Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 30
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Perception ; 29(12): 1437-45, 2000.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11257967

RESUMO

Diaz-Caneja (1928) made some prescient observations about binocular rivalry. Being in French, however, his paper remained largely unknown to the broader research community. His findings are similar to those reported very recently by contemporary researchers who had independently observed similar phenomena. Using concentric circles and parallel lines as stimuli, Diaz-Caneja presented half of each form to opposite eyes to provoke binocular rivalry. He observed periods in the ensuing binocular alternations in which rivalry occurred between the good Gestalt forms, despite the fact that they were distributed between the eyes. He proposed that each half of a good form generates synchronised oscillations in the visual system, and that this synchronisation enables the dichoptically viewed halves of the one form to be perceived as a whole.


Assuntos
Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Animais , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Humanos , Vias Visuais/fisiologia
2.
Vision Res ; 39(16): 2649-57, 1999 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10492827

RESUMO

In a previous study we found that blurred edges presented in peripheral vision look sharper than when they are looked at directly, a phenomenon we have called peripheral sharpness overconstancy (Galvin et al. (1997). Vision Research, 37, 2035-2039). In the current study we show that when visibility of the stimulus edges is compromised by very brief presentations, we can demonstrate sharpness overconstancy for static, foveal viewing. We also test whether the degree of sharpening is a function of the current visual context, but find no difference between the peripheral sharpness overconstancy (at 24 degrees eccentricity) of edges measured in a blurred context and that measured in a sharp context. We conclude that if the visual system does carry a template for sharp edges which contributes to edge appearance when visibility is poor, then that template is resistant to changes in context.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Campos Visuais , Adulto , Sensibilidades de Contraste , Fóvea Central/fisiologia , Humanos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Psicometria , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
3.
Vision Res ; 39(7): 1305-20, 1999 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10343844

RESUMO

Broerse and O'Shea [(1995) Vision Research, 35, 207-226] proposed that the subjective colours in McCollough effects (MEs) consist of two components: edge colours appearing along the edges of contours, and spread colours radiating from edge colours into adjacent uncontoured regions of test patterns. This proposal was examined in five experiments. First, we demonstrated that fine coloured lines located immediately adjacent to the edges of otherwise achromatic square-wave gratings (i.e. colour-fringed gratings) are sufficient to induce MEs comparable in strength to MEs induced with desaturated versions of traditional uniformly-coloured gratings (Experiments 1 & 2). We then quantified edge and spread colours while varying light/dark duty cycles (white-bar width) in gratings with colour-fringed edges (Experiment 3), uniformly-coloured gratings (Experiment 4), and in achromatic gratings tinged with ME colours after adaptation to colour-fringed gratings (Experiment 5). Whereas the perceived magnitude of edge colours remained constant in all cases, spread colours remained constant only for uniformly-coloured gratings. For both MEs and gratings with colour-fringed edges, spread colours decreased as a function of increasing duty cycle, confirming that conventional MEs may be simulated by gratings with colour-fringed edges. We propose that edge colours arise as a consequence of neural operations correcting for the eye's chromatic aberration, while spread colours reveal a neural filling-in process operating to achieve colour constancy. In seeking to implement these suggestions, we present a putative framework based on the receptive-field properties of single cells described in contemporary neurophysiological investigations of colour.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Adaptação Ocular , Pós-Imagem , Animais , Humanos , Ilusões Ópticas/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Distribuição Aleatória , Rotação
4.
Vision Res ; 37(15): 2035-9, 1997 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9327051

RESUMO

Although much has been learned about the spatial sampling and filtering properties of peripheral vision, little attention has been paid to the remarkably clear appearance of the peripheral visual field. To study the apparent sharpness of stimuli presented in the periphery, we presented Gaussian blurred horizontal edges at 8.3, 16.6, 24, 32, and 40 deg eccentricity. Observers adjusted the sharpness of a similar edge, viewed foveally, to match the appearance of the peripheral stimulus. All observers matched blurred peripheral stimuli with sharper foveal stimuli. We have called this effect "sharpness overconstancy". For field sizes of 4 deg, there was greater overconstancy at larger eccentricities. Scaling the field size of the peripheral stimuli by a cortical magnification factor produced sharpness overconstancy which was independent of eccentricity. In both cases, there was a slight sharpness underconstancy for peripherally presented edges blurred only slightly. We consider various explanations of peripheral sharpness overconstancy.


