Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 47
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Int J Dev Neurosci ; 82(2): 159-167, 2022 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35023591

RESUMO

In this study, we investigated how different environmental cue and the proficiency of body motion influenced detour learning behaviour and cue preference in cue conflict situations. Domestic chicks were trained to detour around an obstacle and follow a fixed route to rejoin with their partners. When the environmental cue was red versus blue vertical stripes, the chicks learned the detour task quicker, and as the number of training trials after route acquisition increased, they switched their preference from the environmental cue to a body-motion cue in the cue conflict test. On the other hand, when the environmental cue was vertical versus horizontal blue stripes, the chicks learned the detour task slower and showed a dependence on the body-motion cue regardless of the number of training trials performed after route acquisition. When the environmental cue was removed, most chicks could still successfully detour according to the specific route on which they had been trained. Furthermore, a significant difference in detour latency was found between chicks using the environmental cue and chicks using the body-motion cue, suggesting separate neuronal circuits responsible for processing the two types of information. Our results demonstrated that young domestic chicks could use both environmental cue and body-motion cues to memorize the route during the detour learning task; however, the detour route preference could be dynamically modulated by difference of the environmental cue and the number of training trials they received.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Aprendizagem Espacial , Animais , Galinhas/fisiologia
2.
Neurosci Bull ; 34(5): 842-848, 2018 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29948842

RESUMO

The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is considered to play a crucial role in many high-level functions, such as cognitive control and emotional regulation. Many studies have reported that the DLPFC can be activated during the processing of emotional information in tasks requiring working memory. However, it is still not clear whether modulating the activity of the DLPFC influences emotional perception in a detection task. In the present study, using transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS), we investigated (1) whether modulating the right DLPFC influences emotional face processing in a detection task, and (2) whether the DLPFC plays equal roles in processing positive and negative emotional faces. The results showed that anodal tDCS over the right DLPFC specifically facilitated the perception of positive faces, but did not influence the processing of negative faces. In addition, anodal tDCS over the right primary visual cortex enhanced performance in the detection task regardless of emotional valence. Our findings suggest, for the first time, that modulating the right DLPFC influences emotional face perception, especially faces showing positive emotion.


Assuntos
Emoções , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Percepção Social , Estimulação Transcraniana por Corrente Contínua , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adulto Jovem
3.
Sheng Li Xue Bao ; 70(1): 79-84, 2018 Feb 25.
Artigo em Chinês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29492518

RESUMO

Superior colliculus-pulvinar-amygdala pathway is one of the subcortical visual pathways in mammalian brain. Some recent studies suggest that this pathway is involved in processing emotion-related visual information. This review discusses the possibility that this pathway is more related to visual alert rather than simply the early visual information processing. The biological significance of this pathway is also discussed. Instead of detecting "where" or "what" the visual target is, the task of this early visual stage is to send out a warning signal, i.e., "something appears", so that the brain can be set up in a state of alert, which is important for the survival of animals. Thus, in the early visual information process, detection of new object "emerging" or "disappearing" takes priority over the acquisition of its feature information of "texture" and "shape", etc. The subcortical pathway may provide the neural basis of early visual warning in topological perception, a biological significance critical for animal survival.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Pulvinar/fisiologia , Colículos Superiores/fisiologia , Vias Visuais , Animais , Emoções , Humanos , Percepção
4.
Neural Regen Res ; 11(9): 1464-1470, 2016 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27857751

RESUMO

Rhesus monkey neural stem cells are capable of differentiating into neurons and glial cells. Therefore, neural stem cell transplantation can be used to promote functional recovery of the nervous system. Rhesus monkey neural stem cells (1 × 105 cells/µL) were injected into bilateral hippocampi of rats with hippocampal lesions. Confocal laser scanning microscopy demonstrated that green fluorescent protein-labeled transplanted cells survived and grew well. Transplanted cells were detected at the lesion site, but also in the nerve fiber-rich region of the cerebral cortex and corpus callosum. Some transplanted cells differentiated into neurons and glial cells clustering along the ventricular wall, and integrated into the recipient brain. Behavioral tests revealed that spatial learning and memory ability improved, indicating that rhesus monkey neural stem cells noticeably improve spatial learning and memory abilities in rats with hippocampal lesions.

