Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 28
Filtrar
1.
Optom Vis Sci ; 99(1): 24-30, 2022 01 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34882612

RESUMO

SIGNIFICANCE: The development of visual acuity has often been looked upon as a function of age. This study considers whether cognition might also be a predictor of acuity in children. The results indicate that cognition is a predictor of acuity and therefore should play a role in vision evaluations and developmental research. PURPOSE: Prior studies have shown that changes in visual acuity in typically developing children occur beyond primary school age. However, these studies almost exclusively use chronological age as the sole predictor for visual development. Because many of the tasks used to measure acuity have a cognitive demand, it is possible that age is not the best predictor for changes in this function. The aims of this study were to explore the effect of cognition on the development of visual acuity and to compare this predictor with age. METHODS: The predictive ability of chronological age and cognition on acuity was assessed in a group of 81 typical children between 5 and 11 years old. RESULTS: Analysis of resulting trajectories showed that, although age indeed was a good predictor, development of visual acuity was equally well predicted by cognition. Moreover, partial correlations showed a strong correlation between cognition and acuity when controlling for age but no significant correlation between age and acuity when controlling for cognition. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that age alone is not the optimal determinant for the development of visual acuity in typical school-aged children, as cognition was also found to be an important predictor.


Assuntos
Cognição , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Criança , Humanos , Acuidade Visual
2.
Eur J Nutr ; 58(7): 2911-2920, 2019 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30327868

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Previous evidence suggests consumption of flavonoids, a sub-class of polyphenols, is associated with improved cognitive function across the lifespan. In particular, acute intervention of a flavonoid-rich wild blueberry (WBB) drink has been shown to boost executive function (EF), short-term memory and mood 2-6 h post-consumption in 7-10-year-old children. However, confirmation of the aspects of EF and memory susceptible to WBB ingestion is required, particularly during childhood, a critical period of neurological development. In addition, the child literature on berry flavonoid supplementation and cognition highlights the potential for such interventions to elicit positive benefits to real-world educational scenarios, such as reading, a complex ability which relies upon aspects of cognition already known to improve following WBB. METHODS: Here we examined which aspects of EF and memory are susceptible to acute WBB, as well as investigating whether acute WBB could further benefit reading ability. Fifty-four healthy children, aged 7-10 years, consumed a 200 ml WBB drink (253 mg anthocyanins) or a matched placebo according to a randomised, single-blind, parallel-groups design. Verbal memory (Auditory Verbal Learning Task; AVLT), EF (Modified Attention Network Task; MANT), and reading efficiency (Test of Word Reading Efficiency-2; TOWRE-2) were assessed at baseline and 2 h post-consumption. RESULTS: For the MANT, significantly quicker RTs were observed for WBB participants when compared to placebo participants on 120 ms trials, without cost to accuracy. Furthermore, WBB participants showed enhanced verbal memory performance on the AVLT, recalling more words than placebo participants on short delay and memory acquisition measures post-consumption. Despite these significant improvements in cognitive performance, no significant effects were observed for reading measures. CONCLUSION: Consumption of WBB was found to significantly improve memory and attentional aspects of EF. This indicates that a flavonoid-rich blueberry product, equivalent to 240 g or 1½ cups of fresh blueberries can provide acute cognitive benefits in children. These findings support accumulating evidence that flavonoid-rich products are beneficial for healthy brain function, particularly during critical developmental periods. However, the lack of findings relating to reading ability suggested acute WBB may not be sufficient to elicit benefits to reading. Chronic supplementation and other more sensitive reading measures should be considered for examining the effects of WBB on such a complex skill in the future.


