Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 39
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23024629

RESUMO

Pharmacological block of inhibition is often used to determine if inhibition contributes to spike selectivity, in which a preferred stimulus evokes more spikes than a null stimulus. When inhibitory block reduces spike selectivity, a common interpretation is that differences between the preferred- and null-evoked inhibitions created the selectivity from less-selective excitatory inputs. In models based on empirical properties of cells from the inferior colliculus (IC) of awake bats, we show that inhibitory differences are not required. Instead, inhibition can enhance spike selectivity by changing the gain, the ratio of output spikes to input current. Within the model, we made preferred stimuli that evoked more spikes than null stimuli using five distinct synaptic mechanisms. In two cases, synaptic selectivity (the differences between the preferred and null inputs) was entirely excitatory, and in two it was entirely inhibitory. In each case, blocking inhibition eliminated spike selectivity. Thus, observing spike rates following inhibitory block did not distinguish among the cases where synaptic selectivity was entirely excitatory or inhibitory. We then did the same modeling experiment using empirical synaptic conductances derived from responses to preferred and null sounds. In most cases, inhibition in the model enhanced spike selectivity mainly by gain modulation and firing rate reduction. Sometimes, inhibition reduced the null gain to zero, eliminating null-evoked spikes. In some cases, inhibition increased the preferred gain more than the null gain, enhancing the difference between the preferred- and null-evoked spikes. Finally, inhibition kept firing rates low. When selectivity is quantified by the selectivity index (SI, the ratio of the difference to the sum of the spikes evoked by the preferred and null stimuli), inhibitory block reduced the SI by increasing overall firing rates. These results are consistent with inhibition shaping spike selectivity by gain control.

2.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 110(4): 2065-84, 2001 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11681385

RESUMO

The perceptual significance of the cochlear amplifier was evaluated by predicting level-discrimination performance based on stochastic auditory-nerve (AN) activity. Performance was calculated for three models of processing: the optimal all-information processor (based on discharge times), the optimal rate-place processor (based on discharge counts), and a monaural coincidence-based processor that uses a non-optimal combination of rate and temporal information. An analytical AN model included compressive magnitude and level-dependent-phase responses associated with the cochlear amplifier, and high-, medium-, and low-spontaneous-rate (SR) fibers with characteristic frequencies (CFs) spanning the AN population. The relative contributions of nonlinear magnitude and nonlinear phase responses to level encoding were compared by using four versions of the model, which included and excluded the nonlinear gain and phase responses in all possible combinations. Nonlinear basilar-membrane (BM) phase responses are robustly encoded in near-CF AN fibers at low frequencies. Strongly compressive BM responses at high frequencies near CF interact with the high thresholds of low-SR AN fibers to produce large dynamic ranges. Coincidence performance based on a narrow range of AN CFs was robust across a wide dynamic range at both low and high frequencies, and matched human performance levels. Coincidence performance based on all CFs demonstrated the "near-miss" to Weber's law at low frequencies and the high-frequency "mid-level bump." Monaural coincidence detection is a physiologically realistic mechanism that is extremely general in that it can utilize AN information (average-rate, synchrony, and nonlinear-phase cues) from all SR groups.


Assuntos
Membrana Basilar/fisiologia , Cóclea/fisiologia , Nervo Coclear/fisiologia , Percepção Sonora/fisiologia , Fibras Nervosas/fisiologia , Discriminação da Altura Tonal/fisiologia , Audiometria de Tons Puros , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/fisiologia , Humanos , Dinâmica não Linear , Psicoacústica
3.
Neural Comput ; 13(10): 2273-316, 2001 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11570999