6.
Behav Brain Res ; 306: 154-9, 2016 06 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27025444

RESUMO

Chronic stress is an important cause for depression. However, not everyone who is exposed to chronic stress will develop depression. Our previous studies demonstrated that early adversity can cause lasting changes in adolescent rhesus monkeys, but depressive symptoms have not been observed. Compared to adults, it is still unknown that whether adolescent rhesus monkeys experiencing early adversity are more likely to develop depressive symptoms. In this study, we investigated the long term relationship between early adversity, chronic stress and adolescent depression for the first time. Eight male rhesus monkeys were reared in maternal separation (MS) or mother-reared (MR) conditions. All of them went through unpredictable chronic stress for two months at their age four. The stressors included space restriction, intimidation, long illumination and fasting. Behavioral and physiological data were collected during the experiment. The results showed that, compared with the MR group, the locomotor activity of MS group was significantly decreased after one month of chronic stress while huddling up and stereotypical behaviors were significantly increased. Moreover, this trend continued and even worsened at the second month. Significantly higher hair cortisol levels and lower body weight were observed in MS group after two months of stress. These results indicate that early adversity is one of the environmental factors which can increase the susceptibility of depression when experiencing chronic stress in the later life. This will further clarify the important roles of early environmental factors in the development of adolescent depression and children rearing conditions should receive more attention.


Assuntos
Depressão/etiologia , Privação Materna , Estresse Psicológico/complicações , Estresse Psicológico/etiologia , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Peso Corporal , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Cabelo/química , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Locomoção/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Distribuição Aleatória , Disfunções Sexuais Psicogênicas/etiologia , Comportamento Estereotipado/fisiologia
7.
Neurosci Bull ; 31(2): 235-41, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25822218

RESUMO

The prefrontal cortex is implicated in cognitive functioning and schizophrenia. Prefrontal dysfunction is closely associated with the symptoms of schizophrenia. In addition to the features typical of schizophrenia, patients also present with aspects of cognitive disorders. Based on these relationships, a monkey model mimicking the cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia has been made using treatment with the non-specific competitive N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist, phencyclidine. The symptoms are ameliorated by atypical antipsychotic drugs such as clozapine. The beneficial effects of clozapine on behavioral impairment might be a specific indicator of schizophrenia-related cognitive impairment.


Assuntos
Modelos Animais de Doenças , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiopatologia , Esquizofrenia/patologia , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatologia , Animais , Antipsicóticos/uso terapêutico , Transtornos Cognitivos , Antagonistas de Aminoácidos Excitatórios/toxicidade , Haplorrinos , Fenciclidina/toxicidade , Córtex Pré-Frontal/patologia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Esquizofrenia/induzido quimicamente , Esquizofrenia/tratamento farmacológico
10.
Dongwuxue Yanjiu ; 35(3): 174-81, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24866487

RESUMO

Postpartum depression (PPD) is a modified form of major depressive disorders (MDD) that can exert profound negative effects on both mothers and infants than MDD. Within the postpartum period, both mothers and infants are susceptible; but because PPD typically occurs for short durations and has moderate symptoms, there exists challenges in exploring and addressing the underlying cause of the depression. This fact highlights the need for relevant animal models. In the present study, postpartum adult female cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) living in breeding groups were observed for typical depressive behavior. The huddle posture behavior was utilized as an indicator of behavioral depression postpartum (BDP) as it has been established as the core depressive-like behavior in primates. Monkeys were divided into two groups: A BDP group (n=6), which were found to spend more time huddling over the first two weeks postpartum than other individuals that formed a non-depression control group (n=4). The two groups were then further analyzed for locomotive activity, stressful events, hair cortisol levels and for maternal interactive behaviors. No differences were found between the BDP and control groups in locomotive activity, in the frequencies of stressful events experienced and in hair cortisol levels. These findings suggested that the postpartum depression witnessed in the monkeys was not related to external factors other than puerperium period. Interestingly, the BDP monkeys displayed an abnormal maternal relationship consisting of increased infant grooming. Taken together, these findings suggest that the adult female cynomolgus monkeys provide a natural model of behavioral postpartum depression that holds a number of advantages over commonly used rodent systems in PPD modeling. The cynomolgus monkeys have a highly-organized social hierarchy and reproductive characteristics without seasonal restriction-similar to humans-as well as much greater homology to humans than rodents. As such, this model may provide a greater translational efficiency and research platform for systematically investigating the etiology, treatment, prevention of PPD.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Depressão Pós-Parto , Macaca fascicularis/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Cabelo/química , Hidrocortisona/química , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Atividade Motora , Gravidez , Estresse Fisiológico
11.
PLoS One ; 9(3): e93112, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24671174

RESUMO

Face perception is integral to human perception system as it underlies social interactions. Saccadic eye movements are frequently made to bring interesting visual information, such as faces, onto the fovea for detailed processing. Just before eye movement onset, the processing of some basic features, such as the orientation, of an object improves at the saccade landing point. Interestingly, there is also evidence that indicates faces are processed in early visual processing stages similar to basic features. However, it is not known whether this early enhancement of processing includes face recognition. In this study, three experiments were performed to map the timing of face presentation to the beginning of the eye movement in order to evaluate pre-saccadic face recognition. Faces were found to be similarly processed as simple objects immediately prior to saccadic movements. Starting ∼ 120 ms before a saccade to a target face, independent of whether or not the face was surrounded by other faces, the face recognition gradually improved and the critical spacing of the crowding decreased as saccade onset was approaching. These results suggest that an upcoming saccade prepares the visual system for new information about faces at the saccade landing site and may reduce the background in a crowd to target the intended face. This indicates an important role of pre-saccadic eye movement signals in human face recognition.


Assuntos
Face/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Movimentos Sacádicos , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adulto Jovem
12.
Neurosci Bull ; 29(5): 553-64, 2013 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23852560

RESUMO

Studies estimating eye movements have demonstrated that non-human primates have fixation patterns similar to humans at the first sight of a picture. In the current study, three sets of pictures containing monkeys, humans or both were presented to rhesus monkeys and humans. The eye movements on these pictures by the two species were recorded using a Tobii eye-tracking system. We found that monkeys paid more attention to the head and body in pictures containing monkeys, whereas both monkeys and humans paid more attention to the head in pictures containing humans. The humans always concentrated on the eyes and head in all the pictures, indicating the social role of facial cues in society. Although humans paid more attention to the hands than monkeys, both monkeys and humans were interested in the hands and what was being done with them in the pictures. This may suggest the importance and necessity of hands for survival. Finally, monkeys scored lower in eye-tracking when fixating on the pictures, as if they were less interested in looking at the screen than humans. The locations of fixation in monkeys may provide insight into the role of eye movements in an evolutionary context.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Adulto , Animais , Evolução Biológica , Feminino , Humanos , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
13.
Dongwuxue Yanjiu ; 34(E2): E50-61, 2013 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23572367

RESUMO

To proceed from sensation to movement, integration and transformation of information from different senses and reference frames are required. Several brain areas are involved in this transformation process, but previous neuroanatomical and neurophysiological studies have implicated the caudal area 7b as one particular component of this transformation system. In this study, we present the first quantitative report on the spatial coding properties of caudal area 7b. The results showed that neurons in this area had intermediate component characteristics in the transformation system; the area contained bimodal neurons, and neurons in this area encode spatial information using a hybrid reference frame. These results provide evidence that caudal area 7b may belong to the reference frame transformation system, thus contributing to our general understanding of the transformation system.


Assuntos
Macaca fascicularis/fisiologia , Macaca fascicularis/psicologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial , Animais , Mapeamento Encefálico , Masculino
14.
Dongwuxue Yanjiu ; 34(E2): E42-9, 2013 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23572366

RESUMO

In animal societies, some stressful events can lead to higher levels of physiological stress. Such stressors, like social rank, also predict an increased vulnerability to an array of diseases. However, the physiological relationship between social rank and stress varies between different species, as well as within groups of a single species. For example, dominant individuals are more socially stressed at times, while at other times it is the subordinate ones who experience this stress. Together, these variations make it difficult to assess disease vulnerability as connected to social interactions. In order to learn more about how physiological rank relationships vary between groups of a single species, cortisol measurements from hair samples were used to evaluate the effects of dominance rank on long-term stress levels in despotic and less stringent female rhesus macaque hierarchal groups. In despotic groups, cortisol levels were found not to be correlated with social rank, but a negative correlation was found between social rank and cortisol levels in less stringent hierarchies. Low ranking monkeys in less stringent groups secreted elevated levels of cortisol compared to higher ranking animals. These data suggest that variations in the strictness of the dominance hierarchy are determining factors in rank related stress physiology. The further consideration of nonhuman primate social system diversity and the linear degree of their hierarchies may allow for the development of valid rank-related stress models that will help increase our understanding and guide the development of new therapeutics for diseases related to human socioeconomic status.


Assuntos
Hidrocortisona/sangue , Macaca mulatta/psicologia , Predomínio Social , Animais , Feminino , Macaca mulatta/sangue , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Masculino , Estresse Psicológico
15.
Dongwuxue Yanjiu ; 34(E1): E26-34, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23389984

RESUMO

Association between the reward caused by consuming drugs and the context in which they are consumed is essential in the formation of morphine-induced conditioned place preference (CPP). Glucocorticoid receptor (GRs) activation in different regions of the brain affects reward-based reinforcement and memory processing. A wide array of studies have demonstrated that blockage of GRs in some brain areas can have an effect on reward-related memory; however, to date there have been no systematic studies about the involvement of glucocorticoids (GCs) in morphine-related reward memory. Here, we used the GR antagonist RU38486 to investigate how GRs blockage affects the sensitization and CPP behavior during different phases of reward memory included acquisition, retrieval and reconsolidation. Interestingly, our results showed RU38486 has the ability to impair the acquisition, retrieval and reconsolidation of reward-based memory in CPP and sensitization behavior. But RU38486 by itself cannot induce CPP or conditioned place aversion (CPA) behavior. Our data provide a much more complete picture of the potential effects that glucocorticoids have on the reward memory of different phases and inhibit the sensitization behavior.