Assuntos
Mirtilos Azuis (Planta) , Cognição/efeitos dos fármacos , Suplementos Nutricionais , Sucos de Frutas e Vegetais , Memória de Curto Prazo/efeitos dos fármacos , Leitura , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Método Simples-Cego
4.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 56(9): 5370-80, 2015 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26275135

RESUMO

PURPOSE: This study investigated whether vergence and accommodation development in preterm infants is preprogrammed or is driven by experience. METHODS: Thirty-two healthy infants, born at mean 34 weeks gestation (range, 31.2-36 weeks), were compared with 45 healthy full-term infants (mean 40.0 weeks) over a 6-month period, starting at 4 to 6 weeks postnatally. Simultaneous accommodation and convergence to a detailed target were measured using a Plusoptix PowerRefII infrared photorefractor as a target moved between 0.33 and 2 m. Stimulus/response gains and responses at 0.33 and 2 m were compared by both corrected (gestational) age and chronological (postnatal) age. RESULTS: When compared by their corrected age, preterm and full-term infants showed few significant differences in vergence and accommodation responses after 6 to 7 weeks of age. However, when compared by chronological age, preterm infants' responses were more variable, with significantly reduced vergence gains, reduced vergence response at 0.33 m, reduced accommodation gain, and increased accommodation at 2 m compared to full-term infants between 8 and 13 weeks after birth. CONCLUSIONS: When matched by corrected age, vergence and accommodation in preterm infants show few differences from full-term infants' responses. Maturation appears preprogrammed and is not advanced by visual experience. Longer periods of immature visual responses might leave preterm infants more at risk of development of oculomotor deficits such as strabismus.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Doenças do Prematuro/fisiopatologia , Recém-Nascido Prematuro/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Estrabismo/fisiopatologia , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Pré-Escolar , Convergência Ocular , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Idade Gestacional , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Doenças do Prematuro/diagnóstico , Masculino , Estrabismo/congênito , Estrabismo/diagnóstico
5.
J AAPOS ; 18(6): 576-83, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25498466

RESUMO

PURPOSE: To propose an alternative and practical model to conceptualize clinical patterns of concomitant intermittent strabismus, heterophoria, and convergence and accommodation anomalies. METHODS: Despite identical ratios, there can be a disparity- or blur-biased "style" in three hypothetical scenarios: normal; high ratio of accommodative convergence to accommodation (AC/A) and low ratio of convergence accommodation to convergence (CA/C); low AC/A and high CA/C. We calculated disparity bias indices (DBI) to reflect these biases and provide early objective data from small illustrative clinical groups that fit these styles. RESULTS: Normal adults (n = 56) and children (n = 24) showed disparity bias (adult DBI 0.43 [95% CI, 0.50-0.36], child DBI 0.20 [95% CI, 0.31-0.07]; P = 0.001). Accommodative esotropia (n = 3) showed less disparity-bias (DBI 0.03). In the high AC/A-low CA/C scenario, early presbyopia (n = 22) showed mean DBI of 0.17 (95% CI, 0.28-0.06), compared to DBI of -0.31 in convergence excess esotropia (n=8). In the low AC/A-high CA/C scenario near exotropia (n = 17) showed mean DBI of 0.27. DBI ranged between 1.25 and -1.67. CONCLUSIONS: Establishing disparity or blur bias adds to AC/A and CA/C ratios to explain clinical patterns. Excessive bias or inflexibility in near-cue use increases risk of clinical problems.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Modelos Teóricos , Erros de Refração/fisiopatologia , Estrabismo/fisiopatologia , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
6.
PLoS One ; 9(3): e91988, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24642662

RESUMO

ERPs were elicited to (1) words, (2) pseudowords derived from these words, and (3) nonwords with no lexical neighbors, in a task involving listening to immediately repeated auditory stimuli. There was a significant early (P200) effect of phonotactic probability in the first auditory presentation, which discriminated words and pseudowords from nonwords; and a significant somewhat later (N400) effect of lexicality, which discriminated words from pseudowords and nonwords. There was no reliable effect of lexicality in the ERPs to the second auditory presentation. We conclude that early sublexical phonological processing differed according to phonotactic probability of the stimuli, and that lexically-based redintegration occurred for words but did not occur for pseudowords or nonwords. Thus, in online word recognition and immediate retrieval, phonological and/or sublexical processing plays a more important role than lexical level redintegration.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Psicolinguística , Adolescente , Adulto , Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fonética , Tempo de Reação , Semântica
7.
J AAPOS ; 18(2): 162-8, 2014 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24582466

