Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 17 de 17
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Am Coll Health ; : 1-4, 2022 Aug 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35997699

RESUMO

Objective: To examine social connection as a protective factor against exam stress. Participants: 55 undergraduate students at two universities. Methods: Students were evaluated on an exam day for their hardest class and at baseline, a day in a week where they had no exams. Social connection, salivary cortisol, perceived stress, and cognitive control (measured with the Stroop test) were assessed. Exam scores were later reported. Results: Higher social connection was associated with lower perceived stress on exam day. At a small liberal arts school, higher levels of social connection were associated with higher Stroop scores. This correlation with cognitive control was not significant at a large public university. Conclusions: These findings indicate that social connection may be a protective factor in mitigating perceived stress and cognitive control capabilities may help facilitate reduced exam stress in some school environments.

2.
Front Physiol ; 13: 897284, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35770191

RESUMO

Heart Rate Variability (HRV) can be a useful metric to capture meaningful information about heart function. One of the non-linear indices used to analyze HRV, Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (DFA), finds short and long-term correlations in RR intervals to capture quantitative information about variability. This study focuses on the impact of visual and mental stimulation on HRV as expressed via DFA within healthy adults. Visual stimulation can activate the automatic nervous system to directly impact physiological behavior such as heart rate. In this investigation of HRV, 70 participants (21 males) viewed images on a screen followed by a math and recall task. Each viewing segment lasted 2 min and 18 s. The math and memory recall task segment lasted 4 min total. This process was repeated 9 times during which the participants' electrocardiogram was recorded. 37 participants (12 males) opted in for an additional 24-h Holter recording after the viewing and task segments of the study were complete. Participants were randomly assigned to either a pure (organized image presentation) or mixed (random image presentation) image regime for the viewing portion of the study to investigate the impact of the external environment on HRV. DFA α1 was extracted from the RR intervals. Our findings suggest that DFA α1 can differentiate between the viewing [DFA α1 range from 0.96 (SD = 0.25) to 1.08 (SD = 0.22)] and the task segments [DFA α1 range from 1.17 (SD = 0.21) to 1.26 (SD = 0.25)], p < 0.0006 for all comparisons. However, DFA α1 was not able to distinguish between the two image regimes. During the 24-hour follow up, participants had an average DFA α1 = 1.09 (SD = 0.14). In conclusion, our findings suggest a graded response in DFA during short term stimulation and a responsiveness in participants to adjust physiologically to their external environment expressed through the DFA exponent.

3.
Cogn Emot ; 34(5): 960-969, 2020 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32065082

RESUMO

In recognition memory paradigms, emotional details are often recognised better than neutral ones, but at the cost of memory for peripheral details. We previously provided evidence that, when peripheral details must be recalled using central details as cues, peripheral details from emotional scenes are at least as likely to be recalled as those from neutral scenes. Here we replicated and explicated this result by implementing a mathematical modelling approach to disambiguate the influence of target type, scene emotionality, scene valence, and their interactions. After incidentally encoding scenes that included neutral backgrounds with a positive, negative, or neutral foreground objects, participants showed equal or better cued recall of components from emotional scenes compared to neutral scenes. There was no evidence of emotion-based impairment in cued recall in either of two experiments, including one in which we replicated the emotion-induced memory trade-off in recognition. Mathematical model fits indicated that the emotionality of the encoded scene was the primary driver of improved cued-recall performance. Thus, even when emotion impairs recognition of peripheral components of scenes, it can preserve the ability to recall which scene components were studied together.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Modelos Psicológicos , Modelos Teóricos , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Emoções , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória
4.
Affect Sci ; 1(3): 172-185, 2020 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36043208

RESUMO

Enhanced emotional memory (EEM) describes memory benefits for emotional items, traditionally attributed to impacts of arousal at encoding; however, attention, semantic relatedness, and distinctiveness likely also contribute in various ways. The current study manipulated arousal, semantic relatedness, and distinctiveness while recording changes in event-related potentials and heart rate during memory encoding. Trials were classified as remembered or forgotten by immediate recall performance. Negative images were remembered significantly better than neutral, and related neutral images were remembered significantly better than unrelated neutral images. Higher P300 and late positive potential (LPP) amplitudes were associated with memory for negative images as compared with related neutral images, suggesting that negative images received additional attentional processing at encoding, and that this cannot be accounted for only by the inherent relatedness of negative stimuli. No encoding benefits were found for related neutral images though they were better remembered than unrelated neutral images, indicating retrieval dynamics impacted memory. When image types were intermixed, greater heart rate changes occurred, and negative and unrelated neutral images received increased elaborative processing as compared with related neutral images, perhaps due to the prioritization of encoding resources. These results suggest encoding and retrieval processes contribute to EEM, with emotional items benefiting additively.

6.
Memory ; 26(6): 751-758, 2018 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29173027

RESUMO

After viewing a scene, individuals differ in what they prioritise and remember. Culture may be one factor that influences scene memory, as Westerners have been shown to be more item-focused than Easterners (see Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922-934). However, cultures may differ in their sensitivity to scene incongruences and emotion processing, which may account for cross-cultural differences in scene memory. The current study uses hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) to examine scene memory while controlling for scene congruency and the perceived emotional intensity of the images. American and East Asian participants encoded pictures that included a positive, negative, or neutral item placed on a neutral background. After a 20-min delay, participants were shown the item and background separately along with similar and new items and backgrounds to assess memory specificity. Results indicated that even when congruency and emotional intensity were controlled, there was evidence that Americans had better item memory than East Asians. Incongruent scenes were better remembered than congruent scenes. However, this effect did not differ by culture. This suggests that Americans' item focus may result in memory changes that are robust despite variations in scene congruency and perceived emotion.


Assuntos
Comparação Transcultural , Emoções/fisiologia , Memória Episódica , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
7.
Cogn Emot ; 31(2): 325-338, 2017 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26577049

RESUMO

Most studies examine the effects of stress on memory for visual information test memory for entire scenes. However, arousal levels may differentially influence memory for backgrounds as opposed to items. Participants encoded scenes that included a negative-high-arousal, negative-moderate-arousal, or neutral item on a neutral background. After a 30-minute (Experiment 1) or 48-hour delay (Experiment 2), participants underwent a stressor or control task while heart rate was recorded. A recognition memory task was then given with items and backgrounds presented separately. High-arousal images had a greater detriment in background memory than moderate-arousal images. Further, though there was evidence that change in cortisol level at retrieval was associated with impaired memory for items, it was not associated with detriments in background memory. Increased heart rate was associated with impaired memory for both items and backgrounds. This suggests that the level of sympathetic and cortisol reactivity differentially affects memory for items and backgrounds.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Memória , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adolescente , Feminino , Frequência Cardíaca/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Saliva/metabolismo , Estresse Fisiológico/fisiologia , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
8.
Cogn Emot ; 30(7): 1352-60, 2016 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26220708

RESUMO

Enhanced emotional memory often comes at the cost of memory for surrounding background information. Narrowed-encoding theories suggest that this is due to narrowed attention for emotional information at encoding, leading to impaired encoding of background information. Recent work has suggested that an encoding-based theory may be insufficient. Here, we examined whether cued recall-instead of previously used recognition memory tasks-would reveal evidence that non-emotional information associated with emotional information was effectively encoded. Participants encoded positive, negative, or neutral objects on neutral backgrounds. At retrieval, they were given either the item or the background as a memory cue and were asked to recall the associated scene element. Counter to narrowed-encoding theories, emotional items were more likely than neutral items to trigger recall of the associated background. This finding suggests that there is a memory trace of this contextual information and that emotional cues may facilitate retrieval of this information.


Assuntos
Emoções , Memória , Rememoração Mental , Adolescente , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
9.
J Undergrad Neurosci Educ ; 13(2): A101-9, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25838801

RESUMO

One challenge that many neuroscience instructors face is how to teach students to communicate within the field. The goal of this project was to improve students' scientific writing in an introductory psychology laboratory course that serves as a feeder course into the neuroscience curriculum. This course included a scaffolded approach - breaking assignments into different sections that build upon each other to allow for more direction and feedback on each section. Students were also provided with examples of scientific writing, given direction on finding and reading journal articles, and were taught how to effectively peer review a paper. Research papers were assessed before (Year 1) and after (Year 2) this scaffolded approach was instituted. The assessment included measures of "Genre Knowledge" for each section of a research paper (abstract, introduction, method, results, discussion) as well as measures of "Writing Elements" (grammar, formatting, clarity, transitions, building to the hypothesis, using evidence). The results indicated that there was an improvement for Genre Knowledge scores when comparing Year 1 to Year 2. However, there was no systematic improvement in Writing Elements. This suggests that this teaching technique was most effective in improving students' ability to write within the scientific genre. The logistics of implementing such an approach are discussed.

10.
Cereb Cortex ; 25(3): 646-57, 2015 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24072888

RESUMO

Separate lines of research have demonstrated that rises in cortisol can benefit memory consolidation, as can the occurrence of sleep soon after encoding. For the first time, we demonstrate that pre-learning cortisol interacts with sleep to benefit memory consolidation, particularly for negative arousing items. Resting cortisol levels during encoding were positively correlated with subsequent memory, but only following a period of sleep. There was no such relation following a period of wakefulness. Using eye tracking, we further reveal that for negative stimuli, this facilitative effect may arise because cortisol strengthens the relationship between looking time at encoding and subsequent memory. We suggest that elevated cortisol may "tag" attended information as important to remember at the time of encoding, thus enabling sleep-based processes to optimally consolidate salient information in a selective manner. Neuroimaging data suggest that this optimized consolidation leads to a refinement of the neural processes recruited for successful retrieval of negative stimuli, with the retrieval of items attended in the presence of elevated cortisol and consolidated over a night of sleep associated with activity in the amygdala and vmPFC.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Hidrocortisona/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Sono/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/análise , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Vigília/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
11.
J Vis Exp ; (88)2014 Jun 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24962611

RESUMO

Although rises in cortisol can benefit memory consolidation, as can sleep soon after encoding, there is currently a paucity of literature as to how these two factors may interact to influence consolidation. Here we present a protocol to examine the interactive influence of cortisol and sleep on memory consolidation, by combining three methods: eye tracking, salivary cortisol analysis, and behavioral memory testing across sleep and wake delays. To assess resting cortisol levels, participants gave a saliva sample before viewing negative and neutral objects within scenes. To measure overt attention, participants' eye gaze was tracked during encoding. To manipulate whether sleep occurred during the consolidation window, participants either encoded scenes in the evening, slept overnight, and took a recognition test the next morning, or encoded scenes in the morning and remained awake during a comparably long retention interval. Additional control groups were tested after a 20 min delay in the morning or evening, to control for time-of-day effects. Together, results showed that there is a direct relation between resting cortisol at encoding and subsequent memory, only following a period of sleep. Through eye tracking, it was further determined that for negative stimuli, this beneficial effect of cortisol on subsequent memory may be due to cortisol strengthening the relation between where participants look during encoding and what they are later able to remember. Overall, results obtained by a combination of these methods uncovered an interactive effect of sleep and cortisol on memory consolidation.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Memória/fisiologia , Sono/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/análise , Masculino , Salvia/química , Salvia/metabolismo , Vigília/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
12.
Cogn Emot ; 28(5): 881-92, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24295041

RESUMO

Individuals are more likely to remember emotional than neutral information, but this benefit does not always extend to the surrounding background information. This memory narrowing is theorised to be linked to the availability of attentional resources at encoding. In contrast to the predictions of this theoretical account, altering participants' attentional resources at encoding by dividing attention did not affect emotion-induced memory narrowing. Attention was divided using three separate manipulations: a digit ordering task (Experiment 1), an arithmetic task (Experiment 2) and an auditory discrimination task (Experiment 3). Across all three experiments, divided attention decreased memory across the board but did not affect the degree of memory narrowing. These findings suggest that theories to explain memory narrowing must be expanded to include other potential mechanisms beyond the limitations of attentional resources.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Estudantes/psicologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adulto Jovem
13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22715324

RESUMO

Many past examinations of memory changes in individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) have focused on changes in memory for trauma. However, it is unclear if these mnemonic differences extend beyond the memory of the trauma to memory for other positive and negative information and if they are specific to individuals with PTSD or extend to other individuals who have experienced trauma. The present study examined the influences of trauma exposure and PTSD on an effect that may parallel tunnel memory in PTSD: the emotion-induced memory trade-off, whereby emotional aspects of an experience are remembered at the expense of the nonemotional context. Three groups of participants (25 with current PTSD, 27 who had experienced trauma but did not have current PTSD, and 25 controls who had neither experienced significant trauma nor met criteria for current PTSD) were shown complex visual scenes that included an item (positive, negative, or neutral) placed on a neutral background. Forty-five minutes later, participants underwent a recognition memory test for the items and backgrounds separately. An emotion-induced memory trade-off was said to occur when there was a significant difference in item and background memory for emotional scenes, but not for neutral scenes. Results indicated that people with PTSD, like the other groups, were more likely to remember positive and negative items than neutral items. Moreover, people with PTSD exhibited a memory trade-off comparable in magnitude to that exhibited by the non-trauma control group. In contrast, trauma-exposed people without a current diagnosis of PTSD did not show a trade-off, because they remembered items within scenes better than their accompanying contexts not only for emotional but also for neutral scenes. These results suggest that (1) the effect of emotion on memory for visual scenes is similar in people with PTSD and control participants, and (2) people who have experienced trauma, but do not have PTSD, may have a different way of attending to and remembering visual scenes, exhibiting less of a memory trade-off than either control participants or people with PTSD.

14.
Cogn Neurosci ; 3(3-4): 150-9, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24171733

RESUMO

Memory for emotional experiences often persists longer than memory for neutral experiences. The present study examined how encoding processes influence memory retention across 0.5- or 24-h delays and whether these processes differ between emotionally arousing and neutral information. Participants encoded items during an fMRI scan. Immediately following the scan, and again 24-h later, participants performed a recognition memory test. The results revealed that, for emotionally arousing information, most regions showed a correspondence to subsequent-memory performance that was at least as strong after the long delay as it was after the short delay. For neutral items, by contrast, many more regions, including portions of the hippocampus and lateral prefrontal cortex, showed a stronger correspondence to subsequent-memory performance after the short delay than the long delay. These results suggest that the processes engaged at the moment of encoding have a longer-lasting relation to subsequent memory for emotionally arousing information than for neutral information.

15.
Neuroimage ; 53(1): 318-24, 2010 Oct 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20542121

RESUMO

Some suggest that arousal is the essential element needed to engage the amygdala. However, the role of arousal in the larger emotional memory network may differ depending on the valence (positive, negative) of the to-be-remembered information. The goal of the current study was to determine the influence of arousal-based changes in amygdalar connectivity for positive and negative items. Participants were shown emotional and neutral pictures while they underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scan. The emotional pictures varied by valence (positive or negative) and arousal (high or low). Approximately 90minutes later, outside of the scanner, participants took a surprise recognition test. Effective connectivity analysis examined how arousal affected successful encoding activity. For negative information, arousal increased the strength of amygdala connections to the inferior frontal gyrus and the middle occipital gyrus, while for positive information arousal decreased the strength of these amygdala efferents. Further, while the effect of arousal on memory for positive information was restricted to amygdalar efferents, arousal had a more widespread effect for negative items, enhancing connectivity between other nodes of the emotional memory network. These findings emphasize that the effect of arousal on the connectivity within the emotional memory network depends on item valence.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Memória/fisiologia , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Adulto , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
16.
J Undergrad Neurosci Educ ; 9(1): A36-42, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23495003

RESUMO

Preparing students for a career in cognitive neuroscience may be especially challenging due to the expense and complexity of many types of cognitive neuroscience technologies. However, it is possible to train students in cognitive neuroscience at a primarily undergraduate university (PUI) in both the classroom and the laboratory. First, we propose specific methods that can be used in the classroom to make cognitive neuroscience material accessible. We also suggest ways to introduce cognitive neuroscience methodology through lab-based courses or activities. Second, we offer suggestions on how to conduct more complex functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalogram (EEG) research with undergraduates at a small school. We hope that these suggestions will be a helpful guide for those wishing to prepare their students for further studies and careers in this exciting and challenging field.

17.
Psychophysiology ; 46(6): 1190-9, 2009 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19674398

RESUMO

This study examined how valence and arousal affect the processes linked to subsequent memory for emotional information. While undergoing an fMRI scan, participants viewed neutral pictures and emotional pictures varying by valence and arousal. After the scan, participants performed a recognition test. Subsequent memory for negative or high arousal information was associated with occipital and temporal activity, whereas memory for positive or low arousal information was associated with frontal activity. Regression analyses confirmed that for negative or high arousal items, temporal lobe activity was the strongest predictor of later memory whereas for positive or low arousal items, frontal activity corresponded most strongly with later memory. These results suggest that the types of encoding processes relating to memory (e.g., sensory vs. elaborative processing) can differ based on the affective qualities of emotional information.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Análise de Regressão , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...