Assuntos
Morfina , Receptores de Glucocorticoides , Animais , Memória/efeitos dos fármacos , Camundongos , Mifepristona , Morfina/farmacologia , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Recompensa
16.
Nat Commun ; 4: 1426, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23385571

RESUMO

Chinese tree shrews (Tupaia belangeri chinensis) possess many features valuable in animals used as experimental models in biomedical research. Currently, there are numerous attempts to employ tree shrews as models for a variety of human disorders: depression, myopia, hepatitis B and C virus infections, and hepatocellular carcinoma, to name a few. Here we present a publicly available annotated genome sequence for the Chinese tree shrew. Phylogenomic analysis of the tree shrew and other mammalians highly support its close affinity to primates. By characterizing key factors and signalling pathways in nervous and immune systems, we demonstrate that tree shrews possess both shared common and unique features, and provide a genetic basis for the use of this animal as a potential model for biomedical research.


Assuntos
Genoma/genética , Tupaia/genética , Animais , China , Variação Genética , Hepacivirus/fisiologia , Hepatite C/genética , Hepatite C/virologia , Humanos , Sistema Imunitário/metabolismo , Inativação Metabólica/genética , Camundongos , Sistema Nervoso/metabolismo , Filogenia , Análise de Sequência de DNA , Tupaia/imunologia
17.
Neurosci Bull ; 29(1): 37-46, 2013 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23361521

RESUMO

Opiates and dopamine (DA) play key roles in learning and memory in humans and animals. Although interactions between these neurotransmitters have been found, their functional roles remain to be fully elucidated, and their dysfunction may contribute to human diseases and addiction. Here we investigated the interactions of morphine and dopaminergic neurotransmitter systems with respect to learning and memory in rhesus monkeys by using the Wisconsin General Test Apparatus (WGTA) delayed-response task. Morphine and DA agonists (SKF-38393, apomorphine and bromocriptine) or DA antagonists (SKF-83566, haloperidol and sulpiride) were co-administered to the monkeys 30 min prior to the task. We found that dose-patterned co-administration of morphine with D1 or D2 antagonists or agonists reversed the impaired spatial working memory induced by morphine or the compounds alone. For example, morphine at 0.01 mg/kg impaired spatial working memory, while morphine (0.01 mg/kg) and apomorphine (0.01 or 0.06 mg/kg) co-treatment ameliorated this effect. Our findings suggest that the interactions between morphine and dopaminergic compounds influence spatial working memory in rhesus monkeys. A better understanding of these interactive relationships may provide insights into human addiction.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação/efeitos dos fármacos , Dopaminérgicos/administração & dosagem , Memória de Curto Prazo/efeitos dos fármacos , Morfina/administração & dosagem , Receptores Dopaminérgicos/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/efeitos dos fármacos , Animais , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Interações Medicamentosas , Comportamento de Procura de Droga/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Distribuição Aleatória , Tempo de Reação , Receptores Dopaminérgicos/efeitos dos fármacos , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia
19.
Dongwuxue Yanjiu ; 33(1): 14-8, 2012 Feb.
Artigo em Chinês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22345003

RESUMO

The clinical use of morphine to reduce pain is limited because of its drug tolerance, dependence and addiction. In the present study, the tree shrews (Tupaia belangeri chinensis) developed morphine tolerance and chronic morphine dependence by morphine injections with increasing doses (5, 10, 15, 20 mg/kg body weight for 7 days). Meanwhile, the naloxone (1.25 mg/kg body weight)-induced conditioned place aversion (CPA) and the withdrawal symptom were also found. The tree shrew model of chronic morphine dependence can be used to investigate the withdrawal symptoms and to select potential withdrawal symptoms reducing drugs in the future.


Assuntos
Modelos Animais de Doenças , Dependência de Morfina/fisiopatologia , Morfina/efeitos adversos , Dor/tratamento farmacológico , Síndrome de Abstinência a Substâncias/fisiopatologia , Tupaia , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Humanos , Masculino , Morfina/administração & dosagem , Dependência de Morfina/etiologia , Dependência de Morfina/psicologia , Síndrome de Abstinência a Substâncias/psicologia
20.
Dongwuxue Yanjiu ; 33(1): 89-91, 2012 Feb.
Artigo em Chinês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22345015

RESUMO

We showed that rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) can develop a morphine dependence. Rhesus monkeys successfully established a conditioned place preference (CPP) induced by morphine treatment and this preference lasted for at least (36.3 ± 1.3) months. This animal model may be useful for research into addiction in humans.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Operante/efeitos dos fármacos , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Macaca mulatta , Dependência de Morfina/psicologia , Morfina/efeitos adversos , Animais , Humanos , Dependência de Morfina/etiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...