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Although eye exercises appear to help heterophoria, convergence insufficiency, and intermittent strabismus, results can be confounded by placebo, practice, and encouragement effects. This study assessed objective changes in vergence and accommodation responses in naive young adults after a 2-week period of eye exercises under controlled conditions to determine the extent to which treatment effects occur over other factors. METHODS: Asymptomatic young adults were randomly assigned to one of two no-treatment (control) groups or to one of six eye exercise groups: accommodation, vergence, both, convergence in excess of accommodation, accommodation in excess of convergence, and placebo. Subjects were tested and retested under identical conditions, except for the second control group, who were additionally encouraged. Objective accommodation and vergence were assessed to a range of targets moving in depth containing combinations of blur, disparity, and proximity/looming cues. RESULTS: A total of 156 subjects were included. Response gain improved more for less naturalistic targets where more improvement was possible. Convergence exercises improved vergence for near across all targets (P = 0.035). Mean accommodation changed similarly but nonsignificantly. No other treatment group differed significantly from the nonencouraged control group, whereas encouraging effort produced significantly increased vergence (P = 0.004) and accommodation (P = 0.005) gains in the second control group. CONCLUSIONS: True treatment effects were small, significantly better only after vergence exercises to a nonaccommodative target, and rarely related to the response they were designed to improve. Exercising accommodation without convergence made no difference to accommodation to cues containing detail. Additional effort improved objective responses the most.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Exercício Físico/fisiologia , Fenômenos Fisiológicos Oculares , Ortóptica/métodos , Estrabismo/terapia , Adolescente , Adulto , Técnicas de Diagnóstico Oftalmológico/instrumentação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estrabismo/diagnóstico , Estrabismo/fisiopatologia , Inquéritos e Questionários , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
8.
Br J Ophthalmol ; 98(5): 679-83, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24532798

RESUMO

AIM: This paper presents Convergence Insufficiency Symptom Survey (CISS) and orthoptic findings in a sample of typical young adults who considered themselves to have normal eyesight apart from weak spectacles. METHODS: The CISS questionnaire was administered, followed by a full orthoptic evaluation, to 167 university undergraduate and postgraduate students during the recruitment phase of another study. The primary criterion for recruitment to this study was that participants 'felt they had normal eyesight'. A CISS score of ≥21 was used to define 'significant' symptoms, and convergence insufficiency (CI) was defined as convergence ≥8 cm from the nose with a fusion range <15Δ base-out with small or no exophoria. RESULTS: The group mean CISS score was 15.4. In all, 17 (10%) of the participants were diagnosed with CI, but 11 (65%) of these did not have significant symptoms. 41 (25%) participants returned a 'high' CISS score of ≥21 but only 6 (15%) of these had genuine CI. Sensitivity of the CISS to detect CI in this asymptomatic sample was 38%; specificity 77%; positive predictive value 15%; and negative predictive value 92%. The area under a receiver operating characteristic curve was 0.596 (95% CI 0.46 to 0.73). CONCLUSIONS: 'Visual symptoms' are common in young adults, but often not related to any clinical defect, while true CI may be asymptomatic. This study suggests that screening for CI is not indicated.


Assuntos
Convergência Ocular , Programas de Rastreamento/estatística & dados numéricos , Transtornos da Motilidade Ocular/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Motilidade Ocular/epidemiologia , Acomodação Ocular , Adolescente , Adulto , Doenças Assintomáticas , Óculos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Programas de Rastreamento/normas , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Prevalência , Curva ROC , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Inquéritos e Questionários , Acuidade Visual , Adulto Jovem
9.
Perception ; 42(7): 693-715, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24344547

RESUMO

Accurate coordination of accommodation and convergence is necessary to view near objects and develop fine motor coordination. We used a remote haploscopic videorefraction paradigm to measure longitudinal changes in simultaneous ocular accommodation and vergence to targets at different depths, and to all combinations of blur, binocular disparity, and change-in-size ('proximity') cues. Infants were followed longitudinally and compared with older children and young adults, with the prediction that sensitivity to different cues would change during development. Mean infant responses to the most naturalistic condition were similar to those of adults from 6-7 weeks (accommodation) and 8-9 weeks (vergence). Proximity cues influenced responses most in infants of less than 14 weeks of age, but sensitivity declined thereafter. Between 12 and 28 weeks of age infants were equally responsive to all three cues, while in older children and adults manipulation of disparity resulted in the greatest changes in response. Despite rapid development of visual acuity (thus increasing availability of blur cues), responses to blur were stable throughout development. Our results suggest that, during much of infancy, vergence and accommodation responses are not dependent on the development of specific depth cues, but make use of any cues available to drive appropriate changes in response.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Acuidade Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Adulto Jovem
10.
Strabismus ; 21(3): 155-64, 2013 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23978142

