' It was concluded that whereas independent hemisphere processing for words and emotions is somewhat prevalent for control children, LD children might not be as strongly lateralized for opposite hemisphere processing of these functions.


Assuntos
Dominância Cerebral , Emoções , Deficiências da Aprendizagem/psicologia , Percepção da Fala , Análise de Variância , Criança , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Proibitinas , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas
5.
6.
Immunity ; 12(5): 547-56, 2000 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10843387

RESUMO

The enormous diversity of the T cell pool makes it difficult to determine whether inherent biases in the naive TCR repertoire can influence T cell responsiveness. In C57BL/6 mice the cytotoxic T lymphocyte response to an immunodominant HSV-1 determinant (gB) is characterized by a prominent bias in Vbeta element usage, associated with a conserved and preferentially D element-encoded CDR3 sequence. Comparison of naive and gB-specific T cell populations revealed a similar enrichment of germline D element-encoded CDR3 sequences in the preimmune repertoire. Strikingly, eliminating the germline coding of the gB-specific CDR3 sequence caused an almost complete loss of the dominant subset of gB-specific T cells, illustrating that CDR3 biases can significantly alter both the composition and strength of an immune response.


Assuntos
Antígenos Virais/imunologia , Regiões Determinantes de Complementaridade , Herpesvirus Humano 1/imunologia , Receptores de Antígenos de Linfócitos T/imunologia , Linfócitos T Citotóxicos/imunologia , Animais , Epitopos Imunodominantes/imunologia , Região Variável de Imunoglobulina/imunologia , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL
7.
Aust Vet J ; 77(1): 30-1, 1999 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10028390
8.
Laterality ; 4(2): 173-92, 1999 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15513112

RESUMO

Handedness and eye-dominance are undoubtedly associated statistically, although a previous meta-analysis has found that the precise relationship is difficult to explain, with about 35% of right-handers and 57% of left-handers being left eye dominant. Of particular difficulty to genetic or other models is that the proportions are distributed asymmetrically around 50%. The present study asked whether this complicated pattern of association occurred because, following Peters, it is necessary to divide right-and left-handers into consistent handers (who write and throw with the same hand) and inconsistent handers (who write and throw with opposite hands). In an analysis of 10,635 subjects from questionnaire studies, 28.8% of left-handers and 1.6% of right-handers by writing were inconsistent for throwing. Our results also showed that writing hand and throwing hand both relate independently to eyedness, that throwing hand is somewhat more strongly associated with eyedness, and that the awkward asymmetry around 50% is now removed, 24.2% of consistent right-handers being left eye dominant compared with 72.3% of consistent left-handers, and 55.4% of inconsistent right-handers compared with 47.0% of inconsistent left-handers. We conclude that eyedness is phenotypically secondary to writing and throwing handedness. In our discussion we note that eyedness runs in families, we present new data suggesting that writing hand and throwing hand are co-inherited, and we argue that further data are now required to model properly the associations of writing hand, throwing hand, and eyedness, as well as probably also footedness and language dominance.

9.
Brain Lang ; 63(2): 237-55, 1998 Jun 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9654433

RESUMO

It has been hypothesized that there is a critical period for first-language acquisition that extends into late childhood and possibly until puberty. The hypothesis is difficult to test directly because cases of linguistic deprivation during childhood are fortunately rare. We present here the case of E.M., a young man who has been profoundly deaf since birth and grew up in a rural area where he received no formal education and had no contact with the deaf community. At the age of 15, E.M. was fitted with hearing aids that corrected his hearing loss to 35 dB, and he began to learn verbal Spanish. We describe his language development over the 4-year period since his acquisition of hearing aids and conclude that the demonstrates severe deficits in verbal comprehension and production that support the critical period hypothesis.


Assuntos
Surdez/terapia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Aprendizagem Verbal , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Auxiliares de Audição , Humanos , Masculino
10.
Neuropsychologia ; 36(2): 143-8, 1998 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9539234

RESUMO

A controversy has existed for some years regarding auditory attentional skills in reading-disabled children. Data have suggested highly developed attentional skills in groups of reading-disabled students, but reduced attentional shifts have also been documented in equivalent groups. Attentional shifts in dichotic listening with forced or directed attention are usually inferred from a significant interaction between attentional task and ear. However, this procedure cannot be used to evaluate individual test performance, and the interaction does not give a useful measure of attentional shifts in dichotic listening meaningful for comparison with other tests of attention. In this paper attentional shifts in dichotic listening are quantified with the Attentional Shift Index (ASI), a measure for evaluating the degree of attentional shift in individual subjects. The ASI is based on the log-odds ratio of hits and intrusion errors when the subject has been tested under conditions of directed or forced attention. When 58.3% of the normative sample showed significant attentional shifts, none of the reading-disabled sample did so. This finding is discussed in relation to different types of deficits that can account for for the lack of auditory attentional shifts.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Dislexia/psicologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Criança , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos
11.
Neuropsychologia ; 36(1): 37-43, 1998 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9533385

