Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 25
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Pharmacol Exp Ther ; 318(1): 223-9, 2006 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16571623

RESUMO

Human polydrug abusers often take combinations of opioids and stimulants, but it is not clear why. Behavioral economics with demand curve analysis is uniquely able to separate two of the possibilities: that the drug combination increases the reinforcing potency of the component drugs or that the drug combination is a more effective reinforcer than either drug alone. Rhesus monkeys self-administered a range of doses of cocaine, remifentanil, and combinations of the drugs through indwelling intravenous catheters; the number of responses required for each drug infusion increased across drug-availability sessions. Combining small doses of cocaine and remifentanil that by themselves resulted in very low rates of responding yielded rates of responding that were higher than the maximum maintained by any dose of the constituent drugs. Nevertheless, demand curve analysis demonstrated that the drug combination was equally elastic as the component drugs, indicating that it was not more effective as a reinforcer than either cocaine or remifentanil alone. This suggests that enhanced self-administration of this particular drug combination is due primarily to the drug enhancement of the potency of the other drug.


Assuntos
Cocaína/administração & dosagem , Piperidinas/administração & dosagem , Tempo de Reação/efeitos dos fármacos , Reforço Psicológico , Animais , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Combinação de Medicamentos , Feminino , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Remifentanil
2.
Acta Astronaut ; 56(9-12): 937-48, 2005.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15835052

RESUMO

The present report describes the development and application of a distributed interactive multi-person simulation in a computer-generated planetary environment as an experimental test bed for modeling the human performance effects of variations in the types of communication modes available, and in the types of stress and incentive conditions underlying the completion of mission goals. The results demonstrated a high degree of interchangeability between communication modes(audio, text) when one mode was not available. Additionally, the addition of time pressure stress to complete tasks resulted in a reduction in performance effectiveness, and these performance reductions were ameliorated via the introduction of positive incentives contingent upon improved performances. The results obtained confirmed that cooperative and productive psychosocial interactions can be maintained between individually isolated and dispersed members of simulated spaceflight crews communicating and problem-solving effectively over extended time intervals without the benefit of one another's physical presence.


Assuntos
Astronautas/psicologia , Comunicação , Simulação por Computador , Processos Grupais , Voo Espacial , Simulação de Ambiente Espacial/psicologia , Comportamento Cooperativo , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Masculino , Isolamento Social , Estresse Psicológico , Interface Usuário-Computador
3.
J Pharmacol Exp Ther ; 301(2): 690-7, 2002 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11961074

RESUMO

The potential contribution of onset and duration of pharmacological action to the reinforcing strength of three intravenously delivered N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonists was evaluated in this study. The onsets and durations of action of ketamine, phencyclidine, and dizocilpine were evaluated by observation and tabulation of their behavioral effects in rhesus monkeys after i.v. administration. The reinforcing effects of each drug were tested in a paradigm in which the fixed ratio requirements for i.v. drug injection were increased systematically. The peak observable effect of ketamine occurred immediately after its administration. There were some immediately observable effects of phencyclidine, although the peak effect of phencyclidine was delayed for 3 to 10 min. Dizocilpine had few immediate effects and a peak effect 32 min after administration. Ketamine had the shortest duration of action, followed by phencyclidine and dizocilpine. Analysis of demand curves and response output curves that were normalized to account for potency differences among the drugs revealed that ketamine and phencyclidine were equally effective as reinforcers, and they were both much stronger reinforcers than was dizocilpine. The data therefore suggest that a fast onset of action increases the reinforcing strength of drugs, although duration of action may play a role as well.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Antagonistas de Aminoácidos Excitatórios/farmacologia , Receptores de N-Metil-D-Aspartato/antagonistas & inibidores , Anestesia , Animais , Maleato de Dizocilpina/administração & dosagem , Maleato de Dizocilpina/farmacologia , Antagonistas de Aminoácidos Excitatórios/administração & dosagem , Feminino , Injeções Intravenosas , Ketamina/administração & dosagem , Ketamina/farmacologia , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Fenciclidina/administração & dosagem , Fenciclidina/farmacologia , Autoadministração
4.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 76(3): 245-63, 2001 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11768710

