Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 68
Filtrar
1.
J Vis ; 24(6): 6, 2024 Jun 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38843389

RESUMO

Infant primates see poorly, and most perceptual functions mature steadily beyond early infancy. Behavioral studies on human and macaque infants show that global form perception, as measured by the ability to integrate contour information into a coherent percept, improves dramatically throughout the first several years after birth. However, it is unknown when sensitivity to curvature and shape emerges in early life or how it develops. We studied the development of shape sensitivity in 18 macaques, aged 2 months to 10 years. Using radial frequency stimuli, circular targets whose radii are modulated sinusoidally, we tested monkeys' ability to radial frequency stimuli from circles as a function of the depth and frequency of sinusoidal modulation. We implemented a new four-choice oddity task and compared the resulting data with that from a traditional two-alternative forced choice task. We found that radial frequency pattern perception was measurable at the youngest age tested (2 months). Behavioral performance at all radial frequencies improved with age. Performance was better for higher radial frequencies, suggesting the developing visual system prioritizes processing of fine visual details that are ecologically relevant. By using two complementary methods, we were able to capture a comprehensive developmental trajectory for shape perception.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Macaca mulatta , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Estimulação Luminosa , Animais , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Masculino , Feminino
2.
bioRxiv ; 2024 Apr 29.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38617271

RESUMO

Crowding is the failure to recognize an object due to insufficient spacing, which slows daily tasks such as reading and search. Across 49 observers, we found large variations in psychophysical crowding distance and retinotopic map size. These measures covary, conserving a 1.4-mm cortical crowding distance (threshold object spacing on the cortical surface) in the human V4 map, but not V1-V3. This links the spacing limit of visual recognition to overall V4 size.

3.
bioRxiv ; 2024 Apr 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38463955

RESUMO

We studied visual development in macaque monkeys using texture stimuli, matched in local spectral content but varying in "naturalistic" structure. In adult monkeys, naturalistic textures preferentially drive neurons in areas V2 and V4, but not V1. We paired behavioral measurements of naturalness sensitivity with separately-obtained neuronal population recordings from neurons in areas V1, V2, V4, and inferotemporal cortex (IT). We made behavioral measurements from 16 weeks of age and physiological measurements as early as 20 weeks, and continued through 56 weeks. Behavioral sensitivity reached half of maximum at roughly 25 weeks of age. Neural sensitivities remained stable from the earliest ages tested. As in adults, neural sensitivity to naturalistic structure increased from V1 to V2 to V4. While sensitivities in V2 and IT were similar, the dimensionality of the IT representation was more similar to V4's than to V2's.

4.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 17: 1255465, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38094145

RESUMO

Online methods allow testing of larger, more diverse populations, with much less effort than in-lab testing. However, many psychophysical measurements, including visual crowding, require accurate eye fixation, which is classically achieved by testing only experienced observers who have learned to fixate reliably, or by using a gaze tracker to restrict testing to moments when fixation is accurate. Alas, both approaches are impractical online as online observers tend to be inexperienced, and online gaze tracking, using the built-in webcam, has a low precision (±4 deg). EasyEyes open-source software reliably measures peripheral thresholds online with accurate fixation achieved in a novel way, without gaze tracking. It tells observers to use the cursor to track a moving crosshair. At a random time during successful tracking, a brief target is presented in the periphery. The observer responds by identifying the target. To evaluate EasyEyes fixation accuracy and thresholds, we tested 12 naive observers in three ways in a counterbalanced order: first, in the laboratory, using gaze-contingent stimulus presentation; second, in the laboratory, using EasyEyes while independently monitoring gaze using EyeLink 1000; third, online at home, using EasyEyes. We find that crowding thresholds are consistent and individual differences are conserved. The small root mean square (RMS) fixation error (0.6 deg) during target presentation eliminates the need for gaze tracking. Thus, this method enables fixation-dependent measurements online, for easy testing of larger and more diverse populations.

