Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 8 de 8
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20652706

RESUMO

Echolocating bats have to assign the received echoes to the correct call that generated them. Failing to do so will result in the perception of virtual targets that are positioned where there is no actual target. The assignment of echoes to the emitted calls can be ambiguous especially if the pulse intervals between calls are short and kept constant. Here, we present first evidence that greater mouse-eared bats deal with ambiguity by changing the pulse interval more often, in particular by reducing the number of calls in the terminal group before landing. This strategy separates virtual targets from real ones according to their change in position. Real targets will always remain in a constant position, and virtual targets will jitter back and forth according to the change in the time interval.


Assuntos
Quirópteros/fisiologia , Ecolocação/fisiologia , Voo Animal/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Animais , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Masculino , Reconhecimento Fisiológico de Modelo/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
2.
Zoonoses Public Health ; 56(6-7): 270-7, 2009 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19497087

RESUMO

Two common bat lyssavirus species have been identified in many European countries: European bat lyssavirus type-1 and -2 (EBLV-1 and EBLV-2). Only limited knowledge on the susceptibility of the natural EBLV-hosts, insectivorous bats, to lyssavirus infection is available. Our study was undertaken to evaluate the susceptibility and pathology associated with an EBLV-1 infection in Eptesicus fuscus following different routes of virus inoculation including intracranial (n = 6), intramuscular (n = 14), oral (n = 7) and intranasal (n = 7). Blood and saliva samples were collected from all bats on a monthly basis. Four bats inoculated intracranially developed rabies with a mean of 11 days to death, whilst seven bats inoculated intramuscularly developed rabies, with an extended incubation period prior to death. We did not observe any mortality in the oral (p.o.) or intranasal (i.n.) groups and both groups had detectable levels of virus neutralizing antibodies (data not shown). Virus shedding was demonstrated in the saliva by virus isolation and the detection of viral RNA in ill bats, particularly immediately prior to the development of disease. In addition, the presence of virus and viral RNA was detected in the thyroid gland in bats challenged experimentally with EBLV-1, which exceeded that detected in all other extra-neural tissue. The significance of detecting EBLV-1 in the thyroid gland of rabid bats is not well understood. We speculate that the infection of the thyroid gland may cause subacute thyroiditis, a transient form of thyroiditis causing hyperthyroidism, resulting in changes in adrenocortical activity that could lead to hormonal dysfunction, thereby distinguishing the clinical presentation of rabies in the rabid host.


Assuntos
Quirópteros/virologia , Lyssavirus/isolamento & purificação , Lyssavirus/patogenicidade , Infecções por Rhabdoviridae/veterinária , Glândula Tireoide/virologia , Animais , Quirópteros/imunologia , Reservatórios de Doenças/veterinária , Reservatórios de Doenças/virologia , RNA Viral/análise , Infecções por Rhabdoviridae/virologia , Especificidade da Espécie
3.
J Gen Virol ; 90(Pt 10): 2493-2502, 2009 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19515825

RESUMO

The serotine bat (Eptesicus serotinus) accounts for the vast majority of bat rabies cases in Europe and is considered the main reservoir for European bat lyssavirus type 1 (EBLV-1, genotype 5). However, so far the disease has not been investigated in its native host under experimental conditions. To assess viral virulence, dissemination and probable means of transmission, captive bats were infected experimentally with an EBLV-1a virus isolated from a naturally infected conspecific from Germany. Twenty-nine wild caught bats were divided into five groups and inoculated by intracranial (i.c.), intramuscular (i.m.) or subcutaneous (s.c.) injection or by intranasal (i.n.) inoculation to mimic the various potential routes of infection. One group of bats was maintained as uninfected controls. Mortality was highest in the i.c.-infected animals, followed by the s.c. and i.m. groups. Incubation periods varied from 7 to 26 days depending on the route of infection. Rabies did not develop in the i.n. group or in the negative-control group. None of the infected bats seroconverted. Viral antigen was detected in more than 50% of the taste buds of an i.c.-infected animal. Shedding of viable virus was measured by virus isolation in cell culture for one bat from the s.c. group at 13 and 14 days post-inoculation, i.e. 7 days before death. In conclusion, it is postulated that s.c. inoculation, in nature caused by bites, may be an efficient way of transmitting EBLV-1 among free-living serotine bats.


Assuntos
Quirópteros , Lyssavirus/fisiologia , Infecções por Rhabdoviridae/veterinária , Animais , Anticorpos Antivirais/sangue , Encéfalo/virologia , Feminino , Coração/virologia , Rim/virologia , Fígado/virologia , Pulmão/virologia , Masculino , Músculo Esquelético/virologia , RNA Viral/isolamento & purificação , Infecções por Rhabdoviridae/sangue , Infecções por Rhabdoviridae/virologia , Glândulas Salivares , Baço/virologia , Glândula Tireoide/virologia , Língua/virologia , Bexiga Urinária/virologia
4.
J Comp Physiol A ; 187(7): 521-8, 2001 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11730299

