Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Vis ; 21(3): 17, 2021 03 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33729451

RESUMO

When a distractor appears close to the target location, saccades are less accurate. However, the presence of a further distractor, remote from those stimuli, increases the saccade response latency and improves accuracy. Explanations for this are either that the second, remote distractor impacts directly on target selection processes or that the remote distractor merely impairs the ability to initiate a saccade and changes the time at which unaffected target selection processes are accessed. In order to tease these two explanations apart, here we examine the relationship between latency and accuracy of saccades to a target and close distractor pair while a remote distractor appears at variable distance. Accuracy improvements are found to follow a similar pattern, regardless of the presence of the remote distractor, which suggests that the effect of the remote distractor is not the result of a direct impact on the target selection process. Our findings support the proposal that a remote distractor impairs the ability to initiate a saccade, meaning the competition between target and close distractor is accessed at a later time, thus resulting in more accurate saccades.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Cognição , Feminino , Humanos , Inibição Psicológica , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
2.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 13216, 2020 08 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32764576

RESUMO

The issue of whether visually-mediated, simple reaction time (VRT) is faster in elite athletes is contentious. Here, we examined if and how VRT is affected by gaze stability in groups of international cricketers (16 females, 28 males), professional rugby-league players (21 males), and non-sporting controls (20 females, 30 males). VRT was recorded via a button-press response to the sudden appearance of a stimulus (circular target-diameter 0.8°), that was presented centrally, or 7.5° to the left or right of fixation. The incidence and timing of saccades and blinks occurring from 450 ms before stimulus onset to 225 ms after onset were measured to quantify gaze stability. Our results show that (1) cricketers have faster VRT than controls; (2) blinks and, in particular, saccades are associated with slower VRT regardless of the level of sporting ability; (3) elite female cricketers had steadier gaze (fewer saccades and blinks) compared to female controls; (4) when we accounted for the presence of blinks and saccades, our group comparisons of VRT were virtually unchanged. The stability of gaze is not a factor that explains the difference between elite and control groups in VRT. Thus we conclude that better gaze stability cannot explain faster VRT in elite sports players.


Assuntos
Atletas , Fixação Ocular , Tempo de Reação , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Piscadela , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Movimentos Sacádicos , Adulto Jovem
3.
J Vis ; 18(2): 5, 2018 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29450501

RESUMO

An ability to predict the time-to-contact (TTC) of moving objects that become momentarily hidden is advantageous in everyday life and could be particularly so in fast-ball sports. Prediction motion (PM) experiments have sought to test this ability using tasks where a disappearing target moves toward a stationary destination. Here, we developed two novel versions of the PM task in which the destination either moved away from (Chase) or toward (Attract) the moving target. The target and destination moved with different speeds such that collision occurred 750, 1,000 or 1,250 ms after target occlusion. To determine if domain-specific experience conveys an advantage in PM tasks, we compared the performance of different sporting groups ranging from internationally competing athletes to non-sporting controls. There was no difference in performance between sporting groups and non-sporting controls but there were significant and independent effects on response error by target speed, destination speed, and occlusion period. We simulated these findings using a revised version of the linear TTC model of response timing for PM tasks (Yakimoff, Bocheva, & Mitrania, 1987; Yakimoff, Mateeff, Ehrenstein, & Hohnsbein, 1993) in which retinal input from the moving destination biases the internal representation of the occluded target. This revision closely reproduced the observed patterns of response error and thus describes a means by which the brain might estimate TTC when the target and destination are in motion.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adulto , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
4.
Sports Med Open ; 3(1): 39, 2017 Nov 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29127516

