Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 50
Filtrar
1.
Heliyon ; 10(5): e27414, 2024 Mar 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38468958

RESUMO

Pareidolia are perceptions of recognizable images or meaningful patterns where none exist. In recent years, this phenomenon has been increasingly studied in healthy subjects and patients with neurological or psychiatric diseases. The current study examined pareidolia production in a group of 53 stroke patients and 82 neurologically healthy controls who performed a natural images task. We found a significant reduction of absolute pareidolia production in left- and right-hemispheric stroke patients, with right-hemispheric patients producing overall fewest pareidolic output. Responses were categorized into 28 distinct categories, with 'Animal', 'Human', 'Face', and 'Body parts' being the most common, accounting for 72% of all pareidolia. Regarding the percentages of the different categories of pareidolia, we found a significant reduction for the percentage of "Body parts" pareidolia in the left-hemispheric patient group as compared to the control group, while the percentage of this pareidolia type was not significantly reduced in right-hemispheric patients compared to healthy controls. These results support the hypothesis that pareidolia production may be influenced by local-global visual processing with the left hemisphere being involved in local and detailed analytical visual processing to a greater extent. As such, a lesion to the right hemisphere, that is believed to be critical for global visual processing, might explain the overall fewest pareidolic output produced by the right-hemispheric patients.

2.
Cortex ; 167: 12-24, 2023 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37515831

RESUMO

Reduplicative paramnesia refers to the delusional belief that there are identical places in different locations. In this case-control study we investigated the clinical, phenomenological, neuropsychological and neuroanatomical data of eleven patients with reduplicative paramnesia and compared them against a control group of eleven patients with severe spatial disorientation without signs of reduplicative paramnesia. We show that most patients with reduplicative paramnesia report that a current place is reduplicated and/or relocated to an other familiar place. Patients with reduplicative paramnesia show a higher prevalence of deficits in the executive functions compared to the control patients, while mnestic and visuo-spatial deficits were both frequent in patients with reduplicative paramnesia and the control group. Patients with reduplicative paramnesia mostly suffer from right hemispheric lesions with a maximal overlap in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Using lesion network mapping we show that lesions causing reduplicative paramnesia are connected to bilateral anterior insula and the right cingulate cortex. We argue that patients with reduplicative paramnesia fail to integrate the actual context with visuo-spatial memories and personal relevant emotional information due to a disruption of the neural network within the anterior temporal lobe, the cingulate cortex and the anterior insula. Also patients with reduplicative paramnesia are not able to resolve this conflict due to the lesion of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and executive dysfunction.


Assuntos
Delusões , Neuroanatomia , Humanos , Neuropsicologia , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico
3.
Front Neurol ; 14: 1183265, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37521297

RESUMO

Odors evoking vivid and intensely felt autobiographical memories are known as the "Proust phenomenon," delineating the particularity of olfaction in being more effective with eliciting emotional memories than other sensory modalities. The phenomenon has been described extensively in healthy participants as well as in patients during pre-epilepsy surgery evaluation after focal stimulation of the amygdalae and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). In this study, we provide the inaugural description of aversive odor-evoked autobiographical memories after stroke in the right hippocampal, parahippocampal, and thalamic nuclei. As potential underlying neural signatures of the phenomenon, we discuss the disinhibition of limbic circuits and impaired communication between the major networks, such as saliency, central executive, and default mode network.