RESUMO

AIM: To describe preliminary findings of how the profile of the use of blur, disparity, and proximal cues varies between non-strabismic groups and those with different types of esotropia. DESIGN: This was a case control study. METHODOLOGY: A remote haploscopic photorefractor measured simultaneous convergence and accommodation to a range of targets containing all combinations of binocular disparity, blur, and proximal (looming) cues. Thirteen constant esotropes, 16 fully accommodative esotropes, and 8 convergence excess esotropes were compared with age- and refractive error-matched controls and 27 young adult emmetropic controls. All wore full refractive correction if not emmetropic. Response AC/A and CA/C ratios were also assessed. RESULTS: Cue use differed between the groups. Even esotropes with constant suppression and no binocular vision (BV) responded to disparity in cues. The constant esotropes with weak BV showed trends for more stable responses and better vergence and accommodation than those without any BV. The accommodative esotropes made less use of disparity cues to drive accommodation (p = 0.04) and more use of blur to drive vergence (p = 0.008) than controls. All esotropic groups failed to show the strong bias for better responses to disparity cues found in the controls, with convergence excess esotropes favoring blur cues. AC/A and CA/C ratios existed in an inverse relationship in the different groups. Accommodative lag of > 1.0 D at 33 cm was common (46%) in the pooled esotropia groups compared with 11% in typical children (p = 0.05). CONCLUSION: Esotropic children use near cues differently from matched non-esotropic children in ways characteristic to their deviations. Relatively higher weighting for blur cues was found in accommodative esotropia compared to matched controls.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Esotropia/fisiopatologia , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
11.
Strabismus ; 21(2): 140-4, 2013 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23713939

RESUMO

AIM: To provide evidence that a near clinical gradient AC/A ratio could instead reflect the CA/C relationship (the accommodation driven by response to disparity). DESIGN: Case control study. METHODOLOGY: 27 emmetropic participants with heterophoria <4 PD, 19 with intermittent distance exotropia, and 17 with near exophoria >6 PD were tested. A remote haploscopic photorefractor, which can measure simultaneous convergence and accommodation to a range of targets containing all combinations of presence or absence of binocular disparity, blur, and proximal (looming) cues, was used to assess response AC/A and CA/C relationships. These were compared with clinical gradient AC/A ratios at near and distance fixation using alternate prism cover test and plus or minus lenses. RESULTS: Although the near and distance clinical AC/A ratios correlated weakly with each other (p = 0.03), neither clinical method correlated with the more accurate response AC/A ratio from the laboratory method (p = 0.88 and p = 0.93, respectively). The laboratory CA/C ratio correlated strongly with the near clinical AC/A ratio (p = 0.004) but only very weakly with the distance ratio (p = 0.16). CONCLUSIONS: The "near gradient AC/A ratio" may actually reflect the CA/C linkage as the dissociation of the prism cover test disrupts vergence accommodation. If the near deviation diverges more with plus lenses, it may be because the lenses allow clear near vision without needing to recruit convergence accommodation to achieve it.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Exotropia/fisiopatologia , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Lentes , Masculino
12.
PLoS One ; 7(10): e48357, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23118992

