Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 104(6): 976-94, 2013 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23627747

RESUMO

People often hold inflated views of their performance on intellectual tasks, with poor performers exhibiting the most inflation. What leads to such excessive confidence? We suggest that the more people approach such tasks in a "rational" (i.e., consistent, algorithmic) manner, relative to those who use more variable or ad hoc approaches, the more confident they become, irrespective of whether they are reaching correct judgments. In 6 studies, participants completed tests involving logical reasoning, intuitive physics, or financial investment. Those more consistent in their approach to the task rated their performances more positively, including those consistently pursuing the wrong rule. Indeed, completely consistent but wrong participants thought almost as highly of their performance as did completely consistent and correct participants. Participants were largely aware of the rules they followed and became more confident in their performance when induced to be more systematic in their approach, no matter how misguided that approach was. In part, the link between decision consistency and (over)confidence was mediated by a neglect of alternative solutions as participants followed a more uniform approach to a task.


Assuntos
Julgamento/fisiologia , Autoavaliação (Psicologia) , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adulto , Algoritmos , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
2.
Organ Behav Hum Decis Process ; 105(1): 98-121, 2008 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19568317

RESUMO

People are typically overly optimistic when evaluating the quality of their performance on social and intellectual tasks. In particular, poor performers grossly overestimate their performances because their incompetence deprives them of the skills needed to recognize their deficits. Five studies demonstrated that poor performers lack insight into their shortcomings even in real world settings and when given incentives to be accurate. An additional meta-analysis showed that it was lack of insight into their own errors (and not mistaken assessments of their peers) that led to overly optimistic estimates among poor performers. Along the way, these studies ruled out recent alternative accounts that have been proposed to explain why poor performers hold such positive impressions of their performance.

3.
Psychol Sci ; 17(4): 305-10, 2006 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16623687

RESUMO

The hindsight bias is an inability to disregard known outcome information when estimating earlier likelihoods of that outcome. The propensity effect, a reversal of this hindsight bias, is apparently unique to judgments involving momentum and trajectory (in which there is a strongly implied propensity toward a specific outcome). In the present study, the propensity effect occurred only in judgments involving dynamic stimuli (computer animations of traffic accidents vs. text descriptions), and only when foresight judgments were temporally near to (vs. far from) a focal outcome. This research was motivated by the applied question of whether the courtroom use of computer animation increases the hindsight bias in jurors' decision making; findings revealed that the hindsight bias was more than doubled when computer animations, rather than text-plus-diagram descriptions, were used. Therefore, in addition to providing theoretical insights of relevance to cognitive, perceptual, and social psychologists, these results have important legal implications.


Assuntos
Cognição , Percepção de Movimento , Percepção Visual , Acidentes de Trânsito , Humanos , Interface Usuário-Computador
4.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 90(3): 412-25, 2006 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16594828

RESUMO

Teasing is ambiguous. Although the literal content of a tease is, by definition, negative, seldom do teasers intend for their tease to be taken literally. Toward this aim, teasers often attempt to mitigate the negative surface content of the tease by communicating via gesture, facial expression, or tone of voice that they are "just kidding." The research presented here suggests that such attempts often fall on deaf ears. Despite teasers' attempts to mitigate the tease, targets are often unaware of--and unmoved by--the teaser's benign intentions. As a result, teasers and targets systematically differ in their perceptions of teasing: Although it is often seen as innocent and playful by the teaser, it tends to be construed as considerably more malicious by the target.


Assuntos
Agressão , Relações Interpessoais , Jogos e Brinquedos , Comportamento Social , Percepção Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Comunicação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estudantes
5.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 88(5): 725-35, 2005 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15898871

RESUMO

Most people believe that they should avoid changing their answer when taking multiple-choice tests. Virtually all research on this topic, however, has suggested that this strategy is ill-founded: Most answer changes are from incorrect to correct, and people who change their answers usually improve their test scores. Why do people believe in this strategy if the data so strongly refute it? The authors argue that the belief is in part a product of counterfactual thinking. Changing an answer when one should have stuck with one's original answer leads to more "if only . . ." self-recriminations than does sticking with one's first instinct when one should have switched. As a consequence, instances of the former are more memorable than instances of the latter. This differential availability provides individuals with compelling (albeit illusory) personal evidence for the wisdom of always following their 1st instinct, with suboptimal test scores the result.