5.
J Vis ; 23(8): 6, 2023 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37540179

RESUMO

Crowding is the failure to recognize an object due to surrounding clutter. Our visual crowding survey measured 13 crowding distances (or "critical spacings") twice in each of 50 observers. The survey includes three eccentricities (0, 5, and 10 deg), four cardinal meridians, two orientations (radial and tangential), and two fonts (Sloan and Pelli). The survey also tested foveal acuity, twice. Remarkably, fitting a two-parameter model-the well-known Bouma law, where crowding distance grows linearly with eccentricity-explains 82% of the variance for all 13 × 50 measured log crowding distances, cross-validated. An enhanced Bouma law, with factors for meridian, crowding orientation, target kind, and observer, explains 94% of the variance, again cross-validated. These additional factors reveal several asymmetries, consistent with previous reports, which can be expressed as crowding-distance ratios: 0.62 horizontal:vertical, 0.79 lower:upper, 0.78 right:left, 0.55 tangential:radial, and 0.78 Sloan-font:Pelli-font. Across our observers, peripheral crowding is independent of foveal crowding and acuity. Evaluation of the Bouma factor, b (the slope of the Bouma law), as a biomarker of visual health would be easier if there were a way to compare results across crowding studies that use different methods. We define a standardized Bouma factor b' that corrects for differences from Bouma's 25 choice alternatives, 75% threshold criterion, and linearly symmetric flanker placement. For radial crowding on the right meridian, the standardized Bouma factor b' is 0.24 for this study, 0.35 for Bouma (1970), and 0.30 for the geometric mean across five representative modern studies, including this one, showing good agreement across labs, including Bouma's. Simulations, confirmed by data, show that peeking can skew estimates of crowding (e.g., greatly decreasing the mean or doubling the SD of log b). Using gaze tracking to prevent peeking, individual differences are robust, as evidenced by the much larger 0.08 SD of log b across observers than the mere 0.03 test-retest SD of log b measured in half an hour. The ease of measurement of crowding enhances its promise as a biomarker for dyslexia and visual health.


Assuntos
Dislexia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Humanos , Fator B do Complemento , Aglomeração
6.
bioRxiv ; 2023 Jul 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37503301

RESUMO

Online methods allow testing of larger, more diverse populations, with much less effort than in-lab testing. However, many psychophysical measurements, including visual crowding, require accurate eye fixation, which is classically achieved by testing only experienced observers who have learned to fixate reliably, or by using a gaze tracker to restrict testing to moments when fixation is accurate. Alas, both approaches are impractical online since online observers tend to be inexperienced, and online gaze tracking, using the built-in webcam, has a low precision (±4 deg, Papoutsaki et al., 2016). The EasyEyes open-source software reliably measures peripheral thresholds online with accurate fixation achieved in a novel way, without gaze tracking. EasyEyes tells observers to use the cursor to track a moving crosshair. At a random time during successful tracking, a brief target is presented in the periphery. The observer responds by identifying the target. To evaluate EasyEyes fixation accuracy and thresholds, we tested 12 naive observers in three ways in a counterbalanced order: first, in the lab, using gaze-contingent stimulus presentation (Kurzawski et al., 2023; Pelli et al., 2016); second, in the lab, using EasyEyes while independently monitoring gaze; third, online at home, using EasyEyes. We find that crowding thresholds are consistent (no significant differences in mean and variance of thresholds across ways) and individual differences are conserved. The small root mean square (RMS) fixation error (0.6 deg) during target presentation eliminates the need for gaze tracking. Thus, EasyEyes enables fixation-dependent measurements online, for easy testing of larger and more diverse populations.

7.
J Vis ; 23(2): 4, 2023 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36745452

RESUMO

Natural images contain information at multiple spatial scales. Though we understand how early visual mechanisms split multiscale images into distinct spatial frequency channels, we do not know how the outputs of these channels are processed further by mid-level visual mechanisms. We have recently developed a texture discrimination task that uses synthetic, multi-scale, "naturalistic" textures to isolate these mid-level mechanisms. Here, we use three experimental manipulations (image blur, image rescaling, and eccentric viewing) to show that perceptual sensitivity to naturalistic structure is strongly dependent on features at high object spatial frequencies (measured in cycles/image). As a result, sensitivity depends on a texture acuity limit, a property of the visual system that sets the highest retinal spatial frequency (measured in cycles/degree) at which observers can detect naturalistic features. Analysis of the texture images using a model observer analysis shows that naturalistic image features at high object spatial frequencies carry more task-relevant information than those at low object spatial frequencies. That is, the dependence of sensitivity on high object spatial frequencies is a property of the texture images, rather than a property of the visual system. Accordingly, we find human observers' ability to extract naturalistic information (their efficiency) is similar for all object spatial frequencies. We conclude that the mid-level mechanisms that underlie perceptual sensitivity effectively extract information from all image features below the texture acuity limit, regardless of their retinal and object spatial frequency.


Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica , Retina , Humanos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos
8.
J Vis ; 18(13): 2, 2018 12 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30508427

RESUMO

Many vision science studies employ machine learning, especially the version called "deep learning." Neuroscientists use machine learning to decode neural responses. Perception scientists try to understand how living organisms recognize objects. To them, deep neural networks offer benchmark accuracies for recognition of learned stimuli. Originally machine learning was inspired by the brain. Today, machine learning is used as a statistical tool to decode brain activity. Tomorrow, deep neural networks might become our best model of brain function. This brief overview of the use of machine learning in biological vision touches on its strengths, weaknesses, milestones, controversies, and current directions. Here, we hope to help vision scientists assess what role machine learning should play in their research.


Assuntos
Aprendizado Profundo , Redes Neurais de Computação , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Algoritmos , Encéfalo , Humanos
9.
J Neurosci ; 37(36): 8734-8741, 2017 09 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28760867

RESUMO

In amblyopia, abnormal visual experience leads to an extreme form of eye dominance, in which vision through the nondominant eye is degraded. A key aspect of this disorder is perceptual suppression: the image seen by the stronger eye often dominates during binocular viewing, blocking the image of the weaker eye from reaching awareness. Interocular suppression is the focus of ongoing work aimed at understanding and treating amblyopia, yet its physiological basis remains unknown. We measured binocular interactions in visual cortex of anesthetized amblyopic monkeys (female Macaca nemestrina), using 96-channel "Utah" arrays to record from populations of neurons in V1 and V2. In an experiment reported recently (Hallum et al., 2017), we found that reduced excitatory input from the amblyopic eye (AE) revealed a form of balanced binocular suppression that is unaltered in amblyopia. Here, we report on the modulation of the gain of excitatory signals from the AE by signals from its dominant fellow eye (FE). Using a dichoptic masking technique, we found that AE responses to grating stimuli were attenuated by the presentation of a noise mask to the FE, as in a normal control animal. Responses to FE stimuli, by contrast, could not be masked from the AE. We conclude that a weakened ability of the amblyopic eye to modulate cortical response gain creates an imbalance of suppression that favors the dominant eye.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT In amblyopia, vision in one eye is impaired as a result of abnormal early visual experience. Behavioral observations in humans with amblyopia suggest that much of their visual loss is due to active suppression of their amblyopic eye. Here we describe experiments in which we studied binocular interactions in macaques with experimentally induced amblyopia. In normal monkeys, the gain of neuronal response to stimulation of one eye is modulated by contrast in the other eye, but in monkeys with amblyopia the balance of gain modulation is altered so that the weaker, amblyopic eye has little effect while the stronger fellow eye has a strong effect. This asymmetric suppression may be a key component of the perceptual losses in amblyopia.


Assuntos
Ambliopia/fisiopatologia , Dominância Ocular , Inibição Neural , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Estimulação Luminosa , Visão Binocular , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Macaca nemestrina , Rede Nervosa/fisiopatologia
10.
J Neurosci ; 37(34): 8216-8226, 2017 08 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28743725

