Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 13 de 13
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 85(8): 2821-2833, 2023 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37731085

RESUMO

It is a familiar but challenging task to manually transfer a liquid-filled container without spilling. The action requires stringent control because the dynamics of interacting with the non-rigid aqueous content is complex. In this work, we sought to discover what properties of a liquid-filled container were predictive of transfer without spilling performance. Two candidate variables were tested (Experiment 1): The distance between liquid surface and the container's rim (h) and the container's diameter (d). Participants attempted to transfer 15 containers (3 ds and 5 hs), one at a time and as fast as possible, without spilling. Kinematic analyses showed that the movement's peak velocity and the first peak acceleration were affected by h; the movement time and the frequency of acceleration change were affected by h and d in a hierarchical manner, where transfer without spilling was first affected by h and for full containers, the thick ones were moved more slowly and went through more acceleration change; for not so full containers, the container's diameter did not have any effect. Next, each of the 15 containers was compared with the other 14, and participants judged from a pair of displayed containers which one was more likely to be moved fast without spilling (Experiment 2). Perceived affordance was affected by h and d but not by whether containers were placed upright or tilted. In general, thinner and less full containers were judged as easier to be moved fast without spilling.

2.
J Vis ; 22(10): 13, 2022 09 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36107125

RESUMO

Traditional visual search tasks in the laboratories typically involve looking for targets in 2D displays with exemplar views of objects. In real life, visual search commonly entails 3D objects in 3D spaces with nonperpendicular viewing and relative motions between observers and search array items, both of which lead to transformations of objects' projected images in lawful but unpredicted ways. Furthermore, observers often do not have to memorize a target before searching, but may refer to it while searching, for example, holding a picture of someone while looking for them from a crowd. Extending the traditional visual search task, in this study, we investigated the effects of image transformation as a result of perspective change yielded by discrete viewing angle change (Experiment 1) or continuous rotation of the search array (Experiment 2) and of having external references on visual search performance. Results showed that when searching from 3D objects with a non-zero viewing angle, performance was similar to searching from 2D exemplar views of objects; when searching for 3D targets from rotating arrays in virtual reality, performance was similar to searching from stationary arrays. In general, discrete or continuous perspective change did not affect the search outcomes in terms of accuracy, response time, and self-rated confidence, or the search process in terms of eye movement patterns. Therefore, visual search does not require the exact match of retinal images. Additionally, being able to see the target during the search improved search accuracy and observers' confidence. It increased search time because, as revealed by the eye movements, observers actively checked back on the reference target. Thus, visual search is an embodied process that involves real-time information exchange between the observers and the environment.


Assuntos
Atenção , Percepção Visual , Atenção/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares , Humanos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
3.
Optom Vis Sci ; 99(5): 470-478, 2022 05 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35149634

RESUMO

SIGNIFICANCE: Using static depth information, normal observers monocularly perceived equidistance with high accuracy. With dynamic depth information and/or monocular viewing experience, they perceived with high precision. Therefore, monocular patients, who were adapted to monocular viewing, should be able to perceive equidistance and perform related tasks. PURPOSE: This study investigated whether normal observers could accurately and precisely perceive equidistance with one eye, in different viewing environments, with various optical information and monocular viewing experience. METHODS: Sixteen normally sighted observers monocularly perceived the distance (5 to 30 m) between a target and the self and replicated it either in some hallways that contained ample static monocular depth information but had a limited field of view or on a lawn that contained less depth information but had a large field of view. Participants remained stationary or walked 5 m before performing the task, as a manipulation of the availability of dynamic depth information. Eight observers wore eye patches for 3 hours before the experiment and gained monocular viewing experience, whereas the others did not. Both accuracy and precision were measured. RESULTS: As long as static monocular depth information was available, equidistance perception was effectively accurate, despite minute underestimation. Perception precision was improved by prior monocular walking and/or experience with monocularity. Accuracy and precision were not affected by the viewing environments. CONCLUSIONS: Using static and dynamic monocular depth information and/or with monocular experience, normal observers judged equidistance with reliable accuracy and precision. This implied that patients with monocular vision, who are better adapted than participants of this study, should also be able to perceive equidistance and perform distance-dependent tasks in natural viewing environments.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade , Percepção de Movimento , Humanos , Visão Binocular , Visão Monocular
4.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 83(1): 389-398, 2021 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33000441