RESUMO

Each of 2 monkeys typically earned their daily food ration by depositing tokens in one of two slots. Tokens deposited in one slot dropped into a bin where they were kept (token kept). Deposits to a second slot dropped into a bin where they could be obtained again (token returned). In Experiment 1, a fixed-ratio (FR) 5 schedule that provided two food pellets was associated with each slot. Both monkeys preferred the token-returned slot. In Experiment 2, both subjects chose between unequal FR schedules with the token-returned slot always associated with the leaner schedule. When the FRs were 2 versus 3 and 2 versus 6, preferences were maintained for the token-returned slot; however, when the ratios were 2 versus 12, preference shifted to the token-kept slot. In Experiment 3, both monkeys chose between equal-valued concurrent variable-interval variable-interval schedules. Both monkeys preferred the slot that returned tokens. In Experiment 4, both monkeys chose between FRs that typically differed in size by a factor of 10. Both monkeys preferred the FR schedule that provided more food per trial. These data show that monkeys will choose so as to increase the number of reinforcers earned (stock optimizing) even when this preference reduces the rate of reinforcement (all reinforcers divided by session time).


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Condicionamento Operante/fisiologia , Reforço por Recompensa , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Esquema de Reforço
5.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 65(2): 401-22, 1996 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8851540

RESUMO

Three experiments were conducted to study the effect of an imperfect substitute for food on demand for food in a closed economy. In Experiments 1 and 2, rats pressed a lever for their entire daily food ration, and a fixed ratio of presses was required for each food pellet. In both experiments, the fixed ratio was held constant during a daily session but was increased between sessions. The fixed ratio was increased over a series of daily sessions once in the absence of concurrently available sucrose and again when sucrose pellets were freely available. For both series, increases in the fixed ratio reduced food intake, but body weight was reduced only in the no-sucrose condition. In the sucrose condition, body weight and total caloric intake (sucrose plus food) were relatively unaffected by increases in the fixed ratio. At all fixed ratios, food intake was proportionally reduced by the intake of sucrose. In Experiment 3, monkeys obtained food or saccharin by pressing keys; the fixed ratio of presses per food pellet was increased once when tap water was each monkey's only source of fluid, again when each monkey's water was sweetened with saccharin, and a third time when each monkey had concurrent access to the saccharin solution and plain water. Increases in the fixed ratio, but not the intake of the saccharin solution, reduced each monkey's food intake. Because neither rats' sucrose nor monkeys' saccharin intakes affected the slope of the respective demand curves for food, monkeys and rats increased their daily output of presses and thereby defended their daily intake of those complementary elements of food. However, sucrose reduced rats' food intake. The relative constancy of body weight and total caloric intake in the sucrose condition is consistent with the possibility that rats tended to regulate caloric intake.


Assuntos
Regulação do Apetite , Comportamento Apetitivo , Ingestão de Energia , Motivação , Animais , Peso Corporal , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Esquema de Reforço , Sacarose/administração & dosagem , Paladar
6.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 64(3): 373-84, 1995 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8551194

RESUMO

The concepts of behavioral economics have proven to be useful for understanding the environmental control of overall levels of responding for a variety of commodities, including reinforcement by drug self-administration. These general concepts have implications for the assessment of abuse liability and drug abuse intervention and the formulation of public policy on drug abuse. An essential requirement is the ability to compare the demand for different drugs directly in order to assess relative abuse liability, and to compare demand for the same drug under different environmental and biological interventions to assess their ability to reduce demand. Until now, such comparisons were hampered by the confounding effect of varying drug doses and potencies that prevent quantitative comparisons of demand elasticity--sensitivity of consumption and responding to the constraint of price (effort). In this paper we describe a procedure to normalize demand-curve analysis that permits dose- and potency-independent comparisons of demand across drugs. The procedure is shown to be effective for comparing drug demand within and across the drug classes. The technique permits a quantitative ordering of demand that is consistent with the peak levels of responding maintained by the drugs. The same technique is generalized for the comparison of other types of reinforcers under different biological conditions.