Assuntos
Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Campos Visuais , Adulto , Feminino , Fóvea Central/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Acuidade Visual
5.
Vision Res ; 37(2): 175-83, 1997 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9068818

RESUMO

We measured binocular rivalry between dichoptic, orthogonal, sinusoidal gratings both having spatial frequencies of 0.5, 1, 2, 4, 8 or 16 c deg-1 in fields ranging from 0.5 to 8 deg of visual angle in diameter. Total time that one or the other grating was exclusively visible had an inverted U-shaped relationship with spatial frequency, with the peak shifting to coarser spatial frequencies as the field size increased. We computed for each spatial frequency the maximum field size over which a criterion duration of exclusive visibility would spread. When expressed as areas, these sizes were inversely proportional to spatial frequency. This dependence of rivalry on spatial frequency is similar to those for stereopsis and fusion, consistent with the notion that all three binocular phenomena have a common mechanism.


Assuntos
Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Análise de Regressão
6.
Perception ; 26(5): 599-612, 1997.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9488884

RESUMO

Studies have shown that blur can act as a pictorial cue to depth perception. But blurring a stimulus reduces its contrast, and studies have also shown that contrast can act as a pictorial cue to depth perception. To determine whether blur and contrast have separate influences on depth perception, each variable was independently manipulated in two experiments. Observers reported depth alternations in a simple reversible figure. Both contrast and blur were found to influence depth perception, but blur had its greatest effect at moderate contrasts. When blurred and sharp stimuli were equated on either Michelson or RMS contrast, blur continued to affect depth perception. Hence blur can act as a depth cue independently of contrast. It is speculated that blur is effective as a pictorial cue because of its usual association with other depth cues, particularly in pictures and photographs.


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Distorção da Percepção , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
7.
Vision Res ; 36(11): 1561-71, 1996 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8759459

RESUMO

To test whether the binocular contour rivalry mechanism is tritanopic, we presented isoluminant, rival stimuli visible only via the short-wavelength-sensitive (S) cones. We stimulated only the S cones with violet gratings superimposed on a bright yellow field that adapted the responses of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive (M and L) cones. We found that an S-cone grating presented to one eye rivalled with an orthogonal grating presented to the other. Rivalry persisted over a range of luminances and contrasts of the S-cone stimuli, and was greater than could be accounted for by nonrival fading. The spatial spread of rivalry from S-cone stimuli is similar to that for the same stimuli when visible also to the M and L cones (luminance stimuli). We found that an S-cone stimulus would rival with a luminance stimulus, and exploited this to determine the equivalent luminance contrast of S-cone stimuli by putting them in a rivalry competition with luminance stimuli. For rivalry, the equivalent luminance contrast of isoluminant, S-cone stimuli is much less than their S-cone contrast. The existence of rivalry with isoluminant stimuli, along with earlier evidence that such stimuli can support stereopsis, challenges the view that an achromatic channel alone drives certain higher level functions such as depth perception.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Fotometria , Disparidade Visual
8.
Vision Res ; 36(5): 707-15, 1996 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8762301

RESUMO

Lehmkuhle and Fox [(1976) Vision Research, 16, 428-430] reported that interocular transfer (IOT) of a translational motion aftereffect (MAE) was greater if the non-adapting eye viewed an equiluminant field than if it viewed a dark field. They recommended equiluminant occlusion of the non-adapted eye when measuring IOT of aftereffects. We tested this proposal in three experiments. First, we assessed IOT with equiluminant and dark occlusion for three different classes of aftereffects. Although transfer was greater with equiluminant occlusion for the translational MAE, there was no significant difference in the amount of transfer for the tilt aftereffect or the contrast threshold elevation effect. Second, we tested the hypothesis that spuriously large IOT could be the result of an aftereffect from tracking eye movements in the non-adapting eye. When potential tracking movements were reduced by using rotating spokes, a rotating spiral or contracting concentric circles, there was a corresponding reduction in the occlusion-dependent transfer. Third, we found that luminance shifts had no influence on the amount of transfer when all contours were eliminated from the non-adapting eye. We conclude that the type of occlusion used for measuring IOT of the translational MAE is important only when visible contours in the non-adapting eye contribute to the adapting process.


Assuntos
Adaptação Ocular , Pós-Efeito de Figura/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Movimentos Oculares , Humanos , Percepção de Movimento
9.
Vision Res ; 35(2): 207-26, 1995 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7839617