RESUMO

Information used in visual event perception includes both static image structure projected from opaque object surfaces and dynamic optic flow generated by motion. Events presented in static blurry grayscale displays have been shown to be recognized only when and after presented with optic flow. In this study, we investigate the effects of optic flow and color on identifying blurry events by studying the identification accuracy and eye-movement patterns. Three types of color displays were tested: grayscale, original colors, or rearranged colors (where the RGB values of the original colors were adjusted). In each color condition, participants identified 12 blurry events in five experimental phases. In the first two phases, static blurry images were presented alone or sequentially with a motion mask between consecutive frames, and identification was poor. In Phase 3, where optic flow was added, identification was comparably good. In Phases 4 and 5, motion was removed, but identification remained good. Thus, optic flow improved event identification during and after its presentation. Color also improved performance, where participants were consistently better at identifying color displays than grayscale or rearranged color displays. Importantly, the effects of optic flow and color were additive. Finally, in both motion and postmotion phases, a significant portion of eye fixations fell in strong optic flow areas, suggesting that participants continued to look where flow was available even after it stopped. We infer that optic flow specified depth structure in the blurry image structure and yielded an improvement in identification from static blurry images.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento , Fluxo Óptico , Cor , Movimentos Oculares , Humanos , Percepção Visual
5.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 61(8): 6, 2020 07 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32634203

RESUMO

Purpose: This study identifies and characterizes the nasotemporal hemifield difference of interocular suppression in subjects who have been successfully treated for strabismus. Methods: Interocular suppression in the nasal and temporal hemifields were measured using two methods, namely, binocular phase combination and dichoptic motion coherence, both entailed suprathreshold stimuli. We tested 29 clinical subjects, who had strabismus (19 with exotropia and 10 with esotropia) but regained good ocular alignment (within 10 prism diopters) after surgical or refractive correction, and 10 control subjects. Results: Both the hemifield binocular phase combination and the hemifield dichoptic motion coherence tests revealed similar nasotemporal asymmetry of interocular suppression. Subjects with previous exotropia showed deeper suppression in the nasal hemifield, and those with previous esotropia showed deeper suppression in the temporal hemifield. This finding was consistent with the hemifield suppression theory. Furthermore, there was deeper suppression but less imbalance of nasotemporal asymmetry in the hemifield dichoptic motion coherence test. Finally, clinical stereopsis and the nasotemporal asymmetry of suppression (P < 0.05 in both tests) were negatively correlated in subjects with previous exotropia and measurable stereopsis. Conclusions: Hemifield asymmetry of interocular suppression in corrected strabismus can be measured by using static and dynamic suprathreshold stimuli. Thus, the evaluation of binocular vision in strabismus should focus on both the magnitude and the pattern of interocular suppression.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiopatologia , Procedimentos Cirúrgicos Oftalmológicos/métodos , Estrabismo/fisiopatologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Músculos Oculomotores/cirurgia , Estimulação Luminosa , Período Pós-Operatório , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Estrabismo/cirurgia
6.
Front Psychol ; 10: 2205, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31611833

RESUMO

Throwing is an important motor skill for human survival and societal development. It has been shown that throwers could select throwable balls for themselves and ball throwability was determined by its size and weight. In this study, we investigated whether throwers could perceive ball throwability for other throwers (experimental confederates) and whether the perceived throwability for others also followed a size-weight relation. Like other types of affordances, throwability entails a scaling between the thrower and the throwing object. This requires knowledge about the thrower and the object. In this study, knowledge about the objects was gained by hefting balls of various sizes and weights; knowledge about the throwers was gained by interacting with throwers in person (Experiment 1) and by viewing videos of confederates throwing (containing kinematic and anthropometric information) or photographs of the confederates standing (containing anthropometric information; Experiment 2). By comparing observers' perceived throwability for others using various materials, we attempted to uncover whether scaling of throwability was based on kinematic or anthropometric information. In this study, participants ranked throwability of balls of various sizes and weights for confederates of different sexes and fitness levels. In all experimental conditions, observers' ranking and confederates' actual throwing performances yielded linear relationships with slopes close to 1 and moderate to high r 2 values. These suggested that participants were able to accurately perceive throwability and choose throwable balls for the confederates. The throwable balls followed a size-weight relation, where bigger balls had to weigh more to be perceived as throwable as smaller balls. Furthermore, there was no difference between throwability perception based on in-person interaction, watching videos of confederates throwing and seeing pictures of the confederates standing. This suggested that the scaling of throwability was likely to be based on anthropometric information. These results enriched our understanding of whether one could perceive the action opportunities for others, and extended the canonical Gibsonian concept of affordance to a social setting and thus could be important for understanding team coordination in sports and interpersonal action collaboration in general.