4.
Virtual Real ; 27(1): 307-331, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36915633

RESUMO

Virtual reality (VR) is a promising tool to promote motor (re)learning in healthy users and brain-injured patients. However, in current VR-based motor training, movements of the users performed in a three-dimensional space are usually visualized on computer screens, televisions, or projection systems, which lack depth cues (2D screen), and thus, display information using only monocular depth cues. The reduced depth cues and the visuospatial transformation from the movements performed in a three-dimensional space to their two-dimensional indirect visualization on the 2D screen may add cognitive load, reducing VR usability, especially in users suffering from cognitive impairments. These 2D screens might further reduce the learning outcomes if they limit users' motivation and embodiment, factors previously associated with better motor performance. The goal of this study was to evaluate the potential benefits of more immersive technologies using head-mounted displays (HMDs). As a first step towards potential clinical implementation, we ran an experiment with 20 healthy participants who simultaneously performed a 3D motor reaching and a cognitive counting task using: (1) (immersive) VR (IVR) HMD, (2) augmented reality (AR) HMD, and (3) computer screen (2D screen). In a previous analysis, we reported improved movement quality when movements were visualized with IVR than with a 2D screen. Here, we present results from the analysis of questionnaires to evaluate whether the visualization technology impacted users' cognitive load, motivation, technology usability, and embodiment. Reports on cognitive load did not differ across visualization technologies. However, IVR was more motivating and usable than AR and the 2D screen. Both IVR and AR rea ched higher embodiment level than the 2D screen. Our results support our previous finding that IVR HMDs seem to be more suitable than the common 2D screens employed in VR-based therapy when training 3D movements. For AR, it is still unknown whether the absence of benefit over the 2D screen is due to the visualization technology per se or to technical limitations specific to the device.

5.
Cortex ; 148: 152-167, 2022 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35176552

RESUMO

Spatial neglect after right-hemispheric stroke, characterized by the failure to attend or respond to the contralesional space, is a strong negative outcome predictor. Neglect is a supramodal syndrome affecting not only the visual but also the auditory modality. Preliminary studies used this audio-visual cross-modal effect to show short-lasting effects on attention towards the neglected space. The aim of the present study was to introduce a new technique of auditory stimulation combining the unspecific effect of music (i.e., patients choose their preferred music) with the effects of auditory spatial cueing (i.e., the music is presented dynamically as moving from right to left). The effect of this new auditory stimulation technique was investigated in two proof-of-concept experiments using repeated-measures, cross-over designs including 21 patients with visual neglect after a first right-hemispheric stroke. In Experiment I (n = 9), neglect patients showed a significantly larger improvement in Letter Cancellation after listening to preferred music with than without auditory spatial cueing. After granting the feasibility of this new auditory stimulation technique, we investigated the long-term aftereffects in Experiment II (n = 12). Herefore, we used video-oculography during Free Visual Exploration, a sensitive and reliable tool to assess spatial attention over time. Listening to music with auditory spatialcueing - as compared to music without auditory spatialcueing - significantly improved neglect severity in terms of visual exploration behaviour for up to 3h. A voxel-based-lesion-symptom mapping analysis over all patients revealed that the response variability in listening to music with auditory spatial cueing is determined by the integrity of the right inferior parietal lobule, the second branch of the superior longitudinal fascicle, and parieto-parietal callosal fibres. Our study shows that listening to music with auditory spatial cueing significantly reduces neglect severity and has the potential to be used as an add-on in the neurorehabilitation of neglect.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Percepção , Acidente Vascular Cerebral , Sinais (Psicologia) , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Transtornos da Percepção/reabilitação , Estudo de Prova de Conceito , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/complicações
6.
Neuropsychologia ; 115: 42-50, 2018 07 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29360517

RESUMO

Deficient inhibitory control leading to perseverative behaviour is often observed in neglect patients. Previous studies investigating the relationship between response inhibition and visual attention have reported contradictory results: some studies found a linear relationship between neglect severity and perseverative behaviour whereas others could not replicate this result. The aim of the present study was to shed further light on the interplay between visual attention and response inhibition in neglect, and to investigate the neural underpinnings of this interplay. We propose the use of the Five-Point Test, a test commonly used to asses nonverbal fluency, as a novel approach in the context of neglect. In the Five-Point Test, participants are required to generate as many different designs as possible, by connecting dots within forty rectangles. We hypothesised that, because of its clear definition of perseverative errors, the Five-Point Test would accurately assess both visual attention as well as perseverative behaviour. We assessed 46 neglect patients with right-hemispheric stroke, and performed voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) to identify neural substrates of perseverative behaviour as well as the spatial distribution of perseverations. Our results showed that the Five-Point Test can reliably measure neglect and perseverative behaviour. We did not find any significant relationship between neglect severity and the frequency of perseverations. However, within the subgroup of neglect patients who displayed perseverative behaviour, the spatial distribution of perseverations significantly depended on the integrity of the right putamen. We discuss the putative role of the putamen as a potential subcortical hub to modulate the complex integration between visual attention and response inhibition processes.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Encéfalo/patologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Percepção/patologia , Putamen/patologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Atenção/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Transtornos da Percepção/etiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Putamen/diagnóstico por imagem , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/complicações , Percepção Visual
7.
Cerebrovasc Dis Extra ; 6(1): 27-31, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27194999

