Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 20
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Memory ; 32(3): 296-307, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38444169

RESUMO

In a large-scale study, we asked people for their memories of The Beatles. Over four thousand respondents completed an online questionnaire. The memory could be related to a song, album, event, TV, film, or even a personal encounter. Respondents judged the age at which the event remembered had occurred and rated the memory for vividness, emotional intensity, valence and rehearsal. We found 38% of the memories were classified as "seeing The Beatles live", 25% "buying Beatles music", 20% "love of The Beatles" and 17% of the memories were "listening to Beatles songs with other people" - what we refer to as cascading memories. Among the younger respondents (aged 26 and under), 84% of the memories were cascading in nature. The memories dated to what we term the "self-defining period" in autobiographical memory (previously termed "the reminiscence bump"), with a mean age-at-encoding of 13.6 years, which is consistent with other studies of memories associated with music. We propose that these memories reflect the formation of generational identity [Mannheim, K. (1952). The problem of generations. In K. Mannheim (Ed.), Essays on the sociology knowledge (pp. 276-321). Routledge & Keegan Paul].


Assuntos
Memória Episódica , Música , Humanos , Adolescente , Rememoração Mental , Música/psicologia , Emoções , Nitrilas
3.
Psychol Sci ; 29(10): 1612-1619, 2018 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30016599

RESUMO

In a large-scale survey, 6,641 respondents provided descriptions of their first memory and their age when they encoded that memory, and they completed various memory judgments and ratings. In good agreement with many other studies, where mean age at encoding of earliest memories is usually found to fall somewhere in the first half of the 3rd year of life, the mean age at encoding here was 3.2 years. The established view is that the distribution around mean age at encoding is truncated, with very few or no memories dating to the preverbal period, that is, below about 2 years of age. However, we found that 2,487 first memories (nearly 40% of the entire sample) dated to an age at encoding of 2 years and younger, with 893 dating to 1 year and younger. We discuss how such improbable, fictional first memories could have arisen and contrast them with more probable first memories, those with an age at encoding of 3 years and older.


Assuntos
Memória Episódica , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores Sexuais , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
4.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 71(2): 449-454, 2018 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27788629

RESUMO

Participants generated both autobiographical memories (AMs) that they believed to be true and intentionally fabricated autobiographical memories (IFAMs). Memories were constructed while a concurrent memory load (random 8-digit sequence) was held in mind or while there was no concurrent load. Amount and accuracy of recall of the concurrent memory load was reliably poorer following generation of IFAMs than following generation of AMs. There was no reliable effect of load on memory generation times; however, IFAMs always took longer to construct than AMs. Finally, replicating previous findings, fewer IFAMs had a field perspective than AMs, IFAMs were less vivid than AMs, and IFAMs contained more motion words (indicative of increased cognitive load). Taken together, these findings show a pattern of systematic differences that mark out IFAMs, and they also show that IFAMs can be identified indirectly by lowered performance on concurrent tasks that increase cognitive load.


Assuntos
Imaginação/fisiologia , Intenção , Memória Episódica , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Repressão Psicológica , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Linguística , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
5.
Br J Clin Psychol ; 55(2): 206-24, 2016 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26296194

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: To examine the impact of memory accessibility on episodic future thinking. DESIGN: Single-case study of neurological patient HCM and an age-matched comparison group of neurologically Healthy Controls. METHODS: We administered a full battery of tests assessing general intelligence, memory, and executive functioning. To assess autobiographical memory, the Autobiographical Memory Interview (Kopelman, Wilson, & Baddeley, 1990. The Autobiographical Memory Interview. Bury St. Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company) was administered. The Past Episodic and Future Episodic sections of Dalla Barba's Confabulation Battery (Dalla Barba, 1993, Cogn. Neuropsychol., 1, 1) and a specifically tailored Mental Time Travel Questionnaire were administered to assess future thinking in HCM and age-matched controls. RESULTS: HCM presented with a deficit in forming new memories (anterograde amnesia) and recalling events from before the onset of neurological impairment (retrograde amnesia). HCM's autobiographical memory impairments are characterized by a paucity of memories from Recent Life. In comparison with controls, two features of his future thoughts are apparent: Reduced episodic future thinking and outdated content of his episodic future thoughts. CONCLUSIONS: This article suggests neuropsychologists should look beyond popular conceptualizations of the past-future relation in amnesia via focussing on reduced future thinking. Investigating both the quantity and quality of future thoughts produced by amnesic patients may lead to developments in understanding the complex nature of future thinking disorders resulting from memory impairments. PRACTITIONER POINTS: We highlight the clinical importance of examining the content of future thoughts in amnesic patients, rather than only its quantitative reduction. We propose an explanation of how quantitative and qualitative aspects of future thinking could be affected by amnesia. This could provide a useful approach to understand clinical cases of impaired prospection. LIMITATIONS: Systematic group investigations are required to fully examine our hypothesis. Although the current study utilized typical future thinking measures, these may be limited and we highlight the need to develop clinically relevant measures of prospection.