RESUMO

A method for calculating psychophysical performance limits based on stochastic neural responses is introduced and compared to previous analytical methods for evaluating auditory discrimination of tone frequency and level. The method uses signal detection theory and a computational model for a population of auditory nerve (AN) fiber responses. The use of computational models allows predictions to be made over a wider parameter range and with more complete descriptions of AN responses than in analytical models. Performance based on AN discharge times (all-information) is compared to performance based only on discharge counts (rate-place). After the method is verified over the range of parameters for which previous analytical models are applicable, the parameter space is then extended. For example, a computational model of AN activity that extends to high frequencies is used to explore the common belief that rate-place information is responsible for frequency encoding at high frequencies due to the rolloff in AN phase locking above 2 kHz. This rolloff is thought to eliminate temporal information at high frequencies. Contrary to this belief, results of this analysis show that rate-place predictions for frequency discrimination are inconsistent with human performance in the dependence on frequency for high frequencies and that there is significant temporal information in the AN up to at least 10 kHz. In fact, the all-information predictions match the functional dependence of human performance on frequency, although optimal performance is much better than human performance. The use of computational AN models in this study provides new constraints on hypotheses of neural encoding of frequency in the auditory system; however, the method is limited to simple tasks with deterministic stimuli. A companion article in this issue ("Evaluating Auditory Performance Limits: II") describes an extension of this approach to more complex tasks that include random variation of one parameter, for example, random-level variation, which is often used in psychophysics to test neural encoding hypotheses.


Assuntos
Nervo Coclear/fisiologia , Audição/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Discriminação da Altura Tonal/fisiologia , Animais , Humanos , Psicofísica
4.
Neural Comput ; 13(10): 2317-38, 2001 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11571000

RESUMO

Previous studies have combined analytical models of stochastic neural responses with signal detection theory (SDT) to predict psychophysical performance limits; however, these studies have typically been limited to simple models and simple psychophysical tasks. A companion article in this issue ("Evaluating Auditory Performance Limits: I") describes an extension of the SDT approach to allow the use of computational models that provide more accurate descriptions of neural responses. This article describes an extension to more complex psychophysical tasks. A general method is presented for evaluating psychophysical performance limits for discrimination tasks in which one stimulus parameter is randomly varied. Psychophysical experiments often randomly vary a single parameter in order to restrict the cues that are available to the subject. The method is demonstrated for the auditory task of random-level frequency discrimination using a computational auditory nerve (AN) model. Performance limits based on AN discharge times (all-information) are compared to performance limits based only on discharge counts (rate place). Both decision models are successful in predicting that random-level variation has no effect on performance in quiet, which is the typical result in psychophysical tasks with random-level variation. The distribution of information across the AN population provides insight into how different types of AN information can be used to avoid the influence of random-level variation. The rate-place model relies on comparisons between fibers above and below the tone frequency (i.e., the population response), while the all-information model does not require such across-fiber comparisons. Frequency discrimination with random-level variation in the presence of high-frequency noise is also simulated. No effect is predicted for all-information, consistent with the small effect in human performance; however, a large effect is predicted for rate-place in noise with random-level variation.


Assuntos
Nervo Coclear/fisiologia , Audição/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Discriminação da Altura Tonal/fisiologia , Animais , Humanos
5.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 110(2): 1020-9, 2001 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11519570

RESUMO

Sensitivity to differences in interaural correlation was measured for 1.3-ERB-wide bands of noise using a 2IFC task at six frequencies: 250, 500, 750, 1000, 1250, and 1500 Hz. The sensitivity index, d', was measured for discriminations between a number of fixed pairs of correlation values. Cumulative d' functions were derived for each frequency and condition. The d' for discriminating any two values of correlation may be recovered from the cumulative d' function by the difference between cumulative d''s for these values. Two conditions were employed: the noisebands were either presented in isolation (narrow-band condition) or in the context of broad, contiguous flanking bands of correlated noise (fringed condition). The cumulative d' functions showed greater sensitivity to differences in correlation close to 1 than close to 0 at low frequencies, but this difference was less pronounced in the fringed condition. Also, a more linear relationship was observed when cumulative d' was plotted as a function of the equivalent signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) in dB for each correlation value, rather than directly against correlation. The equivalent SNR was the SNR at which the interaural correlation in an NoS(pi) stimulus would equal the interaural correlation of the noise used in the experiment. The maximum cumulative d' declined above 750 Hz. This decline was steeper for the fringed than for the narrow-band condition. For the narrow-band condition, the total cumulative d' was variable across listeners. All cumulative d' functions were closely fitted using a simple two-parameter function. The complete data sets, averaged across listeners, from the fringed and narrow-band conditions were fitted using functions to describe the changes in these parameters over frequency, in order to produce an interpolated family of curves that describe sensitivity at frequencies between those tested. These curves predict the spectra recovered by the binaural system when complex sounds, such as speech, are masked by noise.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Discriminação da Altura Tonal , Atenção , Limiar Auditivo , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Humanos , Psicoacústica
7.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 107(1): 517-27, 2000 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10641660