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Screening of aphasia in acute stroke is crucial for directing patients to early language therapy. The Language Screening Test (LAST), originally developed in French, is a validated language screening test that allows detection of a language deficit within a few minutes. The aim of the present study was to develop and validate two parallel German versions of the LAST. METHODS: The LAST includes subtests for naming, repetition, automatic speech, and comprehension. For the translation into German, task constructs and psycholinguistic criteria for item selection were identical to the French LAST. A cohort of 101 stroke patients were tested, all of whom were native German speakers. Validation of the LAST was based on (1) analysis of equivalence of the German versions, which was established by administering both versions successively in a subset of patients, (2) internal validity by means of internal consistency analysis, and (3) external validity by comparison with the short version of the Token Test in another subset of patients. RESULTS: The two German versions were equivalent as demonstrated by a high intraclass correlation coefficient of 0.91. Furthermore, an acceptable internal structure of the LAST was found (Cronbach's α = 0.74). A highly significant correlation (r = 0.74, p < 0.0001) between the LAST and the short version of the Token Test indicated good external validity of the scale. CONCLUSION: The German version of the LAST, available in two parallel versions, is a new and valid language screening test in stroke.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Testes de Linguagem , Programas de Rastreamento/métodos , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Afasia/complicações , Afasia/diagnóstico , Feminino , Alemanha , Humanos , Transtornos da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Terapia da Linguagem , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/diagnóstico , Traduções
8.
BMC Geriatr ; 15: 176, 2015 Dec 29.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26714495

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Crossing a street can be a very difficult task for older pedestrians. With increased age and potential cognitive decline, older people take the decision to cross a street primarily based on vehicles' distance, and not on their speed. Furthermore, older pedestrians tend to overestimate their own walking speed, and could not adapt it according to the traffic conditions. Pedestrians' behavior is often tested using virtual reality. Virtual reality presents the advantage of being safe, cost-effective, and allows using standardized test conditions. METHODS: This paper describes an observational study with older and younger adults. Street crossing behavior was investigated in 18 healthy, younger and 18 older subjects by using a virtual reality setting. The aim of the study was to measure behavioral data (such as eye and head movements) and to assess how the two age groups differ in terms of number of safe street crossings, virtual crashes, and missed street crossing opportunities. Street crossing behavior, eye and head movements, in older and younger subjects, were compared with non-parametric tests. RESULTS: The results showed that younger pedestrians behaved in a more secure manner while crossing a street, as compared to older people. The eye and head movements analysis revealed that older people looked more at the ground and less at the other side of the street to cross. CONCLUSIONS: The less secure behavior in street crossing found in older pedestrians could be explained by their reduced cognitive and visual abilities, which, in turn, resulted in difficulties in the decision-making process, especially under time pressure. Decisions to cross a street are based on the distance of the oncoming cars, rather than their speed, for both groups. Older pedestrians look more at their feet, probably because of their need of more time to plan precise stepping movement and, in turn, pay less attention to the traffic. This might help to set up guidelines for improving senior pedestrians' safety, in terms of speed limits, road design, and mixed physical-cognitive trainings.


Assuntos
Acidentes de Trânsito/psicologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Comportamento/fisiologia , Tomada de Decisões , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Movimentos da Cabeça/fisiologia , Pedestres/psicologia , Acidentes de Trânsito/prevenção & controle , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento , Simulação por Computador , Feminino , Voluntários Saudáveis , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Caminhada/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
9.
Neuroscience ; 164(4): 1609-14, 2009 Dec 29.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19782729

RESUMO

After a lesion of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), the perception of a contra-lesional stimulus in presence of a simultaneous, ipsilesional stimulus may be impaired, a phenomenon referred to as visual extinction. In the present study, visual extinction was transiently induced in healthy subjects by interfering with the function of the right PPC by means of continuous theta burst stimulation (TBS). We investigated to which extent the horizontal and vertical position of visual stimuli influenced the extinction rate. A single TBS train over the right PPC induced a significant increase of left visual extinctions of at least 30 min. Left visual extinction rate was higher when the left sided visual stimulus was presented at a more eccentric position on the horizontal axis (irrespective of right sided visual stimulus position) and in the lower part of the visual field. The results are discussed within the framework of current explanatory models and of putative inter- and intrahemispheric mechanisms directing visuospatial attention.


