Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 41
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(48): e2301642120, 2023 Nov 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37983511

RESUMO

Science is among humanity's greatest achievements, yet scientific censorship is rarely studied empirically. We explore the social, psychological, and institutional causes and consequences of scientific censorship (defined as actions aimed at obstructing particular scientific ideas from reaching an audience for reasons other than low scientific quality). Popular narratives suggest that scientific censorship is driven by authoritarian officials with dark motives, such as dogmatism and intolerance. Our analysis suggests that scientific censorship is often driven by scientists, who are primarily motivated by self-protection, benevolence toward peer scholars, and prosocial concerns for the well-being of human social groups. This perspective helps explain both recent findings on scientific censorship and recent changes to scientific institutions, such as the use of harm-based criteria to evaluate research. We discuss unknowns surrounding the consequences of censorship and provide recommendations for improving transparency and accountability in scientific decision-making to enable the exploration of these unknowns. The benefits of censorship may sometimes outweigh costs. However, until costs and benefits are examined empirically, scholars on opposing sides of ongoing debates are left to quarrel based on competing values, assumptions, and intuitions.


Assuntos
Censura Científica , Ciência , Responsabilidade Social , Custos e Análise de Custo
2.
Behav Brain Sci ; 44: e109, 2021 09 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34588049

RESUMO

This article is extraordinarily rigorous and rich, although there are reasons to be skeptical of its theory that music originated to signal group quality and infant solicitude. These include the lack of any signature of the centrality of these functions in the distribution or experience of music; of a role for the pleasure taken in music; and of its connections with language.


Assuntos
Música , Preparações Farmacêuticas , Humanos , Lactente
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(45): 27767-27776, 2020 11 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33093198

RESUMO

Humans and viruses have been coevolving for millennia. Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2, the virus that causes COVID-19) has been particularly successful in evading our evolved defenses. The outcome has been tragic-across the globe, millions have been sickened and hundreds of thousands have died. Moreover, the quarantine has radically changed the structure of our lives, with devastating social and economic consequences that are likely to unfold for years. An evolutionary perspective can help us understand the progression and consequences of the pandemic. Here, a diverse group of scientists, with expertise from evolutionary medicine to cultural evolution, provide insights about the pandemic and its aftermath. At the most granular level, we consider how viruses might affect social behavior, and how quarantine, ironically, could make us susceptible to other maladies, due to a lack of microbial exposure. At the psychological level, we describe the ways in which the pandemic can affect mating behavior, cooperation (or the lack thereof), and gender norms, and how we can use disgust to better activate native "behavioral immunity" to combat disease spread. At the cultural level, we describe shifting cultural norms and how we might harness them to better combat disease and the negative social consequences of the pandemic. These insights can be used to craft solutions to problems produced by the pandemic and to lay the groundwork for a scientific agenda to capture and understand what has become, in effect, a worldwide social experiment.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , COVID-19/psicologia , Características Humanas , Pandemias/ética , Comportamento Social , COVID-19/epidemiologia , COVID-19/prevenção & controle , Demografia/tendências , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pandemias/estatística & dados numéricos , Distanciamento Físico
4.
Science ; 366(6468)2019 11 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31753969

RESUMO

What is universal about music, and what varies? We built a corpus of ethnographic text on musical behavior from a representative sample of the world's societies, as well as a discography of audio recordings. The ethnographic corpus reveals that music (including songs with words) appears in every society observed; that music varies along three dimensions (formality, arousal, religiosity), more within societies than across them; and that music is associated with certain behavioral contexts such as infant care, healing, dance, and love. The discography-analyzed through machine summaries, amateur and expert listener ratings, and manual transcriptions-reveals that acoustic features of songs predict their primary behavioral context; that tonality is widespread, perhaps universal; that music varies in rhythmic and melodic complexity; and that elements of melodies and rhythms found worldwide follow power laws.


Assuntos
Antropologia Cultural , Música , Canto , Percepção Auditiva , Comportamento , Comparação Transcultural , Dança , Humanos , Cuidado do Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Amor , Psicoacústica , Religião
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 116(28): 13751-13758, 2019 07 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31253709

RESUMO

People often coordinate for mutual gain, such as keeping to opposite sides of a stairway, dubbing an object or place with a name, or assembling en masse to protest a regime. Because successful coordination requires complementary choices, these opportunities raise the puzzle of how people attain the common knowledge that facilitates coordination, in which a person knows X, knows that the other knows X, knows that the other knows that he knows, ad infinitum. We show that people are highly sensitive to the distinction between common knowledge and mere private or shared knowledge, and that they deploy this distinction strategically in diverse social situations that have the structure of coordination games, including market cooperation, innuendo, bystander intervention, attributions of charitability, self-conscious emotions, and moral condemnation.