RESUMO

The goal of this research was to investigate the changes in neural processing in mild cognitive impairment. We measured phase synchrony, amplitudes, and event-related potentials in veridical and false memory to determine whether these differed in participants with mild cognitive impairment compared with typical, age-matched controls. Empirical mode decomposition phase locking analysis was used to assess synchrony, which is the first time this analysis technique has been applied in a complex cognitive task such as memory processing. The technique allowed assessment of changes in frontal and parietal cortex connectivity over time during a memory task, without a priori selection of frequency ranges, which has been shown previously to influence synchrony detection. Phase synchrony differed significantly in its timing and degree between participant groups in the theta and alpha frequency ranges. Timing differences suggested greater dependence on gist memory in the presence of mild cognitive impairment. The group with mild cognitive impairment had significantly more frontal theta phase locking than the controls in the absence of a significant behavioural difference in the task, providing new evidence for compensatory processing in the former group. Both groups showed greater frontal phase locking during false than true memory, suggesting increased searching when no actual memory trace was found. Significant inter-group differences in frontal alpha phase locking provided support for a role for lower and upper alpha oscillations in memory processing. Finally, fronto-parietal interaction was significantly reduced in the group with mild cognitive impairment, supporting the notion that mild cognitive impairment could represent an early stage in Alzheimer's disease, which has been described as a 'disconnection syndrome'.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/fisiopatologia , Memória/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Comportamento/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados , Neuroimagem Funcional , Humanos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Análise de Ondaletas
13.
Clin Exp Optom ; 95(2): 153-9, 2012 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22283788

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: In a previous study, we demonstrated that children with early onset myopia had greater instability of accommodation than a group of emmetropic children. Since that study was correlational, we were unable to determine the causal relationship between this and myopic progression. To address this, we examined the children two years later. We predicted that if accommodative instability was causing the myopic progression, instability at Visit 1 should predict the refractive error at Visit 2. Additionally, instability at Visit 1 should predict myopic progression. METHODS: Thirteen myopic and 16 emmetropic children were included in the analysis. Dynamic measures of accommodation were made using eccentric photorefraction (PowerRefractor) while children viewed targets set at three distances (accommodative demands), namely, 0.25 metres (4.00 D demand), 0.5 metres (2.00 D demand) and 4.00 metres (0.25 D demand). RESULTS: Both refractive error and accommodative instability at Visit 1 were highly correlated with the same measures at Visit 2. Children with myopia showed greater instability of accommodation (0.38 D) than children with emmetropia (0.26 D) at the 4.00 D target on Visit 1 and this instability of accommodation weakly predicted myopic progression. CONCLUSIONS: The results presented in the present study suggest that instability of accommodation accompanies myopic progression, although a casual relationship cannot be established.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Miopia/etiologia , Miopia/fisiopatologia , Adolescente , Idade de Início , Criança , Progressão da Doença , Óculos , Seguimentos , Humanos , Miopia/terapia , Optometria/métodos , Optometria/normas , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
14.
Acta Ophthalmol ; 90(2): e109-17, 2012 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22280437

RESUMO

PURPOSE: This study considered whether vergence drives accommodation or accommodation drives vergence during the control of distance exotropia for near fixation. High accommodative convergence to accommodation (AC/A) ratios are often used to explain this control, but the role of convergence to drive accommodation (the CA/C relationship) is rarely considered. Atypical CA/C characteristics could equally, or better, explain common clinical findings. METHODS: Nineteen distance exotropes, aged 4-11 years, were compared while controlling their deviation with 27 non-exotropic controls aged 5-9 years. Simultaneous vergence and accommodation responses were measured to a range of targets incorporating different combinations of blur, disparity and looming cues at four fixation distances between 2 m and 33 cm. Stimulus and response AC/A and CA/C ratios were calculated. RESULTS: Accommodation responses for near targets (p = 0.017) and response gains (p = 0.026) were greater in the exotropes than in the controls. Despite higher clinical stimulus AC/A ratios, the distance exotropes showed lower laboratory response AC/A ratios (p = 0.02), but significantly higher CA/C ratios (p = 0.02). All the exotropes, whether the angle changed most with lenses ('controlled by accommodation') or on occlusion ('controlled by fusion'), used binocular disparity not blur as their main cue to target distance. CONCLUSIONS: Increased vergence demand to control intermittent distance exotropia for near also drives significantly more accommodation. Minus lens therapy is more likely to act by correcting overaccommodation driven by controlling convergence, rather than by inducing blur-driven vergence. The use of convergence as a major drive to accommodation explains many clinical characteristics of distance exotropia, including apparently high near stimulus AC/A ratios.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Exotropia/fisiopatologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Erros de Refração/fisiopatologia , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia
15.
Br J Ophthalmol ; 96(4): 508-13, 2012 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21873311