Assuntos
Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Atenção/fisiologia , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Adulto Jovem
10.
Neuroscience ; 151(3): 921-8, 2008 Feb 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18160225

RESUMO

The aim of the current study was to examine the effect of theta burst repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) on the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activation during repeated functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measurements. Theta burst rTMS was applied over the right frontal eye field in seven healthy subjects. Subsequently, repeated fMRI measurements were performed during a saccade-fixation task (block design) 5, 20, 35, and 60 min after stimulation. We found that theta burst rTMS induced a strong and long-lasting decrease of the BOLD signal response of the stimulated frontal eye field at 20 and 35 min. Furthermore, less pronounced alterations of the BOLD signal response with different dynamics were found for remote oculomotor areas such as the left frontal eye field, the pre-supplementary eye field, the supplementary eye field, and both parietal eye fields. Recovery of the BOLD signal changes in the anterior remote areas started earlier than in the posterior remote areas. These results show that a) the major inhibitory impact of theta burst rTMS occurs directly in the stimulated area itself, and that b) a lower effect on remote, oculomotor areas can be induced.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Olho , Lobo Frontal/irrigação sanguínea , Lobo Frontal/efeitos da radiação , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Adulto , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Oxigênio/sangue , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/efeitos da radiação , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/efeitos da radiação , Fatores de Tempo , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana/métodos
11.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 79(4): 474-7, 2008 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17951279

RESUMO

Despite their relevance for locomotion and social interaction in everyday situations, little is known about the cortical control of vertical saccades in humans. Results from microstimulation studies indicate that both frontal eye fields (FEFs) contribute to these eye movements. Here, we present a patient with a damaged right FEF, who hardly made vertical saccades during visual exploration. This finding suggests that, for the cortical control of exploratory vertical saccades, integrity of both FEFs is indeed important.


Assuntos
Encefalopatias/fisiopatologia , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Lobo Frontal/fisiopatologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Encefalopatias/diagnóstico , Eletroculografia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Exame Neurológico , Doenças do Nervo Oculomotor/diagnóstico , Doenças do Nervo Oculomotor/fisiopatologia , Transtornos da Percepção/diagnóstico , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Acompanhamento Ocular Uniforme/fisiologia , Remissão Espontânea , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador , Tomografia Computadorizada por Raios X
12.
Neuroimage ; 31(2): 641-8, 2006 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16497518

RESUMO

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) is a means to study the function and connectivity of brain areas. The present study addressed the question of hemispheric asymmetry of frontal regions and aimed to further understand the acute effects of high- and low-frequency rTMS on regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF). Sixteen healthy right-handed men were imaged using H(2)(15)O positron emission tomography (PET) immediately after stimulation. High (10 Hz)- and low (1 Hz)-frequency suprathreshold short-duration rTMS was applied over either the left or right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). Slow and fast rTMS applied over the left DLPFC significantly increased CBF in the stimulated area. Compared to baseline, slow rTMS induced a significant increase in CBF contralateral to the stimulation site, in the right caudate body and in the anterior cingulum. Furthermore, slow rTMS decreased CBF in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC, ipsilateral to stimulation side). Fast rTMS applied over the right DLPFC was associated with increased activity at the stimulation site, in the bilateral orbitofrontal cortex and in the left medial thalamus compared to 1-Hz rTMS. These results show that rCBF changes induced by prefrontal rTMS differ upon hemisphere stimulated and vary with stimulation frequency. These differential neurophysiological effects of short-train rTMS with respect to side and frequency suggest hemisphere-dependent functional circuits of frontal cortico-subcortical areas.