Assuntos
Efeito Espectador/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Percepção Social , Teoria da Mente , Comportamento Cooperativo , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Conhecimento , Masculino , Mentalização
6.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 148(1): 158-173, 2019 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30335447

RESUMO

Why do people esteem anonymous charitable giving? We connect normative theories of charitability (captured in Maimonides' Ladder of Charity) with evolutionary theories of partner choice to test predictions on how attributions of charitability are affected by states of knowledge: whether the identity of the donor or of the beneficiary is revealed to the other. Consistent with the theories, in Experiments 1-2 participants judged a double-blind gift as more charitable than one to a revealed beneficiary, which in turn was judged as more charitable than one from a revealed donor. We also found one exception: Participants judged a donor who revealed only himself as slightly less, rather than more, charitable than one who revealed both identities. Experiment 3 explains the exception as a reaction to the donor's perceived sense of superiority and disinterest in a social relationship. Experiment 4 found that donors were judged as more charitable when the gift was shared knowledge (each aware of the other's identity, but unsure of the other's awareness) than when it was common knowledge (awareness of awareness). Experiment 5, which titrated anonymity against donation size, found that not even a hundredfold larger gift could compensate for the disapproval elicited by a donor revealing his identity. Experiment 6 showed that participants' judgments of charitability flip depending on whose perspective they take: Observers disapprove of donations that they would prefer as beneficiaries. Together, these experiments provide insight into why people care about how a donor gives, not just how much. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Altruísmo , Instituições de Caridade , Comportamento de Escolha , Percepção Social , Adulto , Humanos
7.
Cognition ; 177: 263-277, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29729947

RESUMO

Children learn language more easily than adults, though when and why this ability declines have been obscure for both empirical reasons (underpowered studies) and conceptual reasons (measuring the ultimate attainment of learners who started at different ages cannot by itself reveal changes in underlying learning ability). We address both limitations with a dataset of unprecedented size (669,498 native and non-native English speakers) and a computational model that estimates the trajectory of underlying learning ability by disentangling current age, age at first exposure, and years of experience. This allows us to provide the first direct estimate of how grammar-learning ability changes with age, finding that it is preserved almost to the crux of adulthood (17.4 years old) and then declines steadily. This finding held not only for "difficult" syntactic phenomena but also for "easy" syntactic phenomena that are normally mastered early in acquisition. The results support the existence of a sharply-defined critical period for language acquisition, but the age of offset is much later than previously speculated. The size of the dataset also provides novel insight into several other outstanding questions in language acquisition.


Assuntos
Período Crítico Psicológico , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Aprendizagem , Multilinguismo , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Criança , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
8.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 43(8): 1173-1182, 2017 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28150958

RESUMO

What is the relationship between the language people use to describe an event and their moral judgments? We test the hypothesis that moral judgment and causative verbs rely on the same underlying mental model of people's actions. Experiment 1a finds that participants choose different verbs to describe the major variants of a moral dilemma, the trolley problem, mirroring differences in their wrongness judgments: they described direct harm with a single causative verb (Adam killed the man), and indirect harm with an intransitive verb in a periphrastic construction (Adam caused the man to die). Experiments 1b and 2 separate physical causality from moral valuation by varying whether the victim is a person or animal and whether the harmful action rescues people or inanimate objects. The results show that people's moral judgments lead them to portray a causal event as either more or less direct and intended, which in turn shapes their verb choices. Experiment 3 finds the same basic asymmetry in verb usage in a production task in which participants freely described what happened. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Julgamento , Princípios Morais , Psicolinguística , Adulto , Comportamento de Escolha , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Psicológicos
9.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 145(5): 621-629, 2016 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26913616

RESUMO

The more potential helpers there are, the less likely any individual is to help. A traditional explanation for this bystander effect is that responsibility diffuses across the multiple bystanders, diluting the responsibility of each. We investigate an alternative, which combines the volunteer's dilemma (each bystander is best off if another responds) with recursive theory of mind (each infers what the others know about what he knows) to predict that actors will strategically shirk when they think others feel compelled to help. In 3 experiments, participants responded to a (fictional) person who needed help from at least 1 volunteer. Participants were in groups of 2 or 5 and had varying information about whether other group members knew that help was needed. As predicted, people's decision to help zigzagged with the depth of their asymmetric, recursive knowledge (e.g., "John knows that Michael knows that John knows help is needed"), and replicated the classic bystander effect when they had common knowledge (everyone knowing what everyone knows). The results demonstrate that the bystander effect may result not from a mere diffusion of responsibility but specifically from actors' strategic computations.


