Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 3 de 3
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Cell ; 187(2): 409-427.e19, 2024 01 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38242086

RESUMO

Certain memories resist extinction to continue invigorating maladaptive actions. The robustness of these memories could depend on their widely distributed implementation across populations of neurons in multiple brain regions. However, how dispersed neuronal activities are collectively organized to underpin a persistent memory-guided behavior remains unknown. To investigate this, we simultaneously monitored the prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, hippocampus, and ventral tegmental area (VTA) of the mouse brain from initial recall to post-extinction renewal of a memory involving cocaine experience. We uncover a higher-order pattern of short-lived beta-frequency (15-25 Hz) activities that are transiently coordinated across these networks during memory retrieval. The output of a divergent pathway from upstream VTA glutamatergic neurons, paced by a slower (4-Hz) oscillation, actuates this multi-network beta-band coactivation; its closed-loop phase-informed suppression prevents renewal of cocaine-biased behavior. Binding brain-distributed neural activities in this temporally structured manner may constitute an organizational principle of robust memory expression.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Memória , Animais , Camundongos , Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Cocaína/farmacologia , Cocaína/metabolismo , Memória/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia
2.
PLoS Biol ; 16(10): e3000022, 2018 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30321171

RESUMO

This Formal Comment responds to Jordan et al., and stresses that if scientific findings are to be robust, training in experimental design and statistics is critical to ensure that research questions, design considerations, and analyses are aligned.


Assuntos
Projetos de Pesquisa , Jordânia
3.
PLoS Biol ; 16(4): e2005282, 2018 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29617358

RESUMO

Biologists determine experimental effects by perturbing biological entities or units. When done appropriately, independent replication of the entity-intervention pair contributes to the sample size (N) and forms the basis of statistical inference. If the wrong entity-intervention pair is chosen, an experiment cannot address the question of interest. We surveyed a random sample of published animal experiments from 2011 to 2016 where interventions were applied to parents and effects examined in the offspring, as regulatory authorities provide clear guidelines on replication with such designs. We found that only 22% of studies (95% CI = 17%-29%) replicated the correct entity-intervention pair and thus made valid statistical inferences. Nearly half of the studies (46%, 95% CI = 38%-53%) had pseudoreplication while 32% (95% CI = 26%-39%) provided insufficient information to make a judgement. Pseudoreplication artificially inflates the sample size, and thus the evidence for a scientific claim, resulting in false positives. We argue that distinguishing between biological units, experimental units, and observational units clarifies where replication should occur, describe the criteria for genuine replication, and provide concrete examples of in vitro, ex vivo, and in vivo experimental designs.


Assuntos
Experimentação Animal/estatística & dados numéricos , Projetos de Pesquisa/estatística & dados numéricos , Estatística como Assunto , Animais , Guias como Assunto , Humanos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Tamanho da Amostra
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...