7.
Vision Res ; 158: 49-57, 2019 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30796993

RESUMO

Perceiving the spatial layout of objects is crucial in visual scene perception. Optic flow provides information about spatial layout. This information is not affected by image blur because motion detection uses low spatial frequencies in image structure. Therefore, perceiving scenes with blurry vision should be effective when optic flow is available. Furthermore, when blurry images and optic flow interact, optic flow specifies spatial relations and calibrates blurry images. Calibrated image structure then preserves spatial relations specified by optic flow after motion stops. Thus, perceiving blurry scenes should be stable when optic flow and blurry images are available. We investigated the types of optic flow that facilitate recognition of blurry scenes and evaluated the stability of performance. Participants identified scenes in blurry videos when viewing single frames and the entire videos that contained translational flow (Experiment 1), rotational flow (Experiment 2) or both (Experiment 3). When first viewing the blurry images, participants identified a few scenes. When viewing blurry video clips, their performance improved with translational flow, whether it was available alone or in combination with rotational flow. Participants were still able to perceive scenes from static blurry images one week later. Therefore, translational flow interacts with blurry image structures to yield effective and stable scene perception. These results imply that observers with blurry vision may be able to identify their surrounds when they locomote.


Assuntos
Fluxo Óptico/fisiologia , Baixa Visão/fisiopatologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
8.
J Vis ; 17(12): 13, 2017 10 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29067401

RESUMO

Events consist of objects in motion. When objects move, their opaque surfaces reflect light and produce both static image structure and dynamic optic flow. The static and dynamic optical information co-specify events. Patients with age-related macular degeneration (AMD) and amblyopia cannot identify static objects because of weakened image structure. However, optic flow is detectable despite blurry vision because visual motion measurement uses low spatial frequencies. When motion ceases, image structure persists and might preserve properties specified by optic flow. We tested whether optic flow and image structure interact to allow event perception with poor static vision. AMD (Experiment 1), amblyopic (Experiments 2 and 3), and normally sighted observers identified common events from either blurry (Experiments 1 and 2) or clear images (Experiment 3), when either single image frames were presented, a sequence of frames was presented with motion masks, or a sequence of frames was presented with detectable motion. Results showed that with static images, but no motion, events were not perceived well by participants other than controls in Experiment 3. However, with detectable motion, events were perceived. Immediately following this and again after five days, participants were able to identify events from the original static images. So, when image structure information is weak, optic flow compensates for it and enables event perception. Furthermore, weakened static image structure information nevertheless preserves information that was once available in optic flow. The combination is powerful and allows events to be perceived accurately and stably despite blurry vision.


Assuntos
Ambliopia/fisiopatologia , Degeneração Macular/fisiopatologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Fluxo Óptico/fisiologia , Baixa Visão/fisiopatologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
9.
Appl Opt ; 56(22): 6410-6418, 2017 Aug 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29047842

RESUMO

Use of motion to break camouflage extends back to the Cambrian [In the Blink of an Eye: How Vision Sparked the Big Bang of Evolution (New York Basic Books, 2003)]. We investigated the ability to break camouflage and continue to see camouflaged targets after motion stops. This is crucial for the survival of hunting predators. With camouflage, visual targets and distracters cannot be distinguished using only static image structure (i.e., appearance). Motion generates another source of optical information, optic flow, which breaks camouflage and specifies target locations. Optic flow calibrates image structure with respect to spatial relations among targets and distracters, and calibrated image structure makes previously camouflaged targets perceptible in a temporally stable fashion after motion stops. We investigated this proposal using laboratory experiments and compared how many camouflaged targets were identified either with optic flow information alone or with combined optic flow and image structure information. Our results show that the combination of motion-generated optic flow and target-projected image structure information yielded efficient and stable perception of camouflaged targets.

10.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 43(7): 1343-1358, 2017 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28301185

RESUMO

Rotating a scene in a frontoparallel plane (rolling) yields a change in orientation of constituent images. When using only information provided by static images to perceive a scene after orientation change, identification performance typically decreases (Rock & Heimer, 1957). However, rolling generates optic flow information that relates the discrete, static images (before and after the change) and forms an embodied memory that aids recognition. The embodied memory hypothesis predicts that upon detecting a continuous spatial transformation of image structure, or in other words, seeing the continuous rolling process and objects undergoing rolling observers should accurately perceive objects during and after motion. Thus, in this case, orientation change should not affect performance. We tested this hypothesis in three experiments and found that (a) using combined optic flow and image structure, participants identified locations of previously perceived but currently occluded targets with great accuracy and stability (Experiment 1); (b) using combined optic flow and image structure information, participants identified hidden targets equally well with or without 30° orientation changes (Experiment 2); and (c) when the rolling was unseen, identification of hidden targets after orientation change became worse (Experiment 3). Furthermore, when rolling was unseen, although target identification was better when participants were told about the orientation change than when they were not told, performance was still worse than when there was no orientation change. Therefore, combined optic flow and image structure information, not mere knowledge about the rolling, enables accurate and stable perception despite orientation change. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Memória/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
11.
Physiol Behav ; 153: 125-32, 2016 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26524512