Assuntos
Efeito Espectador/fisiologia , Comportamento Social , Teoria da Mente/fisiologia , Adulto , Emoções , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
11.
Behav Brain Sci ; 38: e154, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26786231

RESUMO

Political bias has indeed been a distorter of psychology, not just in particular research areas but in an aversion to the explanatory depth available from politically fraught fields like evolution. I add two friendly amendments to the target article: (1) The leftist moral narrative may be based on zero-sum competition among identity groups rather than continuous progress; and (2) ideological bias should be dealt with not just via diversity of ideological factions but by minimizing the influence of ideology altogether.


Assuntos
Narração , Política , Viés
12.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(52): E5616-22, 2014 Dec 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25512502

RESUMO

Languages vary enormously in global importance because of historical, demographic, political, and technological forces. However, beyond simple measures of population and economic power, there has been no rigorous quantitative way to define the global influence of languages. Here we use the structure of the networks connecting multilingual speakers and translated texts, as expressed in book translations, multiple language editions of Wikipedia, and Twitter, to provide a concept of language importance that goes beyond simple economic or demographic measures. We find that the structure of these three global language networks (GLNs) is centered on English as a global hub and around a handful of intermediate hub languages, which include Spanish, German, French, Russian, Portuguese, and Chinese. We validate the measure of a language's centrality in the three GLNs by showing that it exhibits a strong correlation with two independent measures of the number of famous people born in the countries associated with that language. These results suggest that the position of a language in the GLN contributes to the visibility of its speakers and the global popularity of the cultural content they produce.


Assuntos
Bases de Dados Factuais , Internet , Modelos Teóricos , Tradução , Humanos
13.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(38): 13790-4, 2014 Sep 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25201988

RESUMO

We identify common genetic variants associated with cognitive performance using a two-stage approach, which we call the proxy-phenotype method. First, we conduct a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in a large sample (n = 106,736), which produces a set of 69 education-associated SNPs. Second, using independent samples (n = 24,189), we measure the association of these education-associated SNPs with cognitive performance. Three SNPs (rs1487441, rs7923609, and rs2721173) are significantly associated with cognitive performance after correction for multiple hypothesis testing. In an independent sample of older Americans (n = 8,652), we also show that a polygenic score derived from the education-associated SNPs is associated with memory and absence of dementia. Convergent evidence from a set of bioinformatics analyses implicates four specific genes (KNCMA1, NRXN1, POU2F3, and SCRT). All of these genes are associated with a particular neurotransmitter pathway involved in synaptic plasticity, the main cellular mechanism for learning and memory.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Herança Multifatorial/fisiologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/genética , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único , Transmissão Sináptica/genética , Proteínas de Ligação ao Cálcio , Moléculas de Adesão Celular Neuronais/genética , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória/fisiologia , Proteínas do Tecido Nervoso/genética , Moléculas de Adesão de Célula Nervosa , Fatores de Transcrição de Octâmero/genética
14.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 107(4): 657-76, 2014 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25111301

RESUMO

Research on human cooperation has concentrated on the puzzle of altruism, in which 1 actor incurs a cost to benefit another, and the psychology of reciprocity, which evolved to solve this problem. We examine the complementary puzzle of mutualism, in which actors can benefit each other simultaneously, and the psychology of coordination, which ensures such benefits. Coordination is facilitated by common knowledge: the recursive belief state in which A knows X, B knows X, A knows that B knows X, B knows that A knows X, ad infinitum. We test whether people are sensitive to common knowledge when deciding whether to engage in risky coordination. Participants decided between working alone for a certain profit and working together for a potentially higher profit that they would receive only if their partner made the same choice. Results showed that more participants attempted risky coordination when they and their prospective partner had common knowledge of the payoffs (broadcast over a loudspeaker) than when they had only shared knowledge (conveyed to both by a messenger) or private knowledge (revealed to each partner separately). These results support the hypothesis that people represent common knowledge as a distinct cognitive category that licenses them to coordinate with others for mutual gain. We discuss how this hypothesis can provide a unified explanation for diverse phenomena in human social life, including recursive mentalizing, performative speech acts, public protests, hypocrisy, and self-conscious emotional expressions.