RESUMO

A tremendous amount of experimental work has attempted to identify reliable behavioural predictors of cerebral lateralization. Preferred handedness has been the most popular predictor, but some recent reports suggest that preferred footedness may serve as a more accurate predictor of functional laterality, especially in the left-handed population. The present study sought to test this claim by selectively recruiting individuals with either 'crossed' lateral preferences (right-handed and left-footed or left-handed and right-footed) or 'uncrossed' lateral preferences (right-handed and right-footed or left-handed and left-footed). Lateralization of emotional perception was assessed with two blocks of the dichotic Emotional Words Test (EWT), and lateral preference for both handedness and footedness was assessed using self-report questionnaires. Ear advantage on the dichotic task varied significantly with preferred foot (P=0.003), but not with preferred hand. Cerebral lateralization may be more related to footedness than to other lateral preferences.


Assuntos
Emoções , , Lateralidade Funcional , Adulto , Córtex Cerebral , Feminino , Previsões , Humanos , Masculino , Destreza Motora , Percepção
12.
Laterality ; 3(1): 41-51, 1998 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15513074

RESUMO

A tremendous amount of experimental work has attempted to identify a reliable behavioural predictor of language lateralisation. Preferred handedness has probably received the most attention, but there are some recent reports that preferred footedness may serve as a more reliable predictor. The present study sought to test this claim by selectively recruiting 32 participants such that the factors of handedness, footedness, and gender were completely crossed. Language lateralisation was assessed with the Fused Dichotic Words Test (FDWT), and lateral preference for both handedness and footedness was assessed using self-report questionnaires. Ear advantage on the dichotic task varied significantly with preferred foot ( P <.001) but not with preferred hand ( P = .196). This result is problematic for evolutionary theories of cerebral lateralisation that claim left-hemispheric language is related to fine manual motor skill and sequencing. Language lateralisation may be more closely related to asymmetrical control of whole-body finely sequenced movements.

13.
Laterality ; 3(3): 221-5, 1998 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15513085

RESUMO

Mentally retarded and age-matched normal controls with no history of parental sinistrality were examined for their handedness with the Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire (Singh & Bryden, 1994). Normal controls showed a clear rightward bias, with the effect being significantly higher for skilled as compared to unskilled hand items. Mentally retarded subjects showed mixed handedness with a nonsignificant difference in performance between skilled and unskilled hand items.

14.
Neuropsychologia ; 35(11): 1467-73, 1997 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9352524

RESUMO

Laterality for the processing of melody and timbre was investigated in 64 right-handed non-musicians. In one block of dichotic-listening trials, participants listened for a prespecified target melody, and in a second block they listened for a prespecified target instrument. Females were more accurate on the left ear in the melody task (whereas males tended to show no ear advantage), but there were no significant ear differences in the timbre task for either sex. This supports the idea of a complementary sex-based pattern of lateralization, with males more strongly lateralized for verbal stimuli and females more strongly lateralized for non-verbal stimuli. No relation was observed between lambda measures for the two tasks, suggesting that laterality for melody processing is independent of laterality for timbre processing.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional , Processos Mentais/fisiologia , Música/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Percepção Auditiva , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais
15.
Behav Brain Res ; 86(1): 79-87, 1997 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9105585

RESUMO

Two experiments using two inbred strains of mice were conducted to study mouse paw preference. In the first study, 250 (50 x 5) paw reaches from each of 12 male and 12 female C57BL/6J mice were observed over an 8-week period using the Collins paw preference apparatus to investigate questions relating to the independence of reaches, the size of the tube into which the animals reach for flakes of food, and practise effects. Animals appeared to be much more highly lateralized when two independent reaches per day for 25 days (I protocol) were observed than when 50 reaches were observed in a single session (S protocol). Paradoxically, however, we found no evidence for the lack of independence of reaches when we examined the sequences of reaches performed by animals under the S protocol conditions. With practise, animals became slightly more lateralized, but there were no effects of sex, and approximately equal numbers of mice were left-pawed as were right-pawed. The size of the tube had no effect. The second study used 30 BALB mice, each of which was tested with both the I and S protocols, with order of testing counterbalanced. There was a significant protocol x order interaction such that those doing S first were more highly lateralized on the I task, but those doing I first were highly lateralized on both tasks.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Animais , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Feminino , Masculino , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos BALB C , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Psicofisiologia
16.
Laterality ; 2(3-4): 317-36, 1997.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15513071

RESUMO

Lateral preferences in humans and other primates may be a key to many important issues in biology and psychology. There is strong evidence that the representation of language functions in the cerebral cortex is different in left-handed (LH) people than in right-handed (RH) people, and an understanding of handedness may lead to valuable clues as to how the brain becomes organised in the way that it does. Although there are clearly cultural effects that influence manual activities, there is nevertheless evidence that human handedness and other lateralities are at least in part genetically determined: compared with two RH parents, one RH and one LH parent are 2-3 times more likely to have an LH child, and two LH parents are 3-4 times more likely to have an LH child. Thus, one might wish to investigate the genetics of laterality with the goal of understanding the biological mechanisms that lead to the preferential use of one hand (or eye or foot). One may also see links between human handedness and footedness and the motor asymmetries found in many nonhuman primate species, and perhaps in lower mammals as well. From this perspective, one might see the study of human laterality as relevant to evolutionary biology. Investigators of both human and nonhuman asymmetries have to grapple with such difficult measurement issues as the relations between preference and performance, and the influences of postural and task demands, and have much to offer each other in the quest for the nature of laterality. Our recent work seems to indicate that the various sensory and motor lateralities may be related, in humans, but not in a simple way. In future work, the challenge will be to identify the relations between the various laterality ''profiles'' and patterns of functional cerebral organisation.

17.
Int J Neurosci ; 87(3-4): 257-65, 1996 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9003986

RESUMO

Here we report the results of a study that tested what has become known as the Geschwind hypothesis (Geschwind & Galaburda, 1987). This study involved 526 people who were tested by an allergist for IgE-mediated allergies and who filled out a 12-item handedness questionnaire that also included questions about eyedness, prevalence of left-handedness in the immediate family, and familial developmental language problems. A series of log-linear analyses revealed that if the definition of "anomalous" included left-handedness and having a first-order left-handed relative, one was indeed more likely to be allergic than were those not so classified. When the definition of anomalous simply included those classified as left-handed, the results were not significant. Parallel findings resulted when the criterion variable was the presence of developmental language disorders. No evidence was found for relations between allergies and developmental language problems.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/genética , Hipersensibilidade Imediata/genética , Transtornos da Linguagem/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Dominância Cerebral/genética , Feminino , Humanos , Imunoglobulina E/imunologia , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
18.
Neuropsychologia ; 34(5): 407-11, 1996 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9148197

RESUMO

This study examines the effect of biased attention on the fused dichotic words test (FDWT) and the CV syllables dichotic listening test (CVT). Eight males and eight females were given both tests with two different instructions: to direct attention to the left ear (DL), or to the right ear (DR). These instructions led to highly significant differences in response on the CVT, but only a marginal shift in performance on the FDWT. While the FDWT is not completely unaffected by attentional manipulations, it is far less influenced by such effects than the CVT. This indicates that subject-initiated shifts of attention are much less likely to affect performance on the FDWT than on other dichotic tests and makes it a more valuable task to assess cerebral speech lateralization.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Adulto , Método Duplo-Cego , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Caracteres Sexuais , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia
19.
Laterality ; 1(1): 5-34, 1996 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15513026

RESUMO

About one in ten people is left-handed and one in three is left-eyed. The extent of the association of handedness and eyedness is unclear, as some eyedness measures are potentially contaminated by measures of handedness. A meta-analysis of hand-eye concordance in 54,087 subjects from 54 populations, found that concordance was 2.69 x greater in questionnaire rather than performance studies, 1.95 x greater in studies using unimanual monocular performance measures, and 6.29 x greater in studies using non-sighting measures of eye-dominance. In the remaining studies, which seemed to show no evidence of bias, the odds-ratio for hand-eye concordance was 2.53 x; in a population with 9.25% left-handedness and 36.53% left-eyedness, 34.43% of right-handers and 57.14% of left-handers are left-eyed. This pattern of hand-eye association poses problems for genetic models of cerebral lateralisation, which must invoke pleiotropic alleles at a single locus or epistatic interactions between multiple loci. There was no evidence that the incidence of eyedness, or the association between eyedness and handedness, differed between the sexes.

20.
Brain Lang ; 49(3): 202-23, 1995 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7640963

RESUMO

Are laterality effects in visual word recognition a product of functional asymmetry of the cerebral hemispheres or a result of stimulus inspection factors such as asymmetric eye fixations before and during visual presentation, different movement time to asymmetric targets, an interaction of the informative components, or morphemic structure of a target with a drop in acuity with distance? Two experiments were conducted to address these questions. Experiment One recorded the inspection of prefixed and matched control words presented unilaterally for 200 msec. Experiment Two displayed suffixed words and their appropriate controls. The words also varied in their information distribution. Reader's eye movements were monitored while they inspected the target words in preparation for a synonym judgement task. Measures of the location of the initial fixation on the screen showed that subjects were not biased to the right visual field prior to stimulus onset. When only those occasions in which the subject had correctly fixated the central cursor were analyzed, a robust right visual field advantage was observed on many of the target inspection time measures, but no reliable difference was observed for faster movement time to the right visual field's target compared to that observed in the left visual field. Neither the distribution of information within the word nor its morphemic structure affected the pattern of any inspection time measures. Consequently, such factors can be ruled out as threats to the hypothesis concerned with the functional asymmetry of the cerebral hemispheres.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica , Semântica , Vocabulário , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Fixação Ocular , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores de Tempo
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...