Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Dev Neuropsychol ; 31(2): 217-38, 2007.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17488217

RESUMO

Deficits in language are a core feature of autism. The superior temporal gyrus (STG) is involved in auditory processing, including language, but also has been implicated as a critical structure in social cognition. It was hypothesized that subjects with autism would display different size-function relationships between the STG and intellectual-language-based abilities when compared to controls. Intellectual ability was assessed by either the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (WISC-III) or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (WAIS-III), where three intellectual quotients (IQ) were computed: verbal (VIQ), performance (PIQ), and full-scale (FSIQ). Language ability was assessed by the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Third Edition (CELF-3), also divided into three index scores: receptive, expressive, and total. Seven to 19-year-old rigorously diagnosed subjects with autism (n = 30) were compared to controls (n = 39; 13 of whom had a deficit in reading) of similar age who were matched on education, PIQ, and head circumference. STG volumes were computed based on 1.5 Tesla magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). IQ and CELF-3 performance were highly interrelated regardless of whether subjects had autism or were controls. Both IQ and CELF-3 ability were positively correlated with STG in controls, but a different pattern was observed in subjects with autism. In controls, left STG gray matter was significantly (r = .42, p < or = .05) related to receptive language on the CELF-3; in contrast, a zero order correlation was found with autism. When plotted by age, potential differences in growth trajectories related to language development associated with STG were observed between controls and those subjects with autism. Taken together, these findings suggest a possible failure in left hemisphere lateralization of language function involving the STG in autism.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/patologia , Transtorno Autístico/fisiopatologia , Idioma , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Inteligência/fisiologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estudos Retrospectivos
2.
Brain Dev ; 29(7): 389-99, 2007 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17204387

RESUMO

The temporal lobe is thought to be abnormal in autism, yet standard volumetric analyses are often unrevealing when age, sex, IQ, and head size are controlled. Quantification of temporal lobe structures were obtained in male subjects with autism and controls, where subjects with head circumference (HC) defined macrocephaly were excluded, so that volume differences were not just related to the higher prevalence of macrocephaly in autism. Various statistical methods were applied to the analysis including a classification and regression tree (CART) method, a non-parametric technique that helps define patterns of relationships that may be meaningful in distinguishing temporal lobe differences between subjects with autism and age and IQ matched controls. Subjects with autism were also compared to a separate control group with reading disorder (RD), with the prediction that the temporal lobe morphometric analysis of the reading disorder controls would be more similar to that of the autism group. The CART method yielded a high specificity in classifying autism subjects from controls based on the relationship between the volume of the left fusiform gyrus (LFG) gray and white matter, the right temporal stem (RTS) and the right inferior temporal gyrus gray matter (RITG-GM). Reading disordered individuals were more similar to subjects with autism. Simple size differences did not distinguish the groups. These findings demonstrate different relationships within temporal lobe structures that distinguish subjects with autism from controls. Results are discussed in terms of pathological connectivity within the temporal lobe as it relates to autism.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/patologia , Análise de Regressão , Lobo Temporal/patologia , Adolescente , Dislexia/patologia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino
3.
Hum Hered ; 60(4): 220-6, 2005.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16391490

RESUMO

Though autism shows strong evidence for genetic etiology, specific genes have not yet been found. We tested for linkage in a candidate region on chromosome 3q25-27 first identified in Finnish autism families [1]. The peak in this previous study was at D3S3037 (183.9 cM). We tested this region in seven affected family members and 24 of their relatives from a single large extended Utah pedigree of Northern European ancestry. A total of 70 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were analyzed from 165 to 204 cM. The maximum NPL-all nonparametric score using SimWalk2snp was 3.53 (empirical p val ue = 0.0003) at 185.2 cM (SNP rs1402229), close to the Finnish peak. A secondary analysis using MCLINK supported this result, with a maximum of 3.92 at 184.6 cM (SNP rs1362645). We tested for alterations in a candidate gene in this region, the fragile X autosomal homolog, FXR1. No variants likely to contribute to autism were found in the coding sequence, exon-intron boundaries, or the promoter region of this gene.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/genética , Mapeamento Cromossômico , Cromossomos Humanos Par 3/genética , Ligação Genética/genética , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Mapeamento Cromossômico/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Linhagem , Proteínas de Ligação a RNA/genética , Utah , População Branca
4.
Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am ; 12(1): 107-22, 2003 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12512401

RESUMO

This article outlines the ingredients the authors feel are critical to making social skills interventions successful for children with autism spectrum disorders. The authors described basic principles for teaching social skills that capitalize on the strengths of such children, while specifically addressing their deficits. The authors applied these widely used principles to group social skills intervention. In particular, social skills groups for children with ASD need to break down complex social behaviors into concrete steps and rules that can be memorized and practiced in a variety of settings. Abstract concepts must be made concrete through a variety of visual, tangible, "hands-on" activities that make socialization fun. Visual structure and predictable routines are essential. Also critical to the success of social skills intervention are instruction and activities that provide necessary support for the language abilities of the participants. A variety of learning opportunities must be used to teach the goals and skills most relevant to children with ASD. These skills must be integrated as intervention progresses. Furthermore, interactions that require the children to focus on peers create a positive social group culture. Within this culture and environment, self-awareness and positive self-esteem can be fostered. A behavior plan that specifies individual goals for group members and a specific system for delivering rewards should be included. Other important ingredients include generalization, which is encouraged through community outings, skill practice in more naturalistic settings, and collaboration with parents and teachers to work on skills outside the group intervention. Weekly therapy does little to change basic deficits of ASD unless there is daily practice and reinforcement of the skills being learned in more natural situations. The authors hope that outlining these principles and specific techniques will encourage more clinicians to offer social skills groups and thus increase their availability around the nation and world. Continued research and treatment for social skills is necessary to provide much needed empiric evidence to determine effectiveness of such interventions.


Assuntos
Síndrome de Asperger/terapia , Transtorno Autístico/terapia , Terapia Comportamental/métodos , Comportamento Social , Adolescente , Síndrome de Asperger/diagnóstico , Síndrome de Asperger/psicologia , Transtorno Autístico/diagnóstico , Transtorno Autístico/psicologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Comunicação , Processos Grupais , Humanos , Autoimagem , Socialização
5.
Am J Med Genet ; 113(3): 231-7, 2002 Dec 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12439889

RESUMO

The broader autism phenotype (BAP) is a subclinical set of personality and other features that is thought to index familiality and/or genetic liability to autism. Eighteen parents of autistic probands with a history of language regression and 70 parents of autistic probands without regression were assessed for features of the BAP and compared with published rates in parents of nonautistic subjects. Parents of probands with regressive and nonregressive autism demonstrated similar rates of the BAP (27.8% vs. 32.9%; P = 0.33). The rate of the BAP was significantly higher in both groups of autism parents than in parents of nonautistic subjects (P < or = 0.01). Thus, this measure of genetic liability is increased equally in families with both forms of autism when compared with controls. Environmental events are therefore unlikely to be the sole cause of regressive autism in our sample. Environmental events, however, may act in an additive or "second-hit" fashion in individuals with a genetic vulnerability to autism.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/psicologia , Regressão Psicológica , Adulto , Transtorno Autístico/genética , Transtorno Autístico/fisiopatologia , Constituição Corporal , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...