RESUMO

Dodwell and O'Shea's [(1987) Vision Research, 27, 569-580] conclusions that contingent coloured aftereffects (CAEs) depend on gobal pattern organization were investigated in four experiments. In Expt 1, we replicated findings that CAEs can be induced with complex patterns (concentric circles; radial spokes) under conditions of systematic eye movements. Contrary to Dodwell and O'Shea's argument that eye movements should uniformly cancel local orientation-colour contingencies, leaving only global effects, we reduced CAE magnitude by halving the diameter of the test stimuli. This suggests that cancellation did not occur uniformly over whole patterns, and that CAEs observed on these patterns are the residuals of uncancelled local orientation-colour contingencies. In Expt 2 we used central-fixation induction procedures to demonstrate that it is possible to induce CAEs with randomly-organized and locally-orthogonal orientation components. These findings are inconsistent with Dodwell and O'Shea's failure to observe CAEs under these conditions, and with their conclusion that global organization is necessary for CAE induction. However, CAEs induced with randomly-organized components were significantly weaker than those induced with globally-organized components. We examined the contribution of global organization in two additional experiments. In Expt 3 we induced CAEs with randomly-organized components under conditions in which the need for central fixation was removed, and found that CAE strength was directly related to the organization as well as the density of local-orientation components. In Expt 4, we found that the global organization of local-orientation components enhanced CAE strength only in regions away from the edges of these components: pattern organization did not affect the strength of CAEs at edges. We interpret these findings as evidence that CAEs may involve separate edge- and spread-colour components, and conclude that such components may account for observations previously attributed to global pattern geometry.


Assuntos
Pós-Imagem/fisiologia , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular , Humanos , Rotação
10.
Vision Res ; 34(12): 1595-604, 1994 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7941367

RESUMO

One consequence of aerial perspective is that far objects have lower contrast than near objects. We tested the efficacy of contrast as a cue for depth perception by measuring the apparent, relative depth of two areas that differed in contrast with a background and in size. We tested monocularly and binocularly. Differences in contrast were achieved by making the areas different in luminance, than varying the luminance of the background. Subjects reported that the area having lower contrast with the background appeared farther than the area having higher contrast. Even when size opposed it, contrast continued to have a significant effect on depth perception. Monocular observation yielded larger apparent depth than binocular observation. Contrast is an effective depth cue in the absence of any other depth information. We suggest that contrast acts as a pictorial depth cue simulating the optical effects of aerial perspective.


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Humanos , Luz , Masculino , Psicometria , Espalhamento de Radiação , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Visão Monocular/fisiologia
11.
Perception ; 23(7): 771-84, 1994.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7845769

RESUMO

A study is reported of human binocular rivalry and fusion over a range of luminances from scotopic to photopic. At scotopic light levels, rivalry alternations were very slow and complete. Suppression spread over much larger areas of the visual field than at photopic light levels. As luminances decreased from photopic to scotopic levels there was a rod-cone break for binocular rivalry. Mean suppression durations became abruptly greater as light levels dropped below those allowing the cones to be active. Horizontal disparities allowing fusion were 4 to 6 times greater at scotopic than at photopic light levels. Binocular vision at scotopic luminances was sluggish and of low resolution. It is as though connections to, and within, binocular vision are changed when light levels allow only rod input.


Assuntos
Atenção , Adaptação à Escuridão , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Disparidade Visual , Visão Binocular , Adulto , Atenção/fisiologia , Adaptação à Escuridão/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Psicofísica , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones/fisiologia , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Bastonetes/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia
12.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 35(1): 313-7, 1994 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8300359

RESUMO

PURPOSE: To compare binocularity in central and peripheral vision of people with early-onset strabismus and people with normal binocular vision. METHODS: Ten subjects with early-onset strabismus, and nine subjects with normal binocular vision were tested. To assess binocularity, interocular transfer (IOT) of a rotary movement aftereffect (MAE) was measured. The MAE stimuli were either confined to the central 2.8 degrees of the visual field or were presented 10 degrees into peripheral vision. RESULTS: In peripheral vision, there was no significant difference in IOT for the two groups of subjects. In central vision, there was a significant decrease of IOT in subjects with early-onset strabismus. Their IOT was, however, significantly greater than zero. CONCLUSIONS: Early-onset strabismus appears to spare binocularity in peripheral vision but reduces it in central vision. It does not abolish binocularity assessed by IOT of MAE, suggesting that some binocular connections survive early-onset strabismus, even in central vision.


Assuntos
Pós-Imagem/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Estrabismo/fisiopatologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Luz , Masculino , Movimento , Retina/fisiologia
13.
Vis Neurosci ; 8(5): 469-78, 1992 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1586647

RESUMO

This paper presents results from psychophysical experiments on human binocular rivalry in central and peripheral vision. Results show that the incidence of periods of exclusive visibility of a given eye's rival target increased with decreasing target size, and for a given sized target exclusive visibility increased with retinal eccentricity. Control measures confirmed that these results were not attributable solely to reduced peripheral acuity, to Troxler's effect, or to spatial frequency. We computed the minimum-sized stimulus that would lead to a criterion level of exclusive visibility of one or the other eye; this we term the spatial zone of binocular rivalry. The change in estimated size of spatial zones of rivalry with eccentricity compares favorably with estimates of human cortical magnification. We propose a model that assumes concentrically organized zones of rivalry. These zones do not function independently, but instead exhibit a high degree of mutual excitatory cooperativity. The model has multiple solutions for the foveal zone size, but the best fits predict a diameter of 5.3 or 7.3 min of visual angle; these values dovetail nicely with our empirical estimates of the foveal zone size.