RESUMO

The binaural system is well-known for its sluggish response to changes in the interaural parameters to which it is sensitive. Theories of binaural unmasking have suggested that detection of signals in noise is mediated by detection of differences in interaural correlation. If these theories are correct, improvements in the intelligibility of speech in favorable binaural conditions is most likely mediated by spectro-temporal variations in interaural correlation of the stimulus which mirror the spectro-temporal amplitude modulations of the speech. However, binaural sluggishness should limit the temporal resolution of the representation of speech recovered by this means. The present study tested this prediction in two ways. First, listeners' masked discrimination thresholds for ascending vs descending pure-tone arpeggios were measured as a function of rate of frequency change in the NoSo and NoSpi binaural configurations. Three-tone arpeggios were presented repeatedly and continuously for 1.6 s, masked by a 1.6-s burst of noise. In a two-interval task, listeners determined the interval in which the arpeggios were ascending. The results showed a binaural advantage of 12-14 dB for NoSpi at 3.3 arpeggios per s (arp/s), which reduced to 3-5 dB at 10.4 arp/s. This outcome confirmed that the discrimination of spectro-temporal patterns in noise is susceptible to the effects of binaural sluggishness. Second, listeners' masked speech-reception thresholds were measured in speech-shaped noise using speech which was 1, 1.5, and 2 times the original articulation rate. The articulation rate was increased using a phase-vocoder technique which increased all the modulation frequencies in the speech without altering its pitch. Speech-reception thresholds were, on average, 5.2 dB lower for the NoSpi than for the NoSo configuration, at the original articulation rate. This binaural masking release was reduced to 2.8 dB when the articulation rate was doubled, but the most notable effect was a 6-8 dB increase in thresholds with articulation rate for both configurations. These results suggest that higher modulation frequencies in masked signals cannot be temporally resolved by the binaural system, but that the useful modulation frequencies in speech are sufficiently low (<5 Hz) that they are invulnerable to the effects of binaural sluggishness, even at elevated articulation rates.


Assuntos
Ruído/efeitos adversos , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Humanos , Fonética , Espectrografia do Som , Fatores de Tempo
8.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 106(4 Pt 1): 1633-54, 1999 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10530009

RESUMO

In a reverberant environment, sounds reach the ears through several paths. Although the direct sound is followed by multiple reflections, which would be audible in isolation, the first-arriving wavefront dominates many aspects of perception. The "precedence effect" refers to a group of phenomena that are thought to be involved in resolving competition for perception and localization between a direct sound and a reflection. This article is divided into five major sections. First, it begins with a review of recent work on psychoacoustics, which divides the phenomena into measurements of fusion, localization dominance, and discrimination suppression. Second, buildup of precedence and breakdown of precedence are discussed. Third measurements in several animal species, developmental changes in humans, and animal studies are described. Fourth, recent physiological measurements that might be helpful in providing a fuller understanding of precedence effects are reviewed. Fifth, a number of psychophysical models are described which illustrate fundamentally different approaches and have distinct advantages and disadvantages. The purpose of this review is to provide a framework within which to describe the effects of precedence and to help in the integration of data from both psychophysical and physiological experiments. It is probably only through the combined efforts of these fields that a full theory of precedence will evolve and useful models will be developed.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Modelos Biológicos , Psicoacústica , Localização de Som/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Humanos , Fatores de Tempo
9.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 105(6): 3436-48, 1999 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10380667