RESUMO

In amblyopia, a visual disorder caused by abnormal visual experience during development, the amblyopic eye (AE) loses visual sensitivity whereas the fellow eye (FE) is largely unaffected. Binocular vision in amblyopes is often disrupted by interocular suppression. We used 96-electrode arrays to record neurons and neuronal groups in areas V1 and V2 of six female macaque monkeys (Macaca nemestrina) made amblyopic by artificial strabismus or anisometropia in early life, as well as two visually normal female controls. To measure suppressive binocular interactions directly, we recorded neuronal responses to dichoptic stimulation. We stimulated both eyes simultaneously with large sinusoidal gratings, controlling their contrast independently with raised-cosine modulators of different orientations and spatial frequencies. We modeled each eye's receptive field at each cortical site using a difference of Gaussian envelopes and derived estimates of the strength of central excitation and surround suppression. We used these estimates to calculate ocular dominance separately for excitation and suppression. Excitatory drive from the FE dominated amblyopic visual cortex, especially in more severe amblyopes, but suppression from both the FE and AEs was prevalent in all animals. This imbalance created strong interocular suppression in deep amblyopes: increasing contrast in the AE decreased responses at binocular cortical sites. These response patterns reveal mechanisms that likely contribute to the interocular suppression that disrupts vision in amblyopes.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Amblyopia is a developmental visual disorder that alters both monocular vision and binocular interaction. Using microelectrode arrays, we examined binocular interaction in primary visual cortex and V2 of six amblyopic macaque monkeys (Macaca nemestrina) and two visually normal controls. By stimulating the eyes dichoptically, we showed that, in amblyopic cortex, the binocular combination of signals is altered. The excitatory influence of the two eyes is imbalanced to a degree that can be predicted from the severity of amblyopia, whereas suppression from both eyes is prevalent in all animals. This altered balance of excitation and suppression reflects mechanisms that may contribute to the interocular perceptual suppression that disrupts vision in amblyopes.


Assuntos
Ambliopia/fisiopatologia , Dominância Ocular/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Córtex Visual/fisiopatologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Macaca nemestrina , Microeletrodos , Estrabismo/fisiopatologia
11.
Nat Neurosci ; 19(4): 613-22, 2016 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26900926

RESUMO

Extensive research has revealed that the ventral visual stream hierarchically builds a robust representation for supporting visual object categorization tasks. We systematically explored the ability of multiple ventral visual areas to support a variety of 'category-orthogonal' object properties such as position, size and pose. For complex naturalistic stimuli, we found that the inferior temporal (IT) population encodes all measured category-orthogonal object properties, including those properties often considered to be low-level features (for example, position), more explicitly than earlier ventral stream areas. We also found that the IT population better predicts human performance patterns across properties. A hierarchical neural network model based on simple computational principles generates these same cross-area patterns of information. Taken together, our empirical results support the hypothesis that all behaviorally relevant object properties are extracted in concert up the ventral visual hierarchy, and our computational model explains how that hierarchy might be built.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia
12.
Int J Tuberc Lung Dis ; 19(12): 1455-62, 2015 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26614186

RESUMO

SETTING: Tuberculosis spondylodiscitis (TS), or Pott's disease, an extra-pulmonary form of tuberculosis (TB), is rare and difficult to diagnose in children. Some cases of severe TB in children were recently explained by inborn errors of immunity affecting the interleukin-12/interferon-gamma (IL-12/IFN-γ) axis. OBJECTIVE: To analyse clinical data on Moroccan children with TS, and to perform immunological and genetic explorations of the IL-12/IFN-γ axis. DESIGN: We studied nine children with TS diagnosed between 2012 and 2014. We investigated the IL-12/IFN-γ circuit by both whole-blood assays and sequencing of the coding regions of 14 core genes of this pathway. RESULTS: A diagnosis of TS was based on a combination of clinical, biological, histological and radiological data. QuantiFERON(®)-TB Gold In-Tube results were positive in 75% of patients. Whole-blood assays showed normal IL-12 and IFN-γ production in all but one patient, who displayed impaired decreased response to IL-12. No candidate disease-causing mutations were detected in the exonic regions of the 14 genes. CONCLUSIONS: TS diagnosis in children remains challenging, and is based largely on imaging. Further investigations of TS in children are required to determine the role of genetic defects in pathways that may or may not be related to the IL-12/IFN-γ axis.


Assuntos
Interferon gama/sangue , Interleucina-12/sangue , Tuberculose da Coluna Vertebral/imunologia , Adolescente , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Tratamento Farmacológico , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Marrocos , Mycobacterium tuberculosis , Estudos Prospectivos , Tomografia Computadorizada por Raios X , Teste Tuberculínico
13.
J Neurosci ; 35(39): 13402-18, 2015 Sep 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26424887

RESUMO

To go beyond qualitative models of the biological substrate of object recognition, we ask: can a single ventral stream neuronal linking hypothesis quantitatively account for core object recognition performance over a broad range of tasks? We measured human performance in 64 object recognition tests using thousands of challenging images that explore shape similarity and identity preserving object variation. We then used multielectrode arrays to measure neuronal population responses to those same images in visual areas V4 and inferior temporal (IT) cortex of monkeys and simulated V1 population responses. We tested leading candidate linking hypotheses and control hypotheses, each postulating how ventral stream neuronal responses underlie object recognition behavior. Specifically, for each hypothesis, we computed the predicted performance on the 64 tests and compared it with the measured pattern of human performance. All tested hypotheses based on low- and mid-level visually evoked activity (pixels, V1, and V4) were very poor predictors of the human behavioral pattern. However, simple learned weighted sums of distributed average IT firing rates exactly predicted the behavioral pattern. More elaborate linking hypotheses relying on IT trial-by-trial correlational structure, finer IT temporal codes, or ones that strictly respect the known spatial substructures of IT ("face patches") did not improve predictive power. Although these results do not reject those more elaborate hypotheses, they suggest a simple, sufficient quantitative model: each object recognition task is learned from the spatially distributed mean firing rates (100 ms) of ∼60,000 IT neurons and is executed as a simple weighted sum of those firing rates. Significance statement: We sought to go beyond qualitative models of visual object recognition and determine whether a single neuronal linking hypothesis can quantitatively account for core object recognition behavior. To achieve this, we designed a database of images for evaluating object recognition performance. We used multielectrode arrays to characterize hundreds of neurons in the visual ventral stream of nonhuman primates and measured the object recognition performance of >100 human observers. Remarkably, we found that simple learned weighted sums of firing rates of neurons in monkey inferior temporal (IT) cortex accurately predicted human performance. Although previous work led us to expect that IT would outperform V4, we were surprised by the quantitative precision with which simple IT-based linking hypotheses accounted for human behavior.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Algoritmos , Animais , Simulação por Computador , Potenciais Evocados Visuais/fisiologia , Humanos , Macaca mulatta , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Especificidade da Espécie , Lobo Temporal/citologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
14.
Vision Res ; 114: 56-67, 2015 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25637856

RESUMO

Amblyopia is a developmental disorder resulting in poor vision in one eye. The mechanism by which input to the affected eye is prevented from reaching the level of awareness remains poorly understood. We recorded simultaneously from large populations of neurons in the supragranular layers of areas V1 and V2 in 6 macaques that were made amblyopic by rearing with artificial strabismus or anisometropia, and 1 normally reared control. In agreement with previous reports, we found that cortical neuronal signals driven through the amblyopic eyes were reduced, and that cortical neurons were on average more strongly driven by the non-amblyopic than by the amblyopic eyes. We analyzed multiunit recordings using standard population decoding methods, and found that visual signals from the amblyopic eye, while weakened, were not degraded enough to explain the behavioral deficits. Thus additional losses must arise in downstream processing. We tested the idea that under monocular viewing conditions, only signals from neurons dominated by - rather than driven by - the open eye might be used. This reduces the proportion of neuronal signals available from the amblyopic eye, and amplifies the interocular difference observed at the level of single neurons. We conclude that amblyopia might arise in part from degradation in the neuronal signals from the amblyopic eye, and in part from a reduction in the number of signals processed by downstream areas.


Assuntos
Ambliopia/fisiopatologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Anisometropia/fisiopatologia , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Dominância Ocular/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Fóvea Central/fisiologia , Macaca , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Estrabismo/fisiopatologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia
15.
Arch Pediatr ; 21(12): 1348-52, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Francês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25445126

RESUMO

IL-12 receptor ß1 deficiency (IL-12Rß1) predisposes patients to mycobacteria and Salmonella infections. We report a case of IL-12Rß1 deficiency with a fatal multi-resistant Salmonella enteritidis infection. This boy was born after from a consanguineous marriage, and diagnosed as having a IL-12Rß1 deficiency since the age of 3 months. He presented with recurrent Salmonella enteritidis essentially digestive localization, complicated by purulent pericarditis at the same germ at the age of two and a half years. At the age of 3, a colonic infiltration due to a Salmonella enteritidis resistant to antibiotics, was complicated by acute intussusception, and the child died. The IL-12Rß1 deficiency is considered as having a good prognosis, in contrast to what happened in our patient. We review therapeutic issues in these patients.


Assuntos
Doenças do Colo/microbiologia , Intussuscepção/microbiologia , Pericardite/microbiologia , Infecções por Salmonella/complicações , Salmonella enteritidis , Doença Aguda , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Masculino , Supuração/microbiologia
16.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 10(12): e1003963, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25521294

RESUMO

The primate visual system achieves remarkable visual object recognition performance even in brief presentations, and under changes to object exemplar, geometric transformations, and background variation (a.k.a. core visual object recognition). This remarkable performance is mediated by the representation formed in inferior temporal (IT) cortex. In parallel, recent advances in machine learning have led to ever higher performing models of object recognition using artificial deep neural networks (DNNs). It remains unclear, however, whether the representational performance of DNNs rivals that of the brain. To accurately produce such a comparison, a major difficulty has been a unifying metric that accounts for experimental limitations, such as the amount of noise, the number of neural recording sites, and the number of trials, and computational limitations, such as the complexity of the decoding classifier and the number of classifier training examples. In this work, we perform a direct comparison that corrects for these experimental limitations and computational considerations. As part of our methodology, we propose an extension of "kernel analysis" that measures the generalization accuracy as a function of representational complexity. Our evaluations show that, unlike previous bio-inspired models, the latest DNNs rival the representational performance of IT cortex on this visual object recognition task. Furthermore, we show that models that perform well on measures of representational performance also perform well on measures of representational similarity to IT, and on measures of predicting individual IT multi-unit responses. Whether these DNNs rely on computational mechanisms similar to the primate visual system is yet to be determined, but, unlike all previous bio-inspired models, that possibility cannot be ruled out merely on representational performance grounds.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Redes Neurais de Computação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Algoritmos , Animais , Macaca mulatta , Masculino
17.
J Clin Immunol ; 34(4): 459-68, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24619622

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Primary immunodeficiencies (PIDs) are a large group of diseases characterized by susceptibility to infections. We provide the first comprehensive report on PIDs in Morocco, the epidemiological, clinical, etiological and outcome features which have never before been described. METHODS: A national registry was established in 2008, grouping together data for PID patients diagnosed since 1998. RESULTS: In total, 421 patients were diagnosed between 1998 and 2012. Parental consanguinity was common (recorded for 43.2 % of patients) and the median time to diagnosis was 2.0 years. Overall, 27.4 % of patients were considered to have well defined syndromes with immunodeficiency (48 cases of hyper-IgE syndrome and 40 of ataxia-telangiectasia); 22.7 % had predominantly antibody deficiencies (29 cases of agammaglobulinemia and 24 of CVID); 20.6 % had combined immunodeficiencies (37 cases of SCID and 26 of MHC II deficiencies) and 17.5 % had phagocyte disorders (14 cases of SCN and 10 of CGD). The principal clinical signs were lower respiratory tract infections (60.8 %), skin infections (33.5 %) and candidiasis (26.1 %). Mortality reached 28.8 %, and only ten patients underwent bone marrow transplantation. We analyzed the impact on mortality of residence, family history, parental consanguinity, date of diagnosis and time to diagnosis, but only date of diagnosis had a significant effect. CONCLUSIONS: The observed prevalence of PID was 0.81/100,000 inhabitants, suggesting considerable underdiagnosis and a need to increase awareness of these conditions in Morocco. The distribution of PIDs was different from that reported in Western countries, with a particularly high proportion of SCID, MHC II deficiencies, hyper-IgE syndrome and autosomal recessive agammaglobulinemia. However, we have now organized a national network, which should improve diagnosis rates in remote regions.


Assuntos
Síndromes de Imunodeficiência/diagnóstico , Síndromes de Imunodeficiência/epidemiologia , Sistema de Registros , Adolescente , Adulto , Transplante de Medula Óssea , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Consanguinidade , Feminino , Humanos , Imunoglobulinas Intravenosas/uso terapêutico , Síndromes de Imunodeficiência/classificação , Síndromes de Imunodeficiência/terapia , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Marrocos/epidemiologia , Prevalência
18.
Arch Pediatr ; 20(3): 282-5, 2013 Mar.
Artigo em Francês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23375710

RESUMO

Streptococcus intermedius is a member of the Streptococcus anginosus group, also known as the Streptococcus milleri group. Although this is a commensal agent of the mouth and upper airways, it has been recognized as an important pathogen in the formation of abscesses. However, it has rarely been involved in the formation of brain abscess in children. We report 4 pediatric cases of brain abscess caused by S. intermedius. Three boys and 1 girl, all aged over 2 years, were admitted for a febrile meningeal syndrome and seizures, caused by a S. intermedius brain abscess. Diagnosis was obtained by brain imaging combined with culture of cerebrospinal fluid. The outcome was favorable after antibiotic therapy and abscess puncture. S. intermedius should be considered a potential pathogen involved in the development of brain abscess in children.