Assuntos
Circulação Cerebrovascular , Córtex Pré-Frontal/anatomia & histologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/irrigação sanguínea , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana/métodos , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Tomografia por Emissão de Pósitrons , Córtex Pré-Frontal/diagnóstico por imagem , Valores de Referência , Fluxo Sanguíneo Regional
13.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 161(5): 513-7, 2005 May.
Artigo em Francês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16106802

RESUMO

The term visual field corresponds to the angular field of view that is seen by the eyes when they are fixed on a point straight-ahead. In neurological patients--e.g. stroke, trauma, or tumour patients--visual field function can be restricted, depending on lesion site and size. In contrast, the term "functional visual field" describes the area of visual field responsiveness under more ordinary viewing conditions. The visual exploration, i.e. the capacity to explore and analyze our visual world, is dependent on the integrity of the visual system and the oculomotor system which has to move the fovea from one object of interest to the next. In this paper, we present a new method to assess the functional visual field, conceptualized as the area that a patient actively scans with eye movements to detect predefined targets placed on everyday scenes. This method allows us to compare three levels of visual field function: (a) the spatial distribution of successful search (hits, i.e. which targets did the patient find?), (b) the spatial distribution of fixations (i.e. where did the patient preferentially search for targets?), and (c) the retinotopic level (i.e. the visual field assessed by perimetry). By integrating these three levels, one can evaluate functional outcomes of visual field disorders. Of particular importance is the question of how a patient compensates for a visual field loss with appropriate eye movements. A further clinical application of this method is the comparison of pre- with post-treatment data. Patients with visual field disorders usually undergo specific exploration trainings, aimed at enhancing the number and amplitude of saccades towards the region of the visual field deficit. The first experiences and clinical application with this method are presented here.


Assuntos
Doenças do Sistema Nervoso/fisiopatologia , Oftalmologia/instrumentação , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Doenças do Sistema Nervoso/terapia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/complicações , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/terapia
14.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 161(5): 575-7, 2005 May.
Artigo em Francês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16106809

RESUMO

In general, intermittent diplopia evokes suspicion of ocular myasthenia gravis. However, other etiologies such as Brown syndrome or myokymia of the superior oblique may provoke intermittent diplopia. We present a case of intermittent diplopia due to a tumor in the cavernous sinus. A 59-year-old patient reported intermittent diplopia after prolonged downward gaze to the right. All other gaze directions failed to provoke symptoms. In 1992, the diagnosis of inactive macroadenoma of the pituitary gland was established and the patient underwent surgery and radiation therapy. At physical examination, prolonged downward gaze to the right of about 2 minutes provoked paresis of abduction, slight ptosis, and restriction of elevation on the left side, corresponding to sixth nerve palsy and palsy of the superior branch of the third nerve on the left side. MRI showed a relapse of the macroadenoma with infiltration of the cavernous sinus on the left side. The patient underwent surgery then focal radiation (gamma-knife). The clinical course was favourable and at the follow-up examination six months later, no diplopia was reported.


Assuntos
Adenoma/complicações , Neoplasias Encefálicas/complicações , Seio Cavernoso , Diplopia/etiologia , Doenças do Nervo Abducente/etiologia , Adenoma/diagnóstico , Adenoma/cirurgia , Neoplasias Encefálicas/diagnóstico , Neoplasias Encefálicas/cirurgia , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Diplopia/diagnóstico , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Exame Neurológico , Radiocirurgia
15.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 161(5): 602-4, 2005 May.
Artigo em Francês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16106817

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Visual disturbances are common after severe head trauma. Some authors report 50 percent of these patients having damage to the visual system, ocular motor nerve palsies, or central eye movement disturbances. Visual disturbances influence prognosis and outcome of neurological rehabilitation, and, in most cases, diagnosis and treatment require interdisciplinary care. CASE REPORT: We report here a 50-year-old patient who suffered from severe head trauma at age 20. After coming out of a 4-week coma, he suffered visual disturbances such as blurred vision and diplopia. Visual acuity was 1.0 and the visual fields were intact. The symptoms remained unexplained for a long time. One year after the trauma, right trochlear palsy was diagnosed and surgical treatment was performed. However, the symptoms persisted, and one year later, a second operation was performed without changing the symptoms. During many years, prisms or refractive corrections were used to improve the visual disturbances, and finally, a psychiatric treatment was started. At age 40, the patient became presbyopic and his visual problems increased, especially for reading. When he was examined for the first time in our consultation, corrected visual acuity was 1.0 with a small hyperphoria of the right eye. During careful examination of the eye downgaze, a conjugate pure torsional nystagmus was observed, which disappeared in primary gaze position. CONCLUSION: Pure torsional nystagmus is difficult to diagnose, especially when it is provoked only by one gaze direction such as in our patient. As a sequel of the severe head trauma with brainstem contusion, this type of nystagmus is a rare form of central vestibular nystagmus, and may by modified by head rotation or suppressed by convergence. The "treatment" of the patient's blurred vision when reading was easy by ordering goggles with a near part placed near the primary gaze position and a 6 base down prismatic correction. Reading was then possible without provoking torsional nystagmus.


