Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 4 de 4
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Ecol Evol ; 13(9): e10476, 2023 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37706165

RESUMO

While cross-species comparisons of birds suggest that as latitude decreases or elevation increases, clutch size decreases and the duration of developmental stages and parental attentiveness increases, studies comparing populations of the same species are rare. We studied populations of house wrens, Troglodytes aedon, at high and low elevations in California and Costa Rica, collecting data on clutch size, the duration of incubation and nestling periods, parental attentiveness, nestling growth rate, and nesting success. Our data support results from cross-species comparisons, but also revealed unanticipated results from low elevation temperate zone house wrens in the southwest. This population had prolonged incubation and nestling periods similar to those found in the tropics. We also found that temperate zone females, especially those at our higher elevation site, spent more of their day incubating than did tropical females. Nest temperature at our high elevation temperate zone site was higher than that at all other tropical sites. Age at fledging did not differ between sites. Total feeding rates per chick and male feedings per chick did not vary between sites. Nest success rates showed the predicted effect of latitude, but not the predicted effects of elevation. Our results extend low elevation house wren research into the southwestern US and contribute the first intraspecific elevational comparison in the Neotropics. Data from our low elevation southwestern site present a unique suite of life history traits that align more with tropical house wrens, although with a larger clutch size, and point to food limitation and/or high predation pressure as being possible drivers of some of these differences. These results highlight the need for additional studies of house wrens and other broadly distributed species at a more diverse array of sites to better understand which forces drive the evolution of different life history strategies across major biogeographical gradients.

2.
Proc Biol Sci ; 289(1978): 20220589, 2022 07 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35858051

RESUMO

The study of song development has focused on temperate zone birds in which typically only males sing. In the bay wren, Cantorchilus nigricapillus, both sexes sing, performing precisely timed, female-initiated duets in which birds alternate sex-specific song phrases. We investigated the development of these sex differences by collecting bay wren eggs and nestlings, and hand-raising them in individual acoustic isolation chambers. Each bird was tutored with either monophonic or stereophonic recordings of bay wren duets or heard no song. As adults, each tutored bird sang repertoires of complete duets, singing both male and female phrases. In addition, some birds sang only the male or female part of some duets to which they were exposed. Mono-tutored birds showed no sex-specificity in these solo songs, whereas stereo-tutored birds only sang solos consistent with their sex. In addition, stereo-tutored birds acquired songs over a longer period than did mono-tutored birds, and stereo-tutored females showed more sex-specificity than did males during early song production. Finally, we observed that tutored and acoustically isolated birds of both sexes invented male-like songs, whereas only males invent songs in the wild. These results reveal the relative roles of environmental versus innate influences in the development of sex-specific song in this species.


Assuntos
Aves Canoras , Animais , Feminino , Aprendizagem , Masculino , Caracteres Sexuais , Vocalização Animal
3.
Biol Bull ; 242(3): 250-258, 2022 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35767413

RESUMO

AbstractCephalopods use dynamic camouflage to blend in with their environment, communicate with conspecifics, and mimic other animals by changing their skin's color, texture, pattern, and shape. Past studies have cataloged common body patterns presented by various cephalopod species to gain insight into the evolution and function of these patterns in the natural environment. The common (Sepia officinalis), pharaoh (Sepia pharaonis), and flamboyant (Metasepia pfefferi) cuttlefish are three of the previously studied species and demonstrate that differences in habitat, physical size, and evolutionary history may influence the capacity and usage of body patterning. We studied the body patterns of an additional cuttlefish species, the dwarf cuttlefish (Sepia bandensis), to investigate further what aspects of ecology or behavior may influence body patterning in cuttlefish. We captured still images and video recordings of individually housed dwarf cuttlefish and analyzed them to catalog the body pattern components displayed. We utilized a quantitative approach to determine body patterns through a maximum likelihood analysis program (AutoClass C). We identified 79 distinct body pattern components, including 8 newly described components, and 7 overall body patterns. Our findings on the body patterning behavior of the dwarf cuttlefish add to a growing database of cephalopod display patterns for future studies and provide insight into the ecological and evolutionary drivers of dynamic camouflage in cephalopods.


Assuntos
Sepia , Animais , Padronização Corporal , Decapodiformes , Ecossistema , Meio Ambiente
4.
Am J Primatol ; 12(1): 55-70, 1987.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31973516

RESUMO

This paper summarizes results of simulation analyses aimed at elucidating the way in which two important dimensions of female rank orders-nepotism and age-gradedness-are affected by variation in individual strategies of rank acquisition on the one hand and by variation in population-wide demographic parameters (eg, birth rate, death rate, etc) on the other. Female rank orders in simulated primate groups were characterized by substantial variability on a year-to-year basis in both nepotism and age-gradedness owing to stochastic processes alone. Although, in general, groups characterized by strongly nepotistic rank orders were not also strongly age-graded, quantitative measures of these two rank-order dimensions for individual groups showed little stability over intervals of time exceeding 3 years. Surprisingly, changes in birth and death rates did not strongly affect either nepotism or age-gradedness so long as average group size remained constant. However, in populations characterized by expanding or declining group size, variation in demographic parameters did indeed exert a powerful influence on the structure of female rank orders. Both nepotism and age-gradedness were also strongly affected, and in quite different ways, by changes in the rules of rank acquisition by females. In sum, the present analyses suggest that differences between species in the degree of nepotism and age-gradedness characteristic of their female rank orders does not in and of itself constitute evidence that the females of those species use markedly different strategies of rank acquisition.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...