Assuntos
Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Humanos , Matemática , Modelos Neurológicos , Psicofísica , Córtex Visual/fisiologia
14.
Perception ; 20(3): 415-8, 1991.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1762884

RESUMO

Measures of the size of various hand parts and their viewing distance when held at arm's length were made on one hundred and eighteen undergraduate students. A simple rule of thumb can be confirmed: Visual angle of the width of the thumb held at arm's length is about 2 deg. The thumbnail subtends about 1.5 deg and the index fingernail about 1 deg in width, when both are held at arm's length. These figures are good approximations for males and females, although a significant, direct, linear relationship exists between hand-part size and the visual angle of the part at arm's length.


Assuntos
Percepção de Distância/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Percepção de Tamanho/fisiologia , Polegar/anatomia & histologia , Adulto , Antropometria , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica
15.
Perception ; 19(2): 207-21, 1990.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2235288

RESUMO

Vernier acuity has usually been tested with stimuli of the same contrast polarity (SC). This traditional vernier acuity was compared to that obtained with stimuli of opposite-contrast (OC) in which one target was brighter than the background and the other was darker. For both bar and dot targets vernier acuity with OC stimuli was about half as good as with SC stimuli. There were large individual differences in the size of the disadvantage with OC stimuli, although thresholds remained within the hyperacuity range. There were also individually-differing biases to see a dark vernier stimulus on one or the other side of a bright stimulus. Differences between OC and SC vernier acuities persisted over a wide range of interstimulus spacings, widths, and contrasts. At extremes of these spatial manipulations acuities became similar, but only because SC acuities were degraded to the level of OC acuities. Subjects showed little improvement in OC vernier acuity, even after 50,000 trials. It is concluded that finest judgements of spatial position arise in a level of the visual system at which light and dark stimuli are treated independently.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Orientação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Acuidade Visual , Humanos , Luz , Psicofísica , Limiar Sensorial
18.
Vision Res ; 27(4): 569-80, 1987.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3660619

RESUMO

It has recently been demonstrated that certain globally orthogonal pattern pairs derived from Hoffman's Lie Transformation Group Model of Neuropsychology (LTG/NP) are sufficient to generate McCollough effects (pattern-contingent colour aftereffects: PCCAES). We now provide evidence that the global factors postulated to generate these particular PCCAEs are also necessary. In Experiment 1 observers made controlled eye movements over the induction patterns and subsequently showed PCCAEs which were as strong as those obtained under central fixation. The induction process therefore did not depend on selectively adapting local oriented and colour-specific edge detectors. The global structure of the inducing patterns appears to be a necessary component of the generation of such aftereffects. In Experiment 2, a number of pattern pairs having various degrees of global organization were used as induction patterns, but in every case there was an equal degree of local orthogonality. Local orthogonality was shown not to be sufficient to induce PCCAEs, but global orthogonality was. These results lend strong support to the notion that McCollough-type PCCAEs are generated in a structure of the visual system somewhat above the level of strictly local operations, whether defined as edge and bar detectors or, equivalently, as units selectively tuned to different spatial frequencies and orientations. The global nature of the effects, and the particular pattern structures involved, lead us to suggest that Hoffman's LTG/NP provides a sound basis for explaining our findings.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Pós-Efeito de Figura/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Fixação Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Rotação
19.
Vision Res ; 27(5): 781-91, 1987.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3660639

RESUMO

Suppression theory attributes the singleness of vision during binocular viewing to inconspicuous binocular rivalry. In two experiments, reaction time (RTs) for detection of a small molecular probe were measured while the eyes viewed identical ("fusion") or different (rivalry) stimuli. As expected, distributions of RTs obtained during binocular rivalry showed large positive skew, from trials in which detection was delayed because the probe was superimposed on the suppressed field. Opposite to the prediction of suppression theory, however, the RT distribution during fusion showed far less positive skew, implying that information is available from both eyes during binocular viewing, as held by fusion theory. These findings were confirmed and extended over a large range of probe luminances. During fusion, log mean RTs fell steeply as log probe luminance was increased up to a critical value, then less steeply for further increases. During rivalry, the same steep branch of the RT-luminance function appeared, but shifted as though the probe was about 0.25 log units dimmer. The second branch was also present, but steeper than for fusion, so that RTs for fusion and rivalry were the same at the highest values tested.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular , Humanos , Luz , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação , Rotação , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Vias Visuais
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...