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The importance of optimal and/or superior vision for participation in high-level sports remains the subject of considerable clinical research interest. Here, we examine the vision and visual history of elite/near-elite cricketers and rugby-league players. METHODS: Stereoacuity (TNO), colour vision, and distance (with/without pinhole) and near visual acuity (VA) were measured in two cricket squads (elite/international-level, female, n = 16; near-elite, male, n = 23) and one professional rugby-league squad (male, n = 20). Refractive error was determined, and details of any correction worn and visual history were recorded. RESULTS: Overall, 63% had their last eye examination within 2 years. However, some had not had an eye examination for 5 years or had never had one (near-elite cricketers 30%; rugby-league players 15%; elite cricketers 6%). Comparing our results for all participants to published data for young, optimally corrected, non-sporting adults, distance VA was ~ 1 line of letters worse than expected. Adopting α = 0.01, the deficit in distance VA was significant, but only for elite cricketers (p < 0.001) (near-elite cricketers, p = 0.02; rugby-league players, p = 0.03). Near VA did not differ between subgroups or relative to published norms for young adults (p > 0.02 for all comparisons). On average, near stereoacuity was better than in young adults, but only in elite cricketers (p < 0.001; p = 0.03, near-elite cricketers; p = 0.47, rugby-league players). On-field visual issues were present in 27% of participants and mostly (in 75% of cases) comprised uncorrected ametropia. Some cricketers (near-elite 17.4%; elite 38%) wore refractive correction during play, but no rugby-league player did. Some individuals with prescribed correction choose not to wear it when playing. CONCLUSIONS: Aside from near stereoacuity in elite cricketers, the basic visual abilities we measured were not better than equivalent, published data for optimally corrected adults; 20-25% exhibited sub-optimal vision, suggesting that the clearest possible vision might not be critical for participation at the highest levels in the sports of cricket or rugby league. Although vision could be improved in a sizeable proportion of our sample, the impact of correcting these, mostly subtle, refractive anomalies on playing performance is unknown.

5.
Conscious Cogn ; 22(2): 562-71, 2013 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23603423

RESUMO

Language is more than a source of information for accessing higher-order conceptual knowledge. Indeed, language may determine how people perceive and interpret visual stimuli. Visual processing in linguistic contexts, for instance, mirrors language processing and happens incrementally, rather than through variously-oriented fixations over a particular scene. The consequences of this atypical visual processing are yet to be determined. Here, we investigated the integration of visual and linguistic input during a reasoning task. Participants listened to sentences containing conjunctions or disjunctions (Nancy examined an ant and/or a cloud) and looked at visual scenes containing two pictures that either matched or mismatched the nouns. Degree of match between nouns and pictures (referential anchoring) and between their expected and actual spatial positions (spatial anchoring) affected fixations as well as judgments. We conclude that language induces incremental processing of visual scenes, which in turn becomes susceptible to reasoning errors during the language-meaning verification process.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Idioma , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tempo de Reação , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia
6.
Res Dev Disabil ; 33(3): 900-8, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22240144

RESUMO

Typically developing (TD) 6-year-olds and 9-year-olds, and older children and adults with Williams syndrome (WS) navigated through brick-wall mazes in a virtual environment. Participants were shown a route through three mazes, each with 6 turns. In each maze the floor of each path section was a different colour such that colour acted as an environmental cue. The colours employed were either easy to verbalise (focal colours) or difficult to verbalise (non-focal colours). We investigated whether participants would verbally code the colour information in the focal colour condition only, and whether this facilitated route-learning. All groups could learn the routes; the WS group required more learning trials to learn the route and achieved lower memory scores than both of the TD groups. Despite this, all groups showed the same pattern of results. There was no effect of condition on the ability to learn the maze. However, when asked which colours featured in each route, higher memory scores were achieved for the focal colour (verbalisable) than the non-focal colour (non-verbalisable) condition. This suggests that, in both young children and individuals with WS, once a route has been learnt, the nature of the environmental cues within it can impact an individual's representation of that route.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Sinais (Psicologia) , Aprendizagem em Labirinto , Orientação , Interface Usuário-Computador , Percepção Visual , Síndrome de Williams/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Humanos , Rememoração Mental , Prática Psicológica , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Percepção Espacial , Comportamento Verbal , Síndrome de Williams/diagnóstico , Adulto Jovem
7.
Iperception ; 1(2): 73-82, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23397017