Assuntos
Abscesso Encefálico/microbiologia , Infecções Estreptocócicas/complicações , Streptococcus intermedius , Adolescente , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
19.
Med Mal Infect ; 42(12): 615-8, 2012 Dec.
Artigo em Francês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23102429

RESUMO

An increased incidence and severity of invasive group A streptococcus (GAS) infections over the past decade have been reported by several authors, but GAS remains an uncommon cause of bacterial meningitis. The aim of this study was to describe and analyze the clinical and biological data of GAS meningitis by reporting 10 new cases of pediatric GAS meningitis and making a literature review. The mean age of patients, seven girls and three boys, was 3 years. There was a history of preexisting or concomitant community-acquired infection in five patients over 10. The outcome was fatal in two cases. All patients received an initial empirical antimicrobial therapy with a third generation cephalosporin switched in six cases to amoxicillin. The prognosis for this type of streptococcal meningitis is usually good, but death may occur even in children without any identified risk factor for severe infection.


Assuntos
Meningites Bacterianas/epidemiologia , Infecções Estreptocócicas/epidemiologia , Streptococcus pyogenes/isolamento & purificação , Amoxicilina/uso terapêutico , Ampicilina/uso terapêutico , Cefalosporinas/uso terapêutico , Pré-Escolar , Infecções Comunitárias Adquiridas/epidemiologia , Infecções Comunitárias Adquiridas/microbiologia , Infecção Hospitalar/epidemiologia , Infecção Hospitalar/microbiologia , Feminino , Hospitais Universitários/estatística & dados numéricos , Humanos , Incidência , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Meningites Bacterianas/tratamento farmacológico , Meningites Bacterianas/microbiologia , Meningites Bacterianas/mortalidade , Marrocos/epidemiologia , Complicações Pós-Operatórias/epidemiologia , Complicações Pós-Operatórias/microbiologia , Infecções Estreptocócicas/tratamento farmacológico , Infecções Estreptocócicas/microbiologia , Infecções Estreptocócicas/mortalidade
20.
Arch Pediatr ; 19(7): 711-3, 2012 Jul.
Artigo em Francês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22682519

RESUMO

Chylothorax is a rare disease (1-2 % of pleural effusions), with a prevalence between 1/8600 and 1/15,000 births. It is characterized by the presence of chyle in the pleural cavity. Three categories of chylothorax are known: congenital chylothorax, which can be either idiopathic or the result of a malformation, and traumatic chylothorax (mostly postoperative). We report the observation of a 9-month-old infant with idiopathic chylothorax revealed by respiratory symptoms, with pleural effusion and collapse of the ipsilateral lung on chest X-ray and ultrasound examination. Cytology and chemical analysis of the pleural fluid showed an exudative liquid with a chylous aspect, a high concentration of albumin (52 g/dL), triglycerides (11.42 g/L), and a high number of cells (6600 cells/mL), with lymphocyte predominance (96 %). The culture was sterile. Chylothorax is usually revealed by dyspnea, but also by nausea, vomiting, anorexia and/or malnutrition. The diagnosis is suspected when milky white fluid is obtained from thoracocentesis and is confirmed by the presence of a triglyceride level greater than 1.2 mmol/L and more than 1000 cells/mL, with lymphocyte predominance. The treatment of chylothorax can be either conservative or surgical. Conservative treatment (medical) has four goals: ensure pleural emptiness, decrease production of chyle, restore and/or maintain proper nutritional status, and treatment of the cause when identified. Surgical intervention is indicated when conservative management fails and aims to stop a radical and permanent leakage of chyle.


Assuntos
Quilotórax , Quilotórax/diagnóstico , Quilotórax/terapia , Progressão da Doença , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...