RESUMO

Natural environments typically contain sound sources other than the source of interest that may interfere with the ability of listeners to extract information about the primary source. Studies of speech intelligibility and localization by normal-hearing listeners in the presence of competing speech are reported on in this work. One, two or three competing sentences [IEEE Trans. Audio Electroacoust. 17(3), 225-246 (1969)] were presented from various locations in the horizontal plane in several spatial configurations relative to a target sentence. Target and competing sentences were spoken by the same male talker and at the same level. All experiments were conducted both in an actual sound field and in a virtual sound field. In the virtual sound field, both binaural and monaural conditions were tested. In the speech intelligibility experiment, there were significant improvements in performance when the target and competing sentences were spatially separated. Performance was similar in the actual sound-field and virtual sound-field binaural listening conditions for speech intelligibility. Although most of these improvements are evident monaurally when using the better ear, binaural listening was necessary for large improvements in some situations. In the localization experiment, target source identification was measured in a seven-alternative absolute identification paradigm with the same competing sentence configurations as for the speech study. Performance in the localization experiment was significantly better in the actual sound-field than in the virtual sound-field binaural listening conditions. Under binaural conditions, localization performance was very good, even in the presence of three competing sentences. Under monaural conditions, performance was much worse. For the localization experiment, there was no significant effect of the number or configuration of the competing sentences tested. For these experiments, the performance in the speech intelligibility experiment was not limited by localization ability.


Assuntos
Meio Ambiente , Localização de Som/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
10.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 105(5): 2821-40, 1999 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10335633

RESUMO

Head-related transfer functions (HRTFs) for human subjects in anechoic space were modeled with modified phase spectra, including minimum-phase-plus-delay, linear-phase, and reversed-phase-plus-delay functions. The overall (wide-band) interaural time delay (ITD) for the modeled HRTFs was made consistent with that of the empirical HRTFs by setting the position-dependent, frequency-independent delay in the HRTF for the lagging ear. Signal analysis of the minimum-phase-plus-delay reconstructions indicated that model HRTFs deviate from empirical HRTF measurements maximally for contralateral azimuths and low elevations. Subjects assessed the perceptual validity of the model HRTFs in a four-interval, two-alternative, forced-choice discrimination paradigm. Results indicate that monaural discrimination performance of subjects was at chance for all three types of HRTF models. Binaural discrimination performance was at chance for the linear-phase HRTFs, was above chance for some locations for the minimum-phase-plus-delay HRTFs, and was above chance for all tested locations for the reversed-phase-plus-delay HRTFs. An analysis of low-frequency timing information showed that all of these results are consistent with efficient use of interaural time differences in the low-frequency components of the stimulus waveforms. It is concluded that listeners are insensitive to HRTF phase spectra as long as the overall ITD of the low-frequency components does not provide a reliable cue. In particular, the minimum-phase-plus-delay approximation to the HRTF phase spectrum is an adequate approximation as long as the low-frequency ITD is appropriate. These results and conclusions are all limited to the anechoic case when the HRTFs correspond to brief impulse responses limited to a few milliseconds.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Cabeça , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Ruído , Psicofísica , Fatores de Tempo
11.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 103(1): 475-93, 1998 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9440334

RESUMO

A model was developed that simulates the binaural response properties of low-frequency inferior colliculus (IC) neurons in response to several types of stimuli. The model incorporates existing models for auditory-nerve fibers, bushy cells in the cochlear nucleus, and cells in medial superior olive (MSO). The IC model neuron receives two inputs, one excitatory from an ipsilateral MSO model cell and one inhibitory from a contralateral MSO model cell. The membrane potential of the IC model neuron (and the other model neurons) is described by Hodgkin-Huxley type equations. Responses of IC neurons are simulated for pure-tone stimuli, binaural beat stimuli, interaural phase-modulated tones, single binaural clicks, and pairs of binaural clicks. The simulation results show most of the observed properties of IC discharge patterns, including the bimodal and unimodal interaural time difference (ITD) functions, sensitivities to direction and rate of change of ITD, ITD-dependent echo suppression, and early and late inhibitions in response to clicks. This study demonstrates that these response properties can be generated by a simple model incorporating ITD-dependent excitation and inhibition from binaural neurons.


Assuntos
Colículos Inferiores/fisiologia , Modelos Biológicos , Condução Nervosa/fisiologia , Inibição Neural/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Humanos , Fatores de Tempo , Nervo Vestibulococlear/fisiologia
12.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 103(1): 494-506, 1998 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9440335

RESUMO

The inferior colliculus (IC) model of Cai et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103, 475-493 (1998)] simulated the binaural response properties of low-frequency IC neurons in response to various acoustic stimuli. This model, however, failed to simulate the sensitivities of IC neurons to dynamically changing temporal features, such as the sharpened dynamic interaural phase difference (IPD) functions. In this paper, the Cai et al. (1998) model is modified such that an adaptation mechanism, viz., an additional channel simulating a calcium-activated, voltage-independent potassium channel which is responsible for afterhyperpolarization, is incorporated in the IC membrane model. Simulations were repeated with this modified model, including the responses to pure tones, binaural beat stimuli, interaural phase-modulated stimuli, binaural clicks, and pairs of binaural clicks. The discharge patterns of the model in response to current injection were also studied and compared with physiological data. It was demonstrated that this model showed all the properties that were simulated by the Cai et al. (1998) model. In addition, it showed some properties that were not simulated by that model, such as the sharpened dynamic IPD functions and adapting discharge patterns in response to current injection.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica , Colículos Inferiores/fisiologia , Condução Nervosa/fisiologia , Inibição Neural/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Humanos , Modelos Biológicos , Fatores de Tempo
13.
Nature ; 396(6713): 747-9, 1998.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9874370

RESUMO

Sounds heard over headphones are typically perceived inside the head (internalized), unlike real sound sources which are perceived outside the head (externalized). If the acoustical waveforms from a real sound source are reproduced precisely using headphones, auditory images are appropriately externalized and localized. The filtering (relative boosting, attenuation and delaying of component frequencies) of a sound by the head and outer ear provides information about the location of a sound source by means of the differences in the frequency spectra between the ears as well as the overall spectral shape. This location-dependent filtering is explicitly described by the head-related transfer function (HRTF) from sound source to ear canal. Here we present sounds to subjects through open-canal tube-phones and investigate how accurately the HRTFs must be reproduced to achieve true three-dimensional perception of auditory signals in anechoic space. Listeners attempted to discriminate between 'real' sounds presented from a loudspeaker and 'virtual' sounds presented over tube-phones. Our results show that the HRTFs can be smoothed significantly in frequency without affecting the perceived location of a sound. Listeners cannot distinguish real from virtual sources until the HRTF has lost most of its detailed variation in frequency, at which time the perceived elevation of the image is the reported cue.


Assuntos
Localização de Som/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Orelha/fisiologia , Movimentos da Cabeça , Humanos , Ilusões
14.
Ear Hear ; 16(4): 331-53, 1995 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8549890

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The purpose of this research was to measure the effects of reference interaural time and intensity differences on binaural performance in listeners with normal hearing and impaired hearing for a number of different binaural tests. Experiment 1 measures the dependence of binaural detection and discrimination performance on reference interaural intensity differences (IID) in the range of +/- 12 dB for listeners with normal hearing. Experiment 2 extends these measures to include reference IIDs and interaural time differences (ITD) for two groups of listeners with normal hearing with offsets in the range of +/- 12 dB and +/- 300 microseconds (group 1) and +/-24 dB and +/- 600 microseconds (group 2). Experiment 3 includes the same tests and conditions as experiment 2 for listeners with various hearing impairments. DESIGN: A set of psychophysical measurements was completed on 11 listeners with sensorineural hearing losses and 9 listeners with clinically normal hearing. The primary measurements were a set of four binaural detection and interaural discrimination thresholds measured for two 1/3-octave bands of Gaussian noise, one centered at 500 Hz and the other at 4000 Hz. Specifically, we measured binaural (antiphasic) detection thresholds for tones centered in the masking noise as well as the just-noticeable differences (JNDs) in IID, ITD, and interaural cross-correlation (ICC) for each of the noise-band stimuli. All measurements were done for a number of combinations of reference IID and ITD. In addition to these primary measurements, several other measurements were made on each subject, including monaural absolute thresholds, monaural intensity discrimination, monaural masked thresholds, and intensity levels required for interaurally balanced loudness and for a centered image. All measurements were made using a relatively quick, adaptive procedure. RESULTS: For the subjects with normal hearing, measured dependencies of the IID and ITD JNDs using noise stimuli on reference ITD and IID are different from those previously reported for tonal stimuli. Binaural performance of the listeners with impaired hearing varies widely across subjects and tests and is generally poorer than that of listeners with normal hearing. CONCLUSIONS: On the basis of the results for subjects with hearing impairments, we have reached several conclusions. First, the results for the binaural measurements cannot be explained in terms of available monaural audiometric and psychophysical measurements on these subjects. Second, the subjects' binaural abilities show no significant improvement with any combinations in the reference values of ITD and IID, providing negative evidence for the hypothesis that degraded performance for some subjects may be due to internal interaural offsets in ITD or IID. Third, the hypothesis that binaural detection and ICC discrimination are related, suggested by Durlach et al (1986), is generally supported. Fourth, binaural detection performance is not simply explained in terms of sensitivities to ITD and IID.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/diagnóstico , Audição , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Audiometria de Tons Puros , Limiar Auditivo , Condução Óssea , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Fatores de Tempo
15.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 95(6): 3475-80, 1994 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8046139

RESUMO

Informational masking was reduced using three stimulus presentation schemes that were intended to perceptually segregate the signal from the masker. The maskers were sets of sinusoids chosen randomly in frequency and intensity on each stimulus interval or, in some conditions, on every masker burst in a series of bursts within intervals. Masker components were excluded from the frequency region surrounding the 1000-Hz signal to minimize the energetic masking. Masked thresholds as great as 60-70 dB above quiet threshold were observed for some subjects in some conditions. It was shown that this informational masking could be reduced as much as 40 dB by: (1) presenting the masker to both ears and signal to one ear; (2) playing different masker samples sequentially in each interval of every trial; or (3) presenting the signal in alternate bursts of multiple, identical masker samples. For the binaural manipulation, informational masking was reduced because the masker and signal were perceived as originating from different interaural locations. In the latter two manipulations, a difference in the spectral or temporal pattern of the signal and masker provided the detection cue. These effects were interpreted as evidence of the importance of perceptual segregation of sounds in noisy listening environments where signal reception is not limited by energetic masking.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Ruído , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Limiar Auditivo , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Audição , Humanos , Mascaramento Perceptivo
16.
Hear Res ; 68(1): 115-30, 1993 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8376209

RESUMO

A point-neuron model for the activity of individual cells in the medial superior olive (MSO) is described and shown to generate discharge patterns consistent with the activity of real neurons as reported in response to low-frequency sinusoidal stimulation. Inputs to the model cell are specified as primarylike firing patterns, and the cell membrane characteristics are specified in terms of constant-potential sources and time-varying conductances. Some conductances are determined in response to the input firings and some in response to output firing times, which are generated when the membrane potential of the model cell crosses threshold. Output firing patterns generated by the model cells are consistent with those reported from neurons in dog and cat MSO. These patterns are also compatible with those of the functionally specified coincidence model described in Colburn et al. (1990). Given these observations, the following questions are addressed: What parameter values in the point-neuron model are required to generate output patterns like those observed? How do these values compare to those expected or estimated from intracellular measurements in brainstem neurons? How might one reconcile the fact that inhibitory inputs are not necessary in the model for the generation of the observed firing patterns with the fact that MSO cells receive numerous inhibitory inputs?


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Neurônios/fisiologia , Núcleo Olivar/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Membrana Celular/fisiologia , Eletrofisiologia , Potenciais da Membrana/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos
17.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 91(5): 2894-902, 1992 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1629482

RESUMO

A model of auditory object formation and an experimental evaluation of the model are described. Specifically, predictions for intensity discrimination and interaural time difference discrimination for the central component of a three-component harmonic complex are evaluated empirically. The onset time of the central, target component is varied relative to the onset times of the remaining, interferer components in order to vary the degree of fusion (versus perceptual segregation) of the target and the interferers. The model, which is based on the idea of attenuation of the components in the nonattended auditory image (in the case of segregated images), predicts lower sensitivity to information at the target component for the fused versus segregated target, and equal sensitivities for completely segregated targets and targets presented in isolation. Results are presented for four subjects with component frequencies of 400, 600, and 800 Hz and with onset time differences of 0 or 250 ms. The target duration was always 100 ms and offset times were the same for all components. The subjective results were as expected, with synchronous onsets yielding one sound object, and asynchrony of the central component yielding two sound objects. Also, the empirical results on interference in the synchronous case were in qualitative agreement with the above predictions. However, significantly more interference was found than was predicted for both synchronous and asynchronous conditions. In fact, the amount of interference found contradicts the simple attenuation model of object formation.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Modelos Biológicos , Adulto , Humanos , Percepção Sonora , Masculino , Psicoacústica , Fatores de Tempo
18.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 91(1): 336-47, 1992 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1737882