Assuntos
Amnésia/psicologia , Imaginação , Rememoração Mental , Pensamento , Adulto , Função Executiva , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória Episódica , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
6.
PLoS One ; 9(10): e110211, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25330251

RESUMO

Memory performance is usually impaired when participants have to encode information while performing a concurrent task. Recent studies using recall tasks have found that emotional items are more resistant to such cognitive depletion effects than non-emotional items. However, when recognition tasks are used, the same effect is more elusive as recent recognition studies have obtained contradictory results. In two experiments, we provide evidence that negative emotional content can reliably reduce the effects of cognitive depletion on recognition memory only if stimuli with high levels of emotional intensity are used. In particular, we found that recognition performance for realistic pictures was impaired by a secondary 3-back working memory task during encoding if stimuli were emotionally neutral or had moderate levels of negative emotionality. In contrast, when negative pictures with high levels of emotional intensity were used, the detrimental effects of the secondary task were significantly attenuated.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Emoções , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento/fisiologia , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
7.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 67(7): 1249-61, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24215680

RESUMO

In a memory survey, adult respondents recalled, dated, and described two earliest positive and negative memories that they were highly confident were memories. They then answered a series of questions that focused on memory details such as clothing, duration, weather, and so on. Few differences were found between positive and negative memories, which on average had 4/5 details and dated to the age of 6/6.5 years. Memory for details about activity, location, and who was present was good; memory for all other details was poorer or at floor. Taken together, these findings indicate that (full) earliest memories may be considerably later than previously thought and that they rarely contain the sort of specific details targeted by professional investigators. The resulting normative profile of memory details reported here can be used to evaluate overly specific childhood autobiographical memories and to identify memory details with a low probability of recall.


Assuntos
Memória Episódica , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Repressão Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Envelhecimento , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Probabilidade , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
8.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24147452

RESUMO

There is clear evidence of a deficit in episodic memory for older adults compared to younger adults. Using an intertrial technique previous research has investigated whether this deficit can be attributed to a decline in encoding or consolidation. On standard memory tests, these two aspects of memory function can be measured by examining the items forgotten or acquired across multiple learning trials. The present study assessed whether age deficits in episodic memory were affected by stimulus characteristics, specifically age of acquisition (AoA). A standard intertrial design was implemented whereby participants studied word lists over several study-test trials. The stimulus characteristics of AoA were manipulated using a pure-list technique. Our findings showed that older adults demonstrate an overall recall deficit which appeared to be a consequence of both an encoding deficit and consolidation weakness. Earlier-acquired words were recalled significantly better than later-acquired words and this was apparently due to both enhanced encoding and consolidation of earlier- over later-acquired words. The key finding is that older adults show a recall advantage for earlier- compared to later-acquired words over the entire experiment to a greater degree than younger adults.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Aprendizagem , Memória , Idoso , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Memória Episódica , Rememoração Mental , Modelos Psicológicos , Testes Psicológicos , Fatores de Tempo , Vocabulário , Adulto Jovem
9.
PLoS One ; 8(11): e76600, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24223704

RESUMO

The processing of notes and chords which are harmonically incongruous with their context has been shown to elicit two distinct late ERP effects. These effects strongly resemble two effects associated with the processing of linguistic incongruities: a P600, resembling a typical response to syntactic incongruities in language, and an N500, evocative of the N400, which is typically elicited in response to semantic incongruities in language. Despite the robustness of these two patterns in the musical incongruity literature, no consensus has yet been reached as to the reasons for the existence of two distinct responses to harmonic incongruities. This study was the first to use behavioural and ERP data to test two possible explanations for the existence of these two patterns: the musicianship of listeners, and the resolved or unresolved nature of the harmonic incongruities. Results showed that harmonically incongruous notes and chords elicited a late positivity similar to the P600 when they were embedded within sequences which started and ended in the same key (harmonically resolved). The notes and chords which indicated that there would be no return to the original key (leaving the piece harmonically unresolved) were associated with a further P600 in musicians, but with a negativity resembling the N500 in non-musicians. We suggest that the late positivity reflects the conscious perception of a specific element as being incongruous with its context and the efforts of musicians to integrate the harmonic incongruity into its local context as a result of their analytic listening style, while the late negativity reflects the detection of the absence of resolution in non-musicians as a result of their holistic listening style.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos , Estimulação Acústica , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Música , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Semântica , Adulto Jovem
10.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 66(9): 1687-706, 2013 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23442091