RESUMO

When searching for insects along edges, Barbastella barbastellus alternated between two signal types. Type-2 signals had durations around 6 ms and were composed of an initial shallowly downward frequency modulated component, starting at about 45 kHz and followed by a shorter more steeply modulated component that ended at about 32 kHz. Type-1 signals were rather stereotyped with durations around 2.5 ms and a very short rise time. They covered an approximately 8 kHz-wide frequency band positioned just below the 12-15 kHz-wide frequency band of type-2 signals, with no or small frequency overlap. In the recordings, type-1 signals almost had always a higher amplitude than type-2 signals, at least partly caused by head movements. Assuming that signal structure reflects function, we hypothesize that type-2 signals have the same adaptive value as the signals with a broadband and narrowband component of other vespertilionids, but with a reverse arrangement of the signal elements. Like the broadband component of the type-2 signals, type-1 signals are well suited to localize background targets. Thus, the localization component may be distributed among two signals separated in time, which has the advantage that both signals can be varied independently in the direction of emission and in amplitude.


Assuntos
Quirópteros/fisiologia , Ecolocação , Comportamento Alimentar , Acústica , Animais , Ultrassom
5.
J Comp Physiol A ; 183(2): 213-24, 1998 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9693992

RESUMO

Four Eptesicus fuscus were trained in a range discrimination experiment to choose the closer of two phantom targets. Echo attenuation was roving between trials returning echoes ranging from -10 dB to -50 dB SPL (sound pressure level) relative to emission SPL. Discrimination thresholds were determined. After sufficient training, ranging performance was stable and about the same in the range between -20 dB and -50 dB with range difference thresholds around 300 microseconds. At -10 dB, performance was poor even after long training. After additional training at a constant relative echo SPL of -30 dB and a delay difference of 300 microseconds the performance measured with roving echo SPL improved at all relative echo SPL between -20 dB and -50 dB but not at -10 dB. The new experimental procedure improved the performance by additional learning, and the bats generalized over a wide range of relative echo SPL. Threshold improved to 100 microseconds when measured at a constant relative echo SPL of -30 dB, again indicating the influence of the experimental procedure. In correspondence to neurophysiological data the ranging performance deteriorates if the echo SPL is close to the emission SPL. Signal duration and emission SPL were variable during range discrimination.


Assuntos
Quirópteros/fisiologia , Ecolocação/fisiologia , Condicionamento Físico Animal , Animais , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Masculino , Pressão , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Som
6.
J Comp Physiol A ; 175(5): 563-71, 1994 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7965921

RESUMO

Four bats of the species Eptesicus fuscus were trained in a two-alternative forced-choice procedure to discriminate between two phantom targets that differed in range. The rewarded stimulus was located at a distance of 52.7 cm, while the other unrewarded stimulus was further away. Only one target was presented at a time. In the first experiment we measured the range discrimination performance at an echo SPL of -28 dB relative to the bat's sonar transmission. A 75% correct performance level was arbitrarily defined as threshold and was obtained at a delay difference of 80 microseconds, corresponding to a range difference of 13.8 mm. In the second experiment the delay difference was fixed at 150 microseconds and the echo SPL varied between -8 and -48 dB relative to sonar emissions. The performance of the bats depended on the relative echo SPL. AT -28 dB the bats showed the best performance. It deteriorated at an increase of the relative echo SPL to -18 dB and -8 dB. The performance also deteriorated when the relative echo SPL was reduced to -38 dB and -48 dB. Only at low relative echo SPLs did the bats partially compensate for the reduction in echo SPL and increased the SPL of their emitted signals by a few dB. Our results support the hypothesis that neurons exhibiting paradoxical latency shift may be involved in encoding target range. This hypothesis predicts a decrease in performance at high echo SPLs as we found it in our experiments. The observed reduction in performance at very low echo SPLs may be due to a decrease in S/N ratio.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Quirópteros/fisiologia , Ecolocação , Animais , Discriminação Psicológica , Masculino , Psicometria , Tempo de Reação
7.
Dev Psychobiol ; 24(7): 453-61, 1991 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1797591

RESUMO

The appetitive behavior of 3- to 6-day-old rat pups was studied by testing their ability to direct their ingestive behavior to a restricted food source. We found that, from 3 days of age, pups were able to feed efficiently from such a source. More specifically, pups that were deprived of nutrition but not of maternal care as well as pups that were dehydrated ingested significantly more than nondeprived animals, and did so whether liquid diet was spread over the entire floor surface beneath them or restricted to a fraction of the floor surface. However, pups that had been nutritionally and maternally deprived were not able to direct their feeding. The general locomotor activation of pups in this latter group appeared to interfere with their ability to direct their behavior to the restricted source. These results indicate that from early ages, developing rats possess the appetitive competence to guide their behavior and suggest that previous findings of poorly directed behavior were a confound of the behavioral activation shown by pups tested in a state of maternal deprivation.


Assuntos
Animais Recém-Nascidos/psicologia , Comportamento Apetitivo , Comportamento Alimentar , Motivação , Animais , Peso Corporal , Feminino , Masculino , Privação Materna , Ratos , Meio Social
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...