RESUMO

Binaural performance was measured as a function of stimulus frequency for four impaired listeners, each with bilaterally symmetric audiograms. The subjects had various degrees and configurations of audiometric losses: two had high-frequency, sensorineural losses; one had a flat sensorineural loss; and one had multiple sclerosis with normal audiometric thresholds. Just noticeable differences (jnd's) in interaural time, interaural intensity, and interaural correlation as well as detection thresholds for NoSo and NoS pi conditions were obtained for narrow-band noise stimuli at octave frequencies from 250-4000 Hz. Performance of the impaired listeners was generally poorer than that of normal-hearing listeners, although it was comparable to normal in a few instances. The patterns of binaural performance showed no apparent relation to the audiometric patterns; even the two subjects with similar degree and configuration of hearing loss have very different binaural performance, both in the level and frequency dependence of their performance. The frequency dependence of performance on individual tests is irregular enough that one cannot confidently interpolate between octaves. In addition, it appears that no subset of the measurements is adequate to characterize the performance in the rest of the measurements with the exception that, within limits, interaural correlation discrimination and NoS pi detection performance are related.


Assuntos
Acústica , Perda Auditiva Bilateral/fisiopatologia , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/fisiopatologia , Audição/fisiologia , Percepção da Altura Sonora , Estimulação Acústica , Humanos , Percepção Sonora , Esclerose Múltipla/fisiopatologia , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Psicoacústica , Percepção do Tempo
19.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 90(4 Pt 1): 1918-26, 1991 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1960285

RESUMO

Results are reported from a series of binaural detection and interaural correlation discrimination experiments at 500 Hz. The experiments include fringed correlation discrimination in which the correlation change is restricted to a narrow (38 Hz) target band of frequencies and the reference correlation is maintained in a fringe band of frequencies. This experiment is designed to be analogous to a detection experiment with a narrow-band target; in both cases the correlation changes only inside the target band. A simplified theoretical framework is used to compare the results of the detection and correlation discrimination experiments. Results are consistent with the notion that binaural detection and interaural correlation discrimination are effected by a common mechanism when the reference correlation is unity (as in the NoS pi case). When the reference correlation is zero (as in the NuSo case), detection performance is significantly better than predicted from the measured ability to discriminate interaural correlation.


Assuntos
Atenção , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Discriminação da Altura Tonal , Localização de Som , Humanos , Percepção Sonora , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Psicoacústica
20.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 89(1): 352-9, 1991 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2002174

RESUMO

Hit and false-alarm rates were measured for detection of a 500-Hz tone target in each of ten reproducible samples of 1/3-oct bandwidth noise centered at 500 Hz for both NoS pi and NoSo conditions. The effects on hit rates of the starting phase of the target relative to individual noise samples were investigated with two target phase angles for three subjects. The major results are: (1) performance varies significantly over masker waveforms; (2) for NoS pi conditions, the effect of target-to-marker phase angle on hit rates is not significant for these narrow-band maskers; (3) for NoSo conditions, the target-to-masker phase angle has a large effect; (4) no significant correlation between NoSo performance and NoS pi performance is seen across masker waveforms. These results are generally consistent wuth previously reported results for wideband maskers [R.H. Gilkey, D.E. Robinson, and T.E. Hanna, "Effects of masker waveform and signal-to-masker phase relation on diotic and dichotic masking by reproducible noise," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 78, 1207-1219 (1985)] with an important exception. Specifically, in the wideband experiment, significant correlation between NoSo and NoS pi performance across noise samples was found. In addition, in the wideband experiment, a small yet statistically significant effect of target-to-masker phase was observed in the NoS pi condition.


Assuntos
Atenção , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Discriminação da Altura Tonal , Adulto , Humanos , Percepção Sonora , Masculino , Psicoacústica , Curva ROC
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...