Assuntos
Comportamento Cooperativo , Relações Interpessoais , Recompensa , Teoria da Mente/fisiologia , Adulto , Altruísmo , Feminino , Humanos , Conhecimento , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Assunção de Riscos
15.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 18(11): 586-95, 2014 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25172525

RESUMO

Specific language impairment (SLI), a genetic developmental disorder, offers insights into the neurobiological and computational organization of language. A subtype, Grammatical-SLI (G-SLI), involves greater impairments in 'extended' grammatical representations, which are nonlocal, hierarchical, abstract, and composed, than in 'basic' ones, which are local, linear, semantic, and holistic. This distinction is seen in syntax, morphology, and phonology, and may be tied to abnormalities in the left hemisphere and basal ganglia, consistent with new models of the neurobiology of language which distinguish dorsal and ventral processing streams. Delineating neurolinguistic phenotypes promises a better understanding of the effects of genes on the brain circuitry underlying normal and impaired language abilities.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Idioma , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Linguagem Infantil , Humanos , Fonética , Psicolinguística
16.
Psychol Sci ; 25(8): 1511-7, 2014 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24898726

RESUMO

What function do facial expressions have? We tested the hypothesis that some expressions serve as honest signals of subjective commitments-in particular, that angry faces increase the effectiveness of threats. In an ultimatum game, proposers decided how much money to offer a responder while seeing a film clip depicting an angry or a neutral facial expression, together with a written threat that was either inherently credible (a 50-50 split) or less credible (a demand for 70% of the money). Proposers offered greater amounts in response to the less credible threat when it was accompanied by an angry expression than when it was accompanied by a neutral expression, but were unaffected by the expression when dealing with the credible threat. This finding supports the hypothesis that angry expressions are honest signals that enhance the credibility of threats.


Assuntos
Ira/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos
17.
Am Psychol ; 68(6): 467-8, 2013 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24016117

RESUMO

Presents an obituary for George A. Miller (1920-2012). Miller ranks among the most important psychologists of the 20th century. In addition to writing one of the best known papers in the history of psychology ("The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on Our Capacity for Processing Information," published in Psychological Review in 1956), Miller also fomented the cognitive revolution, invented psycholinguistics and cognitive psychology, imported powerful ideas from the theories of information, communication, grammar, semantics, and artificial intelligence, and left us a sparkling oeuvre that proves that a rigorous scientist needn't write in soggy prose. Honors rained down on Miller. APA gave him the Award for Distinguished Scientific Contributions (1963), the American Psychological Foundation Gold Medal Award for Life Achievement in Psychological Science (1990), the William James Book Award (1992, for The Science of Words), and the Award for Lifetime Contributions to Psychology (2003), and named a prize after him, as did the Cognitive Neuroscience Society. Miller was also honored by the Association for Psychological Science and the American Speech and Hearing Association. In 2000, he won the John P. McGovern Award in the Behavioral Sciences from the American Association for the Advancement of Science, and in 1991, the National Medal of Science, the country's highest scientific honor.


Assuntos
Psicolinguística/história , Psicologia/história , Ciência Cognitiva/história , História do Século XX , História do Século XXI
18.
Am J Public Health ; 103 Suppl 1: S152-66, 2013 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23927501

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: We explain why traits of interest to behavioral scientists may have a genetic architecture featuring hundreds or thousands of loci with tiny individual effects rather than a few with large effects and why such an architecture makes it difficult to find robust associations between traits and genes. METHODS: We conducted a genome-wide association study at 2 sites, Harvard University and Union College, measuring more than 100 physical and behavioral traits with a sample size typical of candidate gene studies. We evaluated predictions that alleles with large effect sizes would be rare and most traits of interest to social science are likely characterized by a lack of strong directional selection. We also carried out a theoretical analysis of the genetic architecture of traits based on R.A. Fisher's geometric model of natural selection and empirical analyses of the effects of selection bias and phenotype measurement stability on the results of genetic association studies. RESULTS: Although we replicated several known genetic associations with physical traits, we found only 2 associations with behavioral traits that met the nominal genome-wide significance threshold, indicating that physical and behavioral traits are mainly affected by numerous genes with small effects. CONCLUSIONS: The challenge for social science genomics is the likelihood that genes are connected to behavioral variation by lengthy, nonlinear, interactive causal chains, and unraveling these chains requires allying with personal genomics to take advantage of the potential for large sample sizes as well as continuing with traditional epidemiological studies.


Assuntos
Cor de Olho/genética , Genes , Cor de Cabelo/genética , Personalidade/genética , Ciências Sociais , Adolescente , Adulto , Comportamento , Fenômenos Biológicos , Feminino , Pesquisa em Genética , Estudo de Associação Genômica Ampla , Humanos , Masculino , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único/genética , Seleção Genética , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
19.
Science ; 338(6105): 327-8, 2012 Oct 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23087229
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...