RESUMO

Episodic future thinking (EFT) has been linked with our ability to remember past events. However, its specific neurocognitive subprocesses have remained elusive. In Experiment 1, a study of healthy older adults was conducted to investigate the candidate subprocesses of EFT. Participants completed a standard EFT cue word task, two memory measures (Verbal Paired Associates I, Source Memory), and two measures of executive function (Trail Making Test, Tower Test). In Experiment 2, healthy young adults also completed an EFT task and neuropsychological measures. The link between neurocognitive measures and five characteristics of EFT was investigated. Specifically, it was found that Source Memory and Trail Making Test performance predicted the episodic specificity of future events in older but not younger adults. Replicating previous findings, older adults produced future events with greater semantic but fewer episodic details than did young adults. These results extend the data and emphasize the importance of the multiple subprocesses underlying EFT.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Memória Episódica , Pensamento/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Aprendizagem por Associação , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Vocabulário , Adulto Jovem
11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22574638

RESUMO

Older adults have a demonstrable episodic memory deficit. The present study aimed to investigate whether the age deficit in episodic memory was influenced by stimulus characteristics known to produce differences in memory performance in younger adults, specifically word frequency. An intertrial paradigm was used whereby participants studied high- or low-frequency lists over several study-test trials, and the loss and gain of individual items was measured across trials; putative measures of consolidation and encoding. The results show that high-frequency words are recalled significantly better than low-frequency words. Older adults acquired high-frequency words at a greater rate across trials than they did for low-frequency words, an effect not evident in the younger adults. Older adults were found to have deficits in both encoding and consolidation as measured by losses and gains of items across trials. The results support the inter-item association theory of the word frequency effect on recall, with the age differences suggesting that memory deficits are sensitive to stimuli characteristics - one interpretation being that the ease of processing of the stimuli at encoding facilitates later recall.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Memória Episódica , Aprendizagem Verbal/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos
12.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 66(6): 1196-203, 2013 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23136887

RESUMO

The aim of the experiment reported here was to investigate the processes underlying the construction of truthful and deliberately fabricated memories. Properties of memories created to be intentionally false (fabricated memories) were compared to properties of memories believed to be true (true memories). Participants recalled and then wrote or spoke true memories and fabricated memories of everyday events. It was found that true memories were reliably more vivid than fabricated memories and were nearly always recalled from a first-person perspective. In contrast, fabricated differed from true memories in that they were judged to be reliably older, were more frequently recalled from a third-person perspective, and linguistic analysis revealed that they required more cognitive effort to generate. No notable differences were found across modality of reporting. Finally, it was found that intentionally fabricated memories were created by recalling and then "editing" true memories. Overall, these findings show that true and fabricated memories systematically differ, despite the fact that both are based on true memories.


Assuntos
Imaginação/fisiologia , Intenção , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Repressão Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
13.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 65(12): 2288-96, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23190177

RESUMO

Studies exploring mental time travel commonly use cue-word paradigms to elicit past and future autobiographical events. However, the effect of trial duration (how long participants are allowed to describe events) on the relationship between episodic and nonepisodic detail and episodic specificity (i.e., whether longer durations increase event specificity) has not been examined. To resolve these issues, a cue-word study was devised whereby participants described past and future events under three randomly administered time constraints: short (1-min), standard (3-min), and long (5-min) durations. Findings indicated that an individual's capacity for episodic and nonepisodic thought for the past and future were unrelated. This lends supports to the idea that independent mechanisms are responsible for episodic and semantic information. This study also offers clarity concerning the effect of different trial durations on episodic specificity, which may aid the design of future studies of mental time travel.


Assuntos
Imaginação , Memória Episódica , Semântica , Adolescente , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Estatística como Assunto , Estatísticas não Paramétricas , Pensamento , Fatores de Tempo , Testes de Associação de Palavras , Adulto Jovem
14.
Behav Res Methods ; 44(1): 81-94, 2012 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21805062

RESUMO

Research into similarities between music and language processing is currently experiencing a strong renewed interest. Recent methodological advances have led to neuroimaging studies presenting striking similarities between neural patterns associated with the processing of music and language--notably, in the study of participants' responses to elements that are incongruous with their musical or linguistic context. Responding to a call for greater systematicity by leading researchers in the field of music and language psychology, this article describes the creation, selection, and validation of a set of auditory stimuli in which both congruence and resolution were manipulated in equivalent ways across harmony, rhythm, semantics, and syntax. Three conditions were created by changing the contexts preceding and following musical and linguistic incongruities originally used for effect by authors and composers: Stimuli in the incongruous-resolved condition reproduced the original incongruity and resolution into the same context; stimuli in the incongruous-unresolved condition reproduced the incongruity but continued postincongruity with a new context dictated by the incongruity; and stimuli in the congruous condition presented the same element of interest, but the entire context was adapted to match it so that it was no longer incongruous. The manipulations described in this article rendered unrecognizable the original incongruities from which the stimuli were adapted, while maintaining ecological validity. The norming procedure and validation study resulted in a significant increase in perceived oddity from congruous to incongruous-resolved and from incongruous-resolved to incongruous-unresolved in all four components of music and language, making this set of stimuli a theoretically grounded and empirically validated resource for this growing area of research.


Assuntos
Idioma , Música , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
15.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 64(9): 1665-71, 2011 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21838653

RESUMO

Judgements of learning (JOLs) are self-made predictions of the likelihood that one will later recall information. The influence of stimulus characteristics on JOLs and recall continues to receive attention, yet there are still a number of unexplored lexical word features that may exert an effect on mnemonic processing. Using a standard cue-target paradigm, we focused on the role of word age of acquisition (AoA) and evaluated the role of both cue and target AoA on responses. We replicated the robust delayed-JOL effect and used a novel items analysis approach to examine the relationship between intrinsic word features and accuracy and reaction times for both JOLs and recall. A consistent effect of target AoA was found, even after controlling for a range of covariates previously shown to impact JOLs and recall. These results expand the role of AoA in word processing and suggest that it is a key variable in memory and metacognition; they also support Koriat's (1997) cue utilization framework.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Julgamento/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Associação de Pares/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Aprendizagem Verbal/fisiologia , Vocabulário , Adulto Jovem
16.
Cognition ; 116(1): 23-32, 2010 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20363469

RESUMO

In two experiments autobiographical memories from childhood were recalled to cue words naming common objects, locations, activities and emotions. Participants recalled their earliest specific memory associated with each word and dated their age at the time of the remembered event. A striking and specific finding emerged: age of earliest memory was systematically later, by several months, than the age of acquisition of the word to which it was associated. This was the case for earlier and later acquired words, for all word types, and for younger as well as older adults. It is suggested that this systematic lag reflects the formation of conceptual knowledge that is abstracted from details represented in early episodic memories. It is not until such knowledge is formed that a word cue and the conceptual knowledge in long-term to which it corresponds, can be used to access specific episodic memories. The implications of this for understanding childhood amnesia and for theories of the development of autobiographical memory are considered.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Memória/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Emoções , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicolinguística , Leitura , Adulto Jovem
17.
Psychopathology ; 43(2): 121-6, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20110764

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: There is a growing awareness of a psychiatric construct that needs to be better defined and understood: Internet addiction (IA). Recently there has been much public concern over the relationship between Internet use and negative affect. This study explored the concept of IA and examined the relationship between addictive symptoms and depression. SAMPLING AND METHODS: An online questionnaire was used to measure participants' Internet use, the functions for which they used the Internet, and their depressive tendencies. Three scales were included: the IA Test, the Internet Function Questionnaire and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). 1,319 respondents completed the questionnaires, with 18 (1.2%) identified as falling in the IA category. RESULTS: Correlational analyses were conducted across the whole data sample. In factorial analyses, the 18 IA respondents were compared to a matched group of non-addicted (NA) respondents in terms of their scores on the Function Test and the BDI. Across the whole data sample, there was a close relationship between IA tendencies and depression, such that IA respondents were more depressed; there were also significant differences between the sexes, with men showing more addictive tendencies than women. In addition, young people were significantly more likely to show addictive symptoms than were older people. There was a significant difference between the IA and the NA group in their levels of depressive symptoms, with the NA group firmly in the non-depressed range, and the IA group in the moderately-to-severely depressed range (F(1, 34) = 22.35; p < 0.001). In terms of the function for which they used the Internet, the IA group engaged significantly more than the NA group in sexually gratifying websites, gaming websites and online community/chat websites. CONCLUSIONS: The concept of IA is emerging as a construct that must be taken seriously. Moreover, it is linked to depression, such that those who regard themselves as dependent on the Internet report high levels of depressive symptoms. Those who show symptoms of IA are likely to engage proportionately more than the normal population in sites that serve as a replacement for real-life socialising. Further work needs to be done on validating this relationship. Future research is needed to corroborate the existing evidence and address the nature of the relationship between IA and depression: there is comorbidity between these conditions that needs greater investigation.


Assuntos
Comportamento Aditivo/epidemiologia , Transtorno Depressivo/epidemiologia , Internet/estatística & dados numéricos , Adolescente , Adulto , Comportamento Aditivo/diagnóstico , Comportamento Aditivo/psicologia , Comorbidade , Estudos Transversais , Transtorno Depressivo/diagnóstico , Transtorno Depressivo/psicologia , Feminino , Inquéritos Epidemiológicos , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores Sexuais , Comportamento Social , Isolamento Social , Inquéritos e Questionários , Revisão da Utilização de Recursos de Saúde/estatística & dados numéricos , Adulto Jovem
18.
Neurobiol Aging ; 27(10): 1425-30, 2006 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16202482

RESUMO

The study examined odour identification ability in healthy older adults at increased risk for developing Alzheimer's disease (AD). We recruited a sample (n = 24) of siblings related to probable AD cases and an age-matched control sample (n = 47). All participants were genotyped for the presence of the ApoE epsilon4 allele. Performance on a simple olfactory task of odour identification was compared according to positive family history of AD and ApoE epsilon4 status. The sibling group showed an odour identification deficit compared to the control group. Whilst there was no independent influence of ApoE epsilon4 status on odour identification, there was a significant interaction between positive family history and ApoE epsilon4 status. Sibling epsilon4 carriers showed the greatest odour identification deficit and their performance was significantly poorer than both the sibling non-epsilon4 carrier and control epsilon4 carrier groups. Odour identification deficits like those reported here are considered to be early cognitive markers of incipient AD. In this respect, these findings support the need to both monitor individuals at increased risk of the disease and introduce olfactory-mediated cognitive tasks into the diagnostic setting.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Doença de Alzheimer/genética , Transtornos do Olfato/diagnóstico , Transtornos do Olfato/genética , Medição de Risco/métodos , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Doença de Alzheimer/epidemiologia , Comorbidade , Família , Feminino , Marcadores Genéticos/genética , Predisposição Genética para Doença/epidemiologia , Predisposição Genética para Doença/genética , Testes Genéticos/métodos , Heterozigoto , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos do Olfato/epidemiologia , Prevalência , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Fatores de Risco , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Reino Unido
19.
Percept Mot Skills ; 97(1): 80-2, 2003 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14604025

RESUMO

Shibihara and Kondo in 2002 reported a reanalysis of the 1997 Kanji picture-naming data of Yamazaki, Ellis, Morrison, and Lambon-Ralph in which independent variables were highly correlated. Their addition of the variable visual familiarity altered the previously reported pattern of results, indicating that visual familiarity, but not age of acquisition, was important in predicting Kanji naming speed. The present paper argues that caution should be taken when drawing conclusions from multiple regression analyses in which the independent variables are so highly correlated, as such multicollinearity can lead to unreliable output.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Idioma , Humanos , Análise de Regressão , Percepção Visual
20.
Q J Exp Psychol A ; 56(4): 705-30, 2003 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12745837

RESUMO

Young and old adults were shown pictured or written verbs and asked to name them as quickly as possible. Simultaneous multiple regression was used to investigate which of a set of potential variables predicted naming speed. Age of acquisition was found to be an important predictor of naming speed in both young and old adults, and for both word and picture naming. Word frequency predicted picture-naming speed only in older adults and failed to make any significant contribution to word-naming speeds for either group of participants. The respective loci and roles of age of acquisition and frequency in lexical processing are discussed in the light of these findings.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento , Aprendizagem , Comportamento Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Cognição , Humanos , Distribuição Aleatória , Tempo de Reação , Valores de Referência , Percepção Visual
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...