Assuntos
Alfentanil/economia , Alfentanil/farmacologia , Cocaína/economia , Cocaína/farmacologia , Macaca mulatta , Metoexital/economia , Metoexital/farmacologia , Nalbufina/economia , Nalbufina/farmacologia , Fenciclidina/economia , Fenciclidina/farmacologia , Reforço Psicológico , Alfentanil/administração & dosagem , Animais , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Cocaína/administração & dosagem , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Metoexital/administração & dosagem , Nalbufina/administração & dosagem , Fenciclidina/administração & dosagem , Autoadministração
7.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 33(2): 165-72, 1993 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8261881

RESUMO

Behavioral economics provides a set of concepts for the analysis of factors that control the allocation of behavioral resources among available reinforcers. Terms from micro-economics describe new phenomena previously ignored within the traditional context of behavior analysis. This article reviews these concepts as an introduction to the three papers that follow. The primary dependent measure within the behavioral economic framework is the level of consumption of available commodities as determined by the level and distribution of instrumental responding. The demand curve provides a quantitative metric for analyzing consumption under the constraint of unit price. When the reinforcer is a drug, the demand curve can be a useful tool for analyzing the level of motivation to consume the drug, its abuse liability, and for evaluating interventions, such as alternative reinforcers or medications, to reduce the motivation to consume the drug and instrumental responding to obtain it. Behavioral economics also provides a framework for formulating, testing, and refining drug abuse policy through a series of empirical steps that maximize effectiveness and minimize undesirable social consequences.


Assuntos
Drogas Ilícitas , Motivação , Psicotrópicos , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/psicologia , Animais , Extinção Psicológica , Humanos , Reforço Psicológico
8.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 56(2): 377-93, 1991 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1955823

RESUMO

The concepts of behavioral economics have proven useful for understanding the environmental control of overall levels of responding for a variety of commodities, including reinforcement by drug self-administration. These general concepts are summarized for application to the analysis of drug-reinforced behavior and proposed as the basis for future applications. This behavioral agenda includes the assessment of abuse liability, the assay of drug-reinforcer interactions, the design of drug abuse interventions, and the formulation of drug abuse public policy. These separate domains of investigation are described as part of an overall strategy for designing model projects to control drug use and testing public policy initiatives.


Assuntos
Psicotrópicos , Esquema de Reforço , Autoadministração/psicologia , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/psicologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Humanos , Política Pública
9.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 53(2): 263-71, 1990 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2324666

RESUMO

In Experiment 1, 4 rats earned their daily food ration by choosing between two levers. One lever delivered two regular and one quinine-adulterated food pellets, and the other delivered two regular and four quinine pellets. A 20-s intertrial interval separated successive choices. Sessions began with 10 forced trials during which only one lever, selected with p = .5 and cued by a light above it, could deliver its reinforcer. Forced trials were followed by 30 or 150 trials, depending on the condition, during which choices to either lever could be reinforced. Over this range, absolute choice of the four-quinine, two-regular-pellet lever was inversely related to the number of free-choice trials, establishing this reinforcer as an inferior good. In Condition 1 of Experiment 2, the prior design was altered in two ways: (a) one lever delivered four quinine pellets, and the other lever delivered one standard pellet; and (b) sessions ended after 140 free-choice trials. When the number of free-choice trials was reduced to 100 (Condition 2), all 3 rats increased their preference for quinine pellets, confirming their status as an inferior good. In the next several conditions, the number of quinine pellets provided for selecting its associated lever was varied between three and four. Preference for the quinine-pellet alternative was inversely related to the number of pellets it provided, a result defining it as a Giffen good. These findings are not accommodated readily by extant choice models and complicate the search for a unitary model of choice.


