Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 4 de 4
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Zoonoses Public Health ; 59(3): 155-63, 2012 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21914152

RESUMO

Zoonotic disease surveillance is typically triggered after animal pathogens have already infected humans. Are there ways to identify high-risk viruses before they emerge in humans? If so, then how and where can identifications be made and by what methods? These were the fundamental questions driving a workshop to examine the future of predictive surveillance for viruses that might jump from animals to infect humans. Virologists, ecologists and computational biologists from academia, federal government and non-governmental organizations discussed opportunities as well as obstacles to the prediction of species jumps using genetic and ecological data from viruses and their hosts, vectors and reservoirs. This workshop marked an important first step towards envisioning both scientific and organizational frameworks for this future capability. Canine parvoviruses as well as seasonal H3N2 and pandemic H1N1 influenza viruses are discussed as exemplars that suggest what to look for in anticipating species jumps. To answer the question of where to look, prospects for discovering emerging viruses among wildlife, bats, rodents, arthropod vectors and occupationally exposed humans are discussed. Finally, opportunities and obstacles are identified and accompanied by suggestions for how to look for species jumps. Taken together, these findings constitute the beginnings of a conceptual framework for achieving a virus surveillance capability that could predict future species jumps.


Assuntos
Doenças Transmissíveis Emergentes/transmissão , Vigilância de Evento Sentinela , Zoonoses , Animais , Animais Domésticos , Animais Selvagens , Doenças Transmissíveis Emergentes/epidemiologia , Reservatórios de Doenças/veterinária , Reservatórios de Doenças/virologia , Vetores de Doenças , Cães , Previsões , Humanos , Vírus da Influenza A Subtipo H1N1 , Vírus da Influenza A Subtipo H3N2 , Infecções por Parvoviridae , Parvovirus Canino , Especificidade da Espécie , Zoonoses/epidemiologia , Zoonoses/virologia
2.
Behav Genet ; 31(3): 275-85, 2001 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11699600

RESUMO

A study was conducted to identify quantitative trait loci (QTLs) that affect learning in honeybees. Two F1 supersister queens were produced from a cross between two established lines that had been selected for differences in the speed at which they reverse a learned discrimination between odors. Different families of haploid drones from two of these F1 queens were evaluated for two kinds of learning performance--reversal learning and latent inhibition--which previously showed correlated selection responses. Random amplified polymorphic DNA markers were scored from recombinant, haploid drone progeny that showed extreme manifestations of learning performance. Composite interval mapping procedures identified two QTLs for reversal learning (lrn2 and lrn3: LOD, 2.45 and 2.75, respectively) and one major QTL for latent inhibition (lrn1: LOD, 6.15). The QTL for latent inhibition did not map to either of the linkage groups that were associated with reversal learning. Identification of specific genes responsible for these kinds of QTL associations will open up new windows for better understanding of genes involved in learning and memory.


Assuntos
Abelhas/genética , Inibição Psicológica , Característica Quantitativa Herdável , Reversão de Aprendizagem , Animais , Cruzamentos Genéticos , Feminino , Marcadores Genéticos/genética , Masculino , Olfato/genética
3.
Insect Mol Biol ; 9(6): 625-34, 2000 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11122471

RESUMO

Our primary objective was to identify techniques to transform the genome of the honey bee (Apis mellifera) with foreign DNA constructs. The strategy we adopted was to linearize foreign DNA and introduce it with sperm during the instrumental insemination of virgin queen honey bees. We analysed extracts from larvae within the same cohort and isolated the predicted fragment by means of PCR amplification of genomic DNA. Larvae that carried the construct also expressed the introduced DNA. We propagated several transgenic lines for up to three generations, which demonstrates its heritability. Once carried by a queen, the construct can be detected in that queen's larvae over several months. However, there was no evidence of integration of the construct, at least as determined by genomic Southern analysis. Nevertheless, this demonstrates the general viability of the technique for introduction of DNA, and it should be augmented by further use of transposable elements that enhance integration.


Assuntos
Abelhas/genética , Espermatozoides/fisiologia , Transformação Genética , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Southern Blotting , Feminino , Proteínas de Fluorescência Verde , Proteínas Luminescentes/genética , Masculino , Plasmídeos , Reação em Cadeia da Polimerase
4.
J Exp Biol ; 195: 91-108, 1994 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7964421

RESUMO

Proboscis extension conditioning of honeybee workers was used to study the processing of odorants when bees were conditioned to binary mixtures. Responses to a set of pure floral odors and pheromones after conditioning have already been described. When bees are conditioned to certain mixtures of odorants, the response to both components is equal to that when they are tested alone. However, mixtures of an aliphatic aldehyde and an alcohol elicit asymmetric response patterns; that is, the response to the aldehyde is much stronger than that to the alcohol. A bee's response to the alcohol after it had been trained in an aldehyde background is significantly lower than when the bee is trained to respond to the same alcohol in the background of another odorant. Such response patterns are not necessarily caused by a behavioral decrement resulting from a compound-unique perceptual effect produced by the mixture. Furthermore, studies of blocking show that behavioral acquisition in response to one component can be hindered or blocked by pretraining with the other component. These results suggest that honeybees can perceive the individual components of some binary mixtures. The similarities in neural processing in olfactory systems of vertebrates and invertebrates mean that such studies could elucidate behavioral mechanisms of olfaction in a wide phylogenetic spectrum of animals.


Assuntos
Abelhas/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Odorantes , Condutos Olfatórios/fisiologia , Álcoois/farmacologia , Aldeídos/farmacologia , Animais , Condicionamento Psicológico/efeitos dos fármacos , Condicionamento Psicológico/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Órgãos dos Sentidos/fisiologia , Olfato/fisiologia , Estimulação Química
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...