Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Behav Res Methods ; 45(4): 1246-58, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23468181

RESUMO

This article introduces EsPal: a Web-accessible repository containing a comprehensive set of properties of Spanish words. EsPal is based on an extensible set of data sources, beginning with a 300 million token written database and a 460 million token subtitle database. Properties available include word frequency, orthographic structure and neighborhoods, phonological structure and neighborhoods, and subjective ratings such as imageability. Subword structure properties are also available in terms of bigrams and trigrams, biphones, and bisyllables. Lemma and part-of-speech information and their corresponding frequencies are also indexed. The website enables users either to upload a set of words to receive their properties or to receive a set of words matching constraints on the properties. The properties themselves are easily extensible and will be added over time as they become available. It is freely available from the following website: http://www.bcbl.eu/databases/espal/ .


Assuntos
Bases de Dados Factuais , Idioma , Processamento de Linguagem Natural , Fonética , Vocabulário , Apresentação de Dados , Humanos , Internet , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Espanha , Fala , Interface para o Reconhecimento da Fala
3.
Psychol Res ; 71(3): 240-51, 2007 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16957956

RESUMO

Using a metric shortcut paradigm, we have found that like honeybees (Dyer in Animal Behaviour 41:239-246, 1991), humans do not seem to build a metric "cognitive map" from path integration. Instead, observers take novel shortcuts based on visual landmarks whenever they are available and reliable (Foo, Warren, Duchon, & Tarr in Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition 31(2):195-215, 2005). In the present experiment we examine whether humans, like ants (Wolf & Wehner in Journal of Experimental Biology 203:857-868, 2000), first use survey-type path knowledge, built up from path integration, and then subsequently shift to reliance on landmarks. In our study participants walked in an immersive virtual environment while head position and orientation were recorded. During training, participants learned two legs of a triangle with feedback: paths from Home to Red and Home to Blue. A configuration of colored posts surrounded the Red location. To test reliance on landmarks, these posts were covertly translated, rotated, or left unchanged during six probe trials. These probe trials were interspersed during the training procedure to measure changes over learning. Dependence on visual landmarks was immediate and sustained during training, and no significant learning effects were observed other than a decrease in hesitation time. Our results suggest that while humans have at least two distinct navigational strategies available to them, unlike ants, a computationally-simpler landmark strategy dominates during novel shortcut navigation.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Meio Ambiente , Comportamento Espacial , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória , Percepção Espacial , Interface Usuário-Computador
4.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 31(2): 195-215, 2005 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15755239

RESUMO

Do humans integrate experience on specific routes into metric survey knowledge of the environment, or do they depend on a simpler strategy of landmark navigation? The authors tested this question using a novel shortcut paradigm during walking in a virtual environment. The authors find that participants could not take successful shortcuts in a desert world but could do so with dispersed landmarks in a forest. On catch trials, participants were drawn toward the displaced landmarks whether the landmarks were clustered near the target location or along the shortcut route. However, when landmarks appeared unreliable, participants fell back on coarse survey knowledge. Like honeybees (F. C. Dyer, 1991), humans do not appear to derive accurate cognitive maps from path integration to guide navigation but, instead, depend on landmarks when they are available.


Assuntos
Cognição , Comportamento Exploratório , Comportamento Espacial , Adolescente , Adulto , Meio Ambiente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção Espacial , Interface Usuário-Computador
5.
Psychol Sci ; 13(3): 272-8, 2002 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12009050

RESUMO

Honeybees fly down the center of a corridor by equating the speed of optic flow in the lateral field of the two eyes. This flow-equalization strategy has been successfully implemented in mobile robots to guide behavior in cluttered environments. We investigated whether humans use a similar strategy to steer down a corridor and determined the relative contributions of equating the speed of flow (.27), the splay angles of base lines (.62), and the visual angles of texture on the left and right walls (.03) to steering behavior. A generalized equalization strategy based on the weighted linear combination of these variables closely models human behavior, providing robust visual control.


Assuntos
Abelhas/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Robótica , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento/fisiologia , Humanos
6.
Perception ; 31(3): 349-74, 2002.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11954696

RESUMO

We examined the roles of information from optic flow and body senses (eg vestibular and proprioceptive information) for path integration, using a triangle completion task in a virtual environment. In two experiments, the contribution of optic flow was isolated by using a joystick control. Five circular arenas were used for testing: (B) both floor and wall texture; (F) floor texture only, reducing information for rotation; (W) wall texture only, reducing information for translation; (N) a no texture control condition, and (P) an array of posts. The results indicate that humans can use optic flow for path integration and are differentially influenced by rotational and translational flow. In a third experiment, participants actively walked in arenas B, F, and N, so body senses were also available. Performance shifted from a pattern of underturning to overturning and exhibited decreased variability, similar responses with and without optic flow, and no attrition. The results indicate that path integration can be performed by integrating optic flow, but when information from body senses is available it appears to dominate.


Assuntos
Sensação/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Animais , Gráficos por Computador , Feminino , Comportamento de Retorno ao Território Vital/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Interface Usuário-Computador
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...