Assuntos
Comportamento Apetitivo , Comportamento de Escolha , Motivação , Paladar , Animais , Masculino , Quinina , Ratos , Ratos Endogâmicos
10.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 53(1): 155-61, 1990 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2299285

RESUMO

In Experiment 1, 4 rats earned their daily food ration by choosing on a trials basis between a "risky" and a "riskless" lever. The risky lever produced either 15 45-mg food pellets or no pellets, and on average provided five pellets per choice. The riskless lever always produced three pellets. Across conditions, the number of trials per session was varied. Body weight and choice of the risky lever decreased as the number of trials per session decreased, even though body weight could only be defended by increased choice of the risky lever. In Experiment 2, trials per session were fixed, but the number of pellets delivered by the risky and riskless levers was either at the same level as in Experiment 1 or tripled from those levels. Now choice of the risky lever was inversely related to the size of reinforcement and to body weight. The results of these experiments show that risk aversion covaries with the amount of food available in a session and the daily variance in the amount of food earned.


Assuntos
Comportamento Apetitivo , Comportamento de Escolha , Motivação , Assunção de Riscos , Animais , Masculino , Ratos , Ratos Endogâmicos , Esquema de Reforço
11.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 52(1): 41-6, 1989 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812589

RESUMO

Three mice chose between concurrent variable-ratio variable-interval schedules to produce a warm air stream while they were housed in a cold chamber. Across conditions, the duration of the warm air stream was varied between 10 and 80 s and was equal for both schedules. Preference for the VI schedule covaried with reinforcer duration as predicted by maximizing accounts of choice.

12.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 50(3): 419-40, 1988 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3209958

RESUMO

Laboratory studies of consumer demand theory require assumptions regarding the definition of price in the absence of a medium of exchange (money). In this study we test the proposition that the fundamental dimension of price is a cost-benefit ratio expressed as the effort expended per unit of food value consumed. Using rats as subjects, we tested the generality of this "unit price" concept by varying four dimensions of price: fixed-ratio schedule, number of food pellets per fixed-ratio completion, probability of reinforcement, and response lever weight or effort. Two levels of the last three factors were combined in a 2 x 2 x 2 design giving eight groups. Each group was studied under a series of six FR schedules. Using the nominal values of all factors to determine unit price, we found that grams of food consumed plotted as a function of unit price followed a single demand curve. Similarly, total work output (responses x effort) conformed to a single function when plotted in terms of unit price. These observations provided a template for interpreting the effects of biological factors, such as brain lesions or drugs, that might alter the cost-benefit ratio.


Assuntos
Comportamento Apetitivo , Motivação , Esquema de Reforço , Animais , Condicionamento Operante , Comportamento Alimentar , Masculino , Aprendizagem por Probabilidade , Ratos , Ratos Endogâmicos
13.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 50(3): 359-60, 1988 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812568
14.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 42(3): 435-52, 1984 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812401

RESUMO

Economics, like behavioral psychology, is a science of behavior, albeit highly organized human behavior. The value of economic concepts for behavioral psychology rests on (1) their empirical validity when tested in the laboratory with individual subjects and (2) their uniqueness when compared to established behavioral concepts. Several fundamental concepts are introduced and illustrated by reference to experimental data: open and closed economies, elastic and inelastic demand, and substitution versus complementarity. Changes in absolute response rate are analyzed in relation to elasticity and intensity of demand. The economic concepts of substitution and complementarity are related to traditional behavioral studies of choice and to the matching relation. The economic approach has many implications for the future of behavioral research and theory. In general, economic concepts are grounded on a dynamic view of reinforcement. The closed-economy methodology extends the generality of behavioral principles to situations in which response rate and obtained rate of reinforcement are interdependent. Analysis of results in terms of elasticity and intensity of demand promises to provide a more direct method for characterizing the effects of "motivational" variables. Future studies of choice should arrange heterogeneous reinforcers with varying elasticities, use closed economies, and modulate scarcity or income. The economic analysis can be extended to the study of performances that involve subtle discriminations or skilled movements that vary in accuracy or quality as opposed to rate or quantity, and thus permit examination of time/accuracy trade-offs.