Assuntos
Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/complicações , Transtornos da Visão/etiologia , Coma , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/psicologia , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/cirurgia , Óculos , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Procedimentos Neurocirúrgicos , Nistagmo Patológico/etiologia , Presbiopia/etiologia , Presbiopia/terapia , Doenças do Nervo Troclear/etiologia , Doenças do Nervo Troclear/cirurgia , Transtornos da Visão/cirurgia , Transtornos da Visão/terapia , Acuidade Visual
16.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 1039: 239-51, 2005 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15826978

RESUMO

The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is involved in the preparation of saccadic eye movements. Lesion studies and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies suggest that the human DLPFC is located in area 46 of Brodmann. The DLPFC has direct connections with the main cortical ocular motor areas, that is with the frontal eye field (FEF) and the supplementary eye field (SEF) in the frontal lobe; with several (associative, attentional, and motor) areas in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), including the parietal eye field (PEF); with the cingulate eye field in the anterior cingulate cortex; and directly downstream with the superior colliculus in the brainstem. Lesion and fMRI studies using the antisaccade paradigm have shown that the DLPFC is involved in the inhibition of unwanted reflexive saccades (triggered toward the target by the PEF), whereas the triggering of correct intentional antisaccades (made in the direction opposite to the target) may depend mainly upon the FEF. The DLPFC also controls short-term spatial working memory involved in memory-guided saccades, as shown by lesion and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies. By contrast, medium-term spatial memory (after 25 s) may be controlled by the medial temporal cortex (MTC). Recently, TMS studies have suggested that the transmission of memorized information from the integrative parietal areas (PPC) to the MTC is performed via both an indirect pathway comprising the DLPFC (i.e., transmission in series) and a direct pathway bypassing the DLPFC (i.e., transmission in parallel). Furthermore, the DLPFC is involved in the preparation of predictive saccades (i.e., saccades made before the appearance of an expected target) and saccade sequences, and, therefore, also controls some aspects of temporal working memory. Lastly, the involvement of the DLPFC has recently been reported in tasks comprising a target selection or a directional decision to make for the forthcoming saccade. These different functions suggest that the DLPFC plays a major role in the decisional processes governing ocular motor behavior.


Assuntos
Nervo Oculomotor/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Mapeamento Encefálico , Lateralidade Funcional , Haplorrinos , Humanos , Memória , Estimulação Luminosa , Córtex Pré-Frontal/anatomia & histologia
17.
Exp Brain Res ; 159(2): 263-7, 2004 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15549281

RESUMO

Few studies have addressed the interaction between instruction content and saccadic eye movement control. To assess the impact of instructions on top-down control, we instructed 20 healthy volunteers to deliberately delay saccade triggering, to make inaccurate saccades or to redirect saccades--i.e. to glimpse towards and then immediately opposite to the target. Regular pro- and antisaccade tasks were used for comparison. Bottom-up visual input remained unchanged and was a gap paradigm for all instructions. In the inaccuracy and delay tasks, both latencies and accuracies were detrimentally impaired by either type of instruction and the variability of latency and accuracy was increased. The intersaccadic interval (ISI) required to correct erroneous antisaccades was shorter than the ISI for instructed direction changes in the redirection task. The word-by-word instruction content interferes with top-down saccade control. Top-down control is a time consuming process, which may override bottom-up processing only during a limited time period. It is questionable whether parallel processing is possible in top-down control, since the long ISI for instructed direction changes suggests sequential planning.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Inibição Neural/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia , Volição/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
18.
Brain ; 127(Pt 2): 431-8, 2004 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14691059