RESUMO

Saccadic eye-movements to a visual target are less accurate if there are distracters close to its location (local distracters). The addition of more distracters, remote from the target location (remote distracters), invokes an involuntary increase in the response latency of the saccade and attenuates the effect of local distracters on accuracy. This may be due to the target and distracters directly competing (direct route) or to the remote distracters acting to impair the ability to disengage from fixation (indirect route). To distinguish between these, we examined the development of saccade competition by recording saccade latency and accuracy responses made to a target and local distracter compared with those made with an addition of a remote distracter. The direct route would predict that the remote distracter impacts on the developing competition between target and local distracter, while the indirect route would predict no change as the accuracy benefit here derives from accessing the same competitive process but at a later stage. We found that the presence of the remote distracter did not change the pattern of accuracy improvement. This suggests that the remote distracter was acting along an indirect route that inhibits disengagement from fixation, slows saccade initiation, and enables more accurate saccades to be made.

8.
Brain Res ; 1298: 92-8, 2009 Nov 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19733156

RESUMO

Inhibition is intimately involved in the ability to select a target for a goal-directed movement. The effect of distracters on the deviation of oculomotor trajectories and landing positions provides evidence of such inhibition. Individual saccade trajectories and landing positions may deviate initially either towards, or away from, a competing distracter--the direction and extent of this deviation depends upon saccade latency and the target to distracter separation. However, the underlying commonality of the sources of oculomotor inhibition has not been investigated. Here we report the relationship between distracter-related deviation of saccade trajectory, landing position and saccade latency. Observers saccaded to a target which could be accompanied by a distracter shown at various distances from very close (10 angular degrees) to far away (120 angular degrees). A fixation-gap paradigm was used to manipulate latency independently of the influence of competing distracters. When distracters were close to the target, saccade trajectory and landing position deviated toward the distracter position, while at greater separations landing position was always accurate but trajectories deviated away from the distracters. Different spatial patterns of deviations across latency were found. This pattern of results is consistent with the metrics of the saccade reflecting coarse pooling of the ongoing activity at the distracter location: saccade trajectory reflects activity at saccade initiation while landing position reveals activity at saccade end.


Assuntos
Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Atenção/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Movimento/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
9.
Exp Brain Res ; 193(3): 467-76, 2009 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19034440

RESUMO

Identifying a stimulus as the target for a goal-directed movement involves inhibiting competing responses. Separable inhibitory interconnections bias local competition to ensure only one stimulus is selected and to alter movement initiation. Behavioural evidence of these inhibitory processes comes from the effects of distracters on oculomotor landing positions and saccade latencies. Here, we investigate the relationship between these two sources of inhibition. Targets were presented with or without close and remote distracters. In separate experiments the possible position and identity of the target and distracters were manipulated. In all cases saccade landing position was found to be less affected by the presence of the close distracter when remote distracters were also present. The involuntary increase in the latency of saccade initiation caused by the presence of the remote distracters alters the state of competitive processes involved in selecting the saccade target thus changing its landing position.


Assuntos
Atenção , Desempenho Psicomotor , Movimentos Sacádicos , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação , Adulto Jovem
10.
Spat Vis ; 18(4): 379-97, 2005.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16167772

RESUMO

Second-order cues are visual stimuli that are detectable by human observers, without eliciting a peak in Fourier energy that corresponds to their perceptual properties. The most commonly studied exemplars of second-order cues are those defined by modulation of local contrast (CM). It is widely accepted that such cues are initially detected separately from first-order, luminance modulated (LM), cues. However, after-effects have been shown to transfer between first- and second-order cues (LM and CM, respectively). This suggests the existence of a late link in the mechanisms that subserve their processing. To extend the investigation of the mechanisms for processing second-order cues we consider cues defined by modulations in local orientation (OM). Using a tilt-after-effect (TAE) paradigm, we found partial transfer of adaptation between LM and OM cues, confirming the presence of a link between first and second-order cues. Furthermore, we found a partial transfer of TAE between OM and CM cues. These results suggest that, at or before the site of adaptation, information from all visual cues is combined. However, as transfer of adaptation is below 100% in all cases, this is only a partial integration of information.


Assuntos
Adaptação Ocular/fisiologia , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Pós-Efeito de Figura/fisiologia , Orientação , Humanos , Postura/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...