16.
Behav Neural Biol ; 40(1): 1-4, 1984 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6732703

RESUMO

Monkeys that were required to repeatedly learn new sequences of responses to obtain food were injected with 0.2 mg/kg of atropine sulfate or methylatropine nitrate. Effects lasted 8 to 12 hr following injection. Both drugs decreased the rate at which the animals worked, but only atropine sulfate increased the number of attempts required to solve the problem and decreased overall accuracy, suggesting a peripheral mode of action for rate-decreasing effects, and a central mode of action for effects of atropine on qualitative aspects of performance.


Assuntos
Derivados da Atropina/farmacologia , Atropina/farmacologia , Encéfalo/efeitos dos fármacos , Condicionamento Operante/efeitos dos fármacos , Memória/efeitos dos fármacos , Rememoração Mental/efeitos dos fármacos , Nervos Periféricos/efeitos dos fármacos , Animais , Humanos , Macaca mulatta , Desempenho Psicomotor/efeitos dos fármacos , Aprendizagem Seriada/efeitos dos fármacos
18.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 34(2): 219-38, 1980 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812188

RESUMO

A review of the relationship between schedule of reinforcement, response rate, and choice suggests that certain unifying concepts from economics can contribute to a more complete science of behavior. Four points are made: 1) a behavioral experiment is an economic system and its characteristics-open or closed-can strongly determine the results; 2) reinforcers can be distinguished by a functional property called elasticity; 3) reinforcers may interact as complements as well as substitutes; 4) no simple choice rule, such as strict matching, can account for all choice behavior.

19.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 29(3): 475-91, 1978 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812071

RESUMO

In the first experiment, two rhesus monkeys earned their entire ration of food and water during daily sessions with no provisions to ensure constant daily intakes. Two variable-interval schedules of food presentations were concurrent with one variable-interval schedule of water presentations; the maximum rate of food presentations arranged by one food schedule was varied. As the rate of food presentations was increased, the absolute level of responding on the two food schedules combined decreased, while responding on the water schedule increased. The preference for the variable food schedule compared to the other food schedule approximately matched the proportion of reinforcers obtained from it. The preference for the variable food schedule compared to the water schedule did not match, but greatly decreased, as the proportion of reinforcers from the food schedule increased. When Experiment I was replicated, with provisions to ensure constant daily intakes of food and water (Experiment II), the absolute response rates under the two food schedules combined and under the water schedule no longer changed with increases in the rate of food during the sessions. On the other hand, choice between the two food schedules remained proportional to the distribution of obtained food pellets. These results were interpreted as indicating that behavior to obtain nonsubstitutable commodities, such as food and water, is strongly controlled by the economic conditions of daily consumption, while choice between substitutable commodities is independent of these factors.

20.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 27(2): 315-26, 1977 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811993

RESUMO

Three monkeys were trained to emit a chain of three responses on three separate levers in a set of six levers to obtain food. The chain producing food (correct chain) was changed each day. During a trial, a press on any lever produced a feedback stimulus; a press on a correct lever produced an additional distinctive stimulus; the third correct press produced a food pellet. Test sessions in which either the food or the distinctive stimuli were removed were interspersed with baseline sessions. In tests without food presentations, the subjects acquired the correct chain rapidly, with a level of accuracy comparable to baseline. Removing the distintive stimuli for either the first or second member of the correct chain greatly retarded acquisition of that member of the chain. Removing all distinctive stimuli often reduced accuracy throughout the chain to chance level, even though food was presented following each correct chain. These results were interpreted as evidence that the distinctive stimuli presented after correct responses functioned as conditioned reinforcers. Reductions in accuracy following an omitted distinctive stimulus indicated that they were also discriminative stimuli for correct responding in their presence.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...