Assuntos
Cultura , Pensamento , Adulto , Afeto , Comportamento de Escolha , Avaliação Educacional , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória
6.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 89(6): 925-36, 2005 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16393025

RESUMO

Without the benefit of paralinguistic cues such as gesture, emphasis, and intonation, it can be difficult to convey emotion and tone over electronic mail (e-mail). Five experiments suggest that this limitation is often underappreciated, such that people tend to believe that they can communicate over e-mail more effectively than they actually can. Studies 4 and 5 further suggest that this overconfidence is born of egocentrism, the inherent difficulty of detaching oneself from one's own perspective when evaluating the perspective of someone else. Because e-mail communicators "hear" a statement differently depending on whether they intend to be, say, sarcastic or funny, it can be difficult to appreciate that their electronic audience may not.


Assuntos
Comunicação , Ego , Correio Eletrônico , Pensamento , Afeto , Humanos , Comunicação não Verbal
7.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 30(3): 328-39, 2004 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15030624

RESUMO

Actions and intentions do not always align. Individuals often have good intentions that they fail to fulfill. The studies presented here suggest that actors and observers differ in the weight they assign to intentions when deciding whether an individual possesses a desirable trait. Participants were more likely to give themselves credit for their intentions than they were to give others credit for theirs (Studies 1 and 2). This caused individuals to evaluate themselves more favorably than they evaluated others (Studies 3-5). Discussion focuses on the motivational and information-processing roots of this actor-observer difference in the weight assigned to intentions as well as the implications of this tendency for everyday judgment and decision making.


Assuntos
Intenção , Autoimagem , Autoavaliação (Psicologia) , Comportamento Social , Desejabilidade Social , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 85(3): 389-408, 2003 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14498778

RESUMO

Six experiments investigated people's optimism in competitions. The studies involved hypothetical and real competitions (course grades in Experiments 1 and 2, a trivia game in Experiments 3-5, and a poker game in Experiment 6) in which the presence of shared adversities and benefits (factors that would generally hinder or help the absolute performance of all competitors) was manipulated. Shared adversities tended to reduce people's subjective likelihoods of winning, whereas shared benefits tended to increase them. The findings suggest that when people judge their likelihood of winning, their assessments of their own strengths and weaknesses have greater impact than their assessments of their competitors' strengths and weaknesses. We identify egocentrism and focalism as two causes of the bias. The experiments revealed moderators of this bias, but also illustrated its robust nature across a variety of conditions.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Atitude , Comportamento Competitivo/fisiologia , Ego , Emoções/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Humanos , Julgamento/fisiologia , Autoavaliação (Psicologia) , Comportamento Social , Estudantes/psicologia
9.
Psychol Sci ; 14(5): 520-4, 2003 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12930487

RESUMO

When individuals choose future activities on the basis of their past experiences, what guides those choices? The present study compared students' predicted, on-line, and remembered spring-break experiences, as well as the influence of these factors on students' desire to take a similar vacation in the future. Predicted and remembered experiences were both more positive-and, paradoxically, more negative-than on-line experiences. Of key importance, path analyses revealed that remembered experience, but neither on-line nor anticipated experience, directly predicted the desire to repeat the experience. These results suggest that although on-line measures may be superior to retrospective measures for approximating objective experience, retrospective measures may be superior for predicting choice.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Computadores de Mão , Emoções , Atividades de Lazer , Retenção Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória de Curto Prazo , Motivação , Meio Social
10.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 82(2): 189-92, 2002 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11831409

RESUMO

J. Kruger and D. Dunning (1999) argued that the unskilled suffer a dual burden: Not only do they perform poorly, but their incompetence robs them of the metacognitive ability to realize it. J. Krueger and R. A. Mueller (2002) replicated these basic findings but interpreted them differently. They concluded that a combination of the better-than-average (BTA) effect and a regression artifact better explains why the unskilled are unaware. The authors of the present article respectfully disagree with this proposal and suggest that any interpretation of J. Krueger and R. A. Mueller's results is hampered because those authors used unreliable tests and inappropriate measures of relevant mediating variables. Additionally, a regression-BTA account cannot explain the experimental data reported in J. Kruger and D. Dunning or a reanalysis following the procedure suggested by J. Krueger and R. A. Mueller.


Assuntos
Conscientização , Cognição , Autoimagem , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...