RESUMO

Eye movement behaviour during visual exploration of 24 patients with probable Alzheimer's disease and 24 age-matched controls was compared in a clock reading task. Controls were found to focus exploration on distinct areas at the end of each clock hand. The sum of these two areas of highest fixation density was defined as the informative region of interest (ROI). In Alzheimer's disease patients, visual exploration was less focused, with fewer fixations inside the ROI, and the time until the first fixation was inside the ROI was significantly delayed. Changes of fixation distribution correlated significantly with the ability to read the clock correctly, but did not correlate with dementia severity. In Alzheimer's disease patients, fixations were longer and saccade amplitudes were smaller. The altered visual exploration in Alzheimer's disease might be related to parietal dysfunction or to an imbalance between a degraded occipito-parietal and relatively preserved occipito-temporal visual network.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/psicologia , Comportamento Exploratório , Movimentos Oculares , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Doença de Alzheimer/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Fixação Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tempo de Reação , Movimentos Sacádicos
19.
Brain ; 126(Pt 6): 1460-73, 2003 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12764065

RESUMO

Three patients with a unilateral cortical lesion affecting the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), i.e. Brodmann area 46, were tested using different paradigms of reflexive saccades (gap and overlap tasks), intentional saccades (antisaccades, memory-guided and predictive saccades) and smooth pursuit movements. Visually guided saccades with gap and overlap, latency of correct antisaccades and memory-guided saccades and the gain of smooth pursuit were normal, compared with controls. These results confirm our anatomical data showing that the adjacent frontal eye field (FEF) was unimpaired in these patients. The specific pattern of abnormalities after a unilateral DLPFC lesion, compared with that of the FEF lesions previously reported, consists mainly of: (i) a bilateral increase in the percentage of errors in the antisaccade task (misdirected reflexive saccades); (ii) a bilateral increase in the variable error in amplitude, without significant decrease in the gain, in the memory-guided saccade task; and (iii) a bilateral decrease in the percentage of anticipatory saccades in the predictive task. Taken together, these results suggest that the DLPFC plays a crucial role in the decisional processes, preparing saccades by inhibiting unwanted reflexive saccades (inhibition), maintaining memorized information for ongoing intentional saccades (short-term spatial memory) or facilitating anticipatory saccades (prediction), depending upon current external environmental and internal circumstances.


Assuntos
Tomada de Decisões , Transtornos da Motilidade Ocular/etiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiopatologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/complicações , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos da Motilidade Ocular/fisiopatologia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Acompanhamento Ocular Uniforme , Tempo de Reação , Movimentos Sacádicos , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/psicologia
20.
Prog Brain Res ; 142: 3-17, 2003.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12693251

RESUMO

Our knowledge of the cortical control of saccadic eye movements (saccades) in humans has recently progressed mainly thanks to lesion and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies, but also to functional imaging. It is now well-known that the frontal eye field is involved in the triggering of intentional saccades, the parietal eye field in that of reflexive saccades, the supplementary eye field (SEF) in the initiation of motor programs comprising saccades, the pre-SEF in learning of these programs, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in saccade inhibition, prediction and spatial working memory. Saccades may also be used as a convenient model of motricity to study general cognitive processes preparing movements, such as attention, spatial memory and motivation. Visuo-spatial attention appears to be controlled by a bilateral parieto-frontal network comprising different parts of the posterior parietal cortex and the frontal areas involved in saccade control, suggesting that visual attentional shifts and saccades are closely linked. Recently, our understanding of the cortical control of spatial memory has noticeably progressed by using the simple visuo-oculomotor model represented by the memory-guided saccade paradigm, in which a single saccade is made to the remembered position of a unique visual item presented a while before. TMS studies have determined that, after a brief stage of spatial integration in the posterior parietal cortex (inferior to 300 ms), short-term spatial memory (i.e. up to 15-20 s) is controlled by the DLPFC. Behavioral and lesion studies have shown that medium-term spatial memory (between 15-20 s and a few minutes) is specifically controlled by the parahippocampal cortex, before long-term memorization (i.e. after a few minutes) in the hippocampal formation. Lastly, it has been shown that the posterior part of the anterior cingulate cortex, called the cingulate eye field, is involved in motivation and the preparation of all intentional saccades, but not in reflexive saccades. These different but complementary study methods used in humans have thus contributed to a better understanding of both eye movement physiology and general cognitive processes preparing motricity as whole.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Humanos , Memória/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...