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Disparity cues can be a major drive to accommodation via the convergence accommodation to convergence (CA/C) linkage, but, on decompensation of exotropia, disparity cues are extinguished by suppression so this drive is lost. This study investigated accommodation and vergence responses to disparity, blur and proximal cues in a group of distance exotropes aged between 4 and 11 years both during decompensation and when exotropic. METHODS: 19 participants with distance exotropia were tested using a PlusoptiXSO4 photo refractor set in a remote haploscopic device that assessed simultaneous vergence and accommodation to a range of targets incorporating different combinations of blur, disparity and proximal cues at four fixation distances between 2 m and 33 cm. Responses on decompensation were compared with those from the same children when their deviation was controlled. RESULTS: Manifest exotropia was more common in the more impoverished cue conditions. When decompensated for near, mean accommodation gain for the all-cue (naturalistic) target was significantly reduced (p<0.0001), with resultant mean under-accommodation of 2.33 D at 33 cm. The profile of near cues usage changed after decompensation, with blur and proximity driving residual responses, but these remaining cues did not compensate for loss of accommodation caused by the removal of disparity. CONCLUSIONS: Accommodation often reduces on decompensation of distance exotropia as the drive from convergence is extinguished, providing a further reason to try to prevent decompensation for near.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Percepção de Distância/fisiologia , Exotropia/fisiopatologia , Disparidade Visual , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Convergência Ocular , Sinais (Psicologia) , Exotropia/diagnóstico , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa
16.
Br J Ophthalmol ; 95(2): 231-7, 2011 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20603431

RESUMO

AIMS: Accommodation to overcome hypermetropia is implicated in emmetropisation. This study recorded accommodation responses in a wide range of emmetropising infants and older children with clinically significant hypermetropia to assess common characteristics and differences. METHODS: A PlusoptiXSO4 photorefractor in a laboratory setting was used to collect binocular accommodation data from participants viewing a detailed picture target moving between 33 cm and 2 m. 38 typically developing infants were studied between 6 and 26 weeks of age and were compared with cross-sectional data from children 5-9 y of age with clinically significant hypermetropia (n = 15), corrected fully accommodative strabismus (n=14) and 27 age-matched controls. RESULTS: Hypermetropes of all ages under-accommodated compared to controls at all distances, whether corrected or not (p < 0.00001) and lag related to manifest refraction. Emmetropising infants under-accommodated most in the distance, while the hypermetropic patient groups under-accommodated most for near. CONCLUSIONS: Better accommodation for near than distance is demonstrated in those hypermetropic children who go on to emmetropise. This supports the approach of avoiding refractive correction in such children. In contrast, hypermetropic children referred for treatment for reduced distance visual acuity are not likely to habitually accommodate to overcome residual hypermetropia left by an under-correction.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Anisometropia/fisiopatologia , Hiperopia/fisiopatologia , Testes Visuais/instrumentação , Acuidade Visual/fisiologia , Anisometropia/complicações , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Resultado do Tratamento , Testes Visuais/métodos
17.
J AAPOS ; 14(5): 447-9, 2010 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20863728

RESUMO

Accommodation is considered to be a symmetrical response and to be driven by the least ametropic and nonamblyopic eye in anisometropia. We report the case of a 4-year-old child with anisometropic amblyopia who accommodates asymmetrically, reliably demonstrating normal accommodation in the nonamblyopic eye and antiaccommodation of the amblyopic eye to near targets. The abnormal accommodation of the amblyopic eye remained largely unchanged during 7 subsequent testing sessions undertaken over the course of therapy. We suggest that a congenital dysinnervation syndrome may result in relaxation of accommodation in relation to near cues and might be a hitherto unconsidered additional etiological factor in anisometropic amblyopia.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Ambliopia/etiologia , Ambliopia/fisiopatologia , Refração Ocular/fisiologia , Anisometropia/etiologia , Anisometropia/fisiopatologia , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos
18.
Behav Res Methods ