Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 8 de 8
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Infancy ; 26(6): 1057-1075, 2021 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34569704

RESUMO

It has been shown that infants can increase or modify a motorically available behavior such as sucking, kicking, arm waving, etc., in response to a positive visual reinforcement (e.g., DeCasper & Fifer, 1980; Millar, 1990; Rochat & Striano, 1999; Rovee-Collier, 1997; Watson & Ramey, 1972). We tested infants to determine if they would also change their vocal behavior in response to contingent feedback, which lacks the social, emotional, and auditory modeling typical of parent-child interaction. Here, we show that in a single five-minute session infants increase the rate of their vocalizations in order to control the appearance of colorful shapes on an iPad screen. This is the first experimental study to demonstrate that infants can rapidly learn to increase their vocalizations, when given positive reinforcement with no social element. This work sets the foundations for future studies into the causal relationship between the number of early vocalizations and the onset of words. In addition, there are potential clinical applications for reinforcing vocal practice in infant populations who are at risk for poor language skills.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem , Voz , Humanos , Lactente , Relações Pais-Filho , Reforço Psicológico , Recompensa
2.
Appl Acoust ; 162: 107183, 2020 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32362663

RESUMO

This project set out to develop an app for infants under one year of age that responds in real time to language-like infant utterances with attractive images on an iPad screen. Language-like vocalisations were defined as voiced utterances which were not high pitched squeals, nor shouts. The app, BabblePlay, was intended for use in psycholinguistic research to investigate the possible causal relationship between early canonical babble and early onset of word production. It is also designed for a clinical setting, (1) to illustrate the importance of feedback as a way to encourage infant vocalisations, and (2) to provide consonant production practice for infant populations that do not vocalise enough or who vocalise in an atypical way, specifically, autistic infants (once they have begun to produce consonants). This paper describes the development and testing of BabblePlay, which responds to an infant's vocalisations with colourful moving shapes on the screen that are analogous to some features of the infant's vocalization including loudness and duration. Validation testing showed high correlation between the app and two human judges in identifying vocalisations in 200 min of BabblePlay recordings, and a feasibility study conducted with 60 infants indicates that they can learn the contingency between their vocalisations and the appearance of shapes on the screen in one five minute BabblePlay session. BabblePlay meets the specification of being a simple and easy-to-use app. It has been shown to be a promising tool for research on infant language development that could lead to its use in home and professional environments to demonstrate the importance of immediate reward for vocal utterances to increase vocalisations in infants.

3.
Ear Hear ; 41(4): 847-854, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31613822

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Bone-conducted vestibular evoked myogenic potentials (VEMPs) are tuned to have their maximum amplitude in response to tone bursts at or below 250 Hz. The low-frequency limitations of clinical bone vibrators have not been established for transient, tone burst stimuli at frequencies that are optimal for eliciting VEMPs. DESIGN: Tone bursts with frequencies of 250 to 2000 Hz were delivered to B71 and B81 bone vibrators and their output was examined using an artificial mastoid. The lower-frequency limit of the transducers was evaluated by examining the spectral output of the bone vibrators. Maximum output levels were evaluated by measuring input-output functions across a range of stimulus levels. RESULTS: Both the B71 and B81 could produce transient tone bursts with frequency as low as 400 Hz. However, tone bursts with frequencies of 250 and 315 Hz resulted in output with peak spectral energy at approximately 400 Hz. From 500 to 2000 Hz, maximum output levels within the linear range were between 120 and 128 dB peak force level. The newer B81 bone vibrator had a maximum output approximately 5 dB higher than the B71 at several frequencies. CONCLUSIONS: These findings demonstrate that both transducers can reach levels appropriate to elicit bone-conducted VEMPs, but the low-frequency limitations of these clinical bone vibrators limit tone burst frequency to approximately 400 Hz when attempting to stimulate the otolith organs via tone bursts.


Assuntos
Potenciais Evocados Miogênicos Vestibulares , Vestíbulo do Labirinto , Estimulação Acústica , Humanos , Processo Mastoide
4.
Front Psychol ; 7: 715, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27242624

RESUMO

This study suggests that familiarity and novelty preferences in infant experimental tasks can in some instances be interpreted together as a single indicator of language advance. We provide evidence to support this idea based on our use of the auditory headturn preference paradigm to record responses to words likely to be either familiar or unfamiliar to infants. Fifty-nine 10-month-old infants were tested. The task elicited mixed preferences: familiarity (longer average looks to the words likely to be familiar to the infants), novelty (longer average looks to the words likely to be unfamiliar) and no-preference (similar-length of looks to both type of words). The infants who exhibited either a familiarity or a novelty response were more advanced on independent indices of phonetic advance than the infants who showed no preference. In addition, infants exhibiting novelty responses were more lexically advanced than either the infants who exhibited familiarity or those who showed no-preference. The results provide partial support for Hunter and Ames' (1988) developmental model of attention in infancy and suggest caution when interpreting studies indexed to chronological age.

5.
J Child Lang ; 41(1): 226-39, 2014 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23253168

RESUMO

Previous studies have shown that by 11 but not by 10 months infants recognize words that have become familiar from everyday life independently of the experimental setting. This study explored the ability of 10-, 11-, and 12-month-old infants to recognize familiar words in sentential context, without experimental training. The headturn preference procedure was used to contrast passages containing words likely to be familiar to the infants with passages containing words unlikely to have been previously heard. Two stimulus words were inserted near the beginning and end of each of a set of simple sentence frames. The ability to recognize the familiar words within sentences emerged only at 12 months of age. The contrast between segmentation abilities as they emerge as a result of everyday exposure to language, as assessed here, and those abilities as measured in studies in which words are experimentally trained is discussed in terms of memory-based mechanisms.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Compreensão , Fatores Etários , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Psicologia da Criança , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Vocabulário
6.
Infant Behav Dev ; 36(4): 642-9, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23911593

RESUMO

This study compared the preference of 27 British English- and 26 Welsh-learning infants for nonwords featuring consonants that occur with equal frequency in the input but that are produced either with equal frequency (Welsh) or with differing frequency (British English) in infant vocalizations. For the English infants a significant difference in looking times was related to the extent of production of the nonword consonants. The Welsh infants, who showed no production preference for either consonant, exhibited no such influence of production patterns on their response to the nonwords. The results are consistent with a previous study that suggested that pre-linguistic babbling helps shape the processing of input speech, serving as an articulatory filter that selectively makes production patterns more salient in the input.


Assuntos
Linguagem Infantil , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Fala/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Idioma , Masculino , Fonética , Medida da Produção da Fala
7.
Infant Behav Dev ; 34(4): 590-601, 2011 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21774986

RESUMO

The headturn preference procedure was used to test 18 infants on their response to three different passages chosen to reflect their individual production patterns. The passages contained nonwords with consonants in one of three categories: (a) often produced by that infant ('own'), (b) rarely produced by that infant but common at that age ('other'), and (c) not generally produced by infants. Infants who had a single 'own' consonant showed no significant preference for either 'own' (a) or 'other' (b) passages. In contrast, infants' with two 'own' consonants exhibited greater attention to 'other' passages (b). Both groups attended equally to the passage featuring consonants rarely produced by infants of that age (c). An analysis of a sample of the infant-directed speech ruled out the mothers' speech as a source of the infant preferences. The production-based shift to a focus on the 'other' passage suggests that nascent production abilities combine with emergent perceptual experience to facilitate word learning.


Assuntos
Atenção , Linguagem Infantil , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Fala/fisiologia , Humanos , Lactente , Acústica da Fala
8.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 53(5): 1280-93, 2010 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20631231

RESUMO

PURPOSE: In this study, the authors looked for effects of vocal practice on phonological working memory. METHOD: A longitudinal design was used, combining both naturalistic observations and a nonword repetition test. Fifteen 26-month-olds (12 of whom were followed from age 11 months) were administered a nonword test including real words, "standard" nonwords (identical for all children), and nonwords based on individual children's production inventory (in and out words). RESULTS: A strong relationship was found between (a) length of experience with consonant production and (b) nonword repetition and between (a) differential experience with specific consonants through production and (b) performance on the in versus out words. CONCLUSIONS: Performance depended on familiarity with words or their subunits and was strongest for real words, weaker for in words, and weakest for out words. The results demonstrate the important role of speech production in the construction of phonological working memory.


Assuntos
Linguagem Infantil , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Memória de Curto Prazo , Prática Psicológica , Fala , Aprendizagem Verbal , Análise de Variância , Pré-Escolar , Discriminação Psicológica , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Fonética
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...