RESUMO

RATIONALE: Clinical studies have shown that patients with exaggerated risk-taking tendencies have high baseline levels of norepinephrine. In this work, we systemically manipulated norepinephrine levels in rats and studied their behavioral changes in a probabilistic discounting task, which is a paradigm for gauging risk taking. METHODS: This study aims to explore the effects of the selective norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor (atomoxetine at doses of 0.6, 1.0 and 1.8 mg/kg), and receptor selective antagonists (propranolol at a single dose of 1.0/kg, and prazosin at a single dose of 0.1 mg/kg), on risk taking using a probabilistic discounting task. In this task, there were two levers available to rats: pressing the 'small/certain' lever guaranteed a single food pellet, and pressing the 'large/risky' lever yielded either four pellets or none. The probability of receiving four food pellets decreased across the four experimental blocks from 100% to 12.5%. RESULTS: Atomoxetine increased the tendency to choose the large/risky lever. It significantly reduced the lose-shift effect (i.e. pressing a different lever after losing a trial), but did not affect the win-stay effect (i.e. pressing the same lever after winning a trial). Furthermore, co-administration of beta-adrenoreceptor antagonist, propranolol, eliminated the effects of atomoxetine on risk taking and the lose-shift effect; but co-administration of alpha1-adrenoreceptor antagonist, prazosin, did not. CONCLUSIONS: Atomoxetine boosted NE levels and increased risk taking. This was because atomoxetine decreased rats' sensitivity to losses. These effects were likely mediated by beta-adrenoreceptor.


Assuntos
Antagonistas Adrenérgicos beta/farmacologia , Cloridrato de Atomoxetina/antagonistas & inibidores , Cloridrato de Atomoxetina/farmacologia , Assunção de Riscos , Antagonistas Adrenérgicos beta/administração & dosagem , Animais , Cloridrato de Atomoxetina/administração & dosagem , Comportamento de Escolha/efeitos dos fármacos , Condicionamento Operante/efeitos dos fármacos , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Masculino , Prazosina/administração & dosagem , Prazosina/farmacologia , Propranolol/administração & dosagem , Propranolol/farmacologia , Ratos
12.
Optom Vis Sci ; 90(10): 1119-27, 2013 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24067408

RESUMO

PURPOSE: From static blurry images, it is difficult to perceive objects because high spatial frequency details are filtered out. However, in the context of events (defined as objects in motion), motion generates optic flow, which provides a depth map of 3D layout and allows good event perception. Visual motion measurement uses low spatial frequencies that remain available in blurry images, making events perceivable. Optic flow and image structure are intrinsically related in vision because optic flow takes one image to the next. Optic flow is powerful in specifying depth structures and it calibrates the degraded image structure; image structure is persistent and it preserves events perceived with ongoing motion, after it stops. Might optic flow and image structures interact and allow events to be perceived despite poor quality images? The answer to this question has implications for event perception with low vision. METHODS: Twenty blurry images depicting each of eight daily events were used as stimuli. Ten normally sighted participants perceived the stimuli and described the events in five ordered conditions: (1) when single frames were presented, (2) when all frames were presented with motion masks, (3) when all frames were presented without motion masks, (4) when single frames were presented, and (5) when single frames were presented 5 days later. RESULTS: With blurry static images alone, participants were unable to identify events. Events were perceptible when the blurred images were played in sequence, making motion-generated information available. Subsequently, when given the original blurry static images again, post-motion performance was vastly superior to the pre-motion performance. Furthermore, the high rate of recognition persisted after 5 days. CONCLUSIONS: Optic flow calibrates low-quality image structure to allow accurate event perception during and after motion. This implies that low-vision observers should perform much better than allowed by weakened image structure information alone.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Fluxo Óptico/fisiologia , Baixa Visão/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
13.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 39(6): 1638-51, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23477699

RESUMO

Visual perception studies typically focus either on optic flow structure or image structure, but not on the combination and interaction of these two sources of information. Each offers unique strengths in contrast to the other's weaknesses. Optic flow yields intrinsically powerful information about 3D structure, but is ephemeral. It ceases when motion stops. Image structure is less powerful in specifying 3D structure, but is stable. It remains when motion stops. Optic flow and image structure are intrinsically related in vision because the optic flow carries one image to the next. This relation is especially important in the context of progressive occlusion, in which optic flow provides information about the location of targets hidden in subsequent image structure. In four experiments, we investigated the role of image structure in "embodied memory" in contrast to memory that is only in the head. We found that either optic flow (Experiment 1) or image structure (Experiment 2) alone were relatively ineffective, whereas the combination was effective and, in contrast to conditions requiring reliance on memory-in-the-head, much more stable over extended time (Experiments 2 through 4). Limits well documented for visual short memory (that is, memory-in-the-head) were strongly exceeded by embodied memory. The findings support J. J. Gibson's (1979/1986, The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception, Boston, MA, Houghton Mifflin) insights about progressive occlusion and the embodied nature of perception and memory.


Assuntos
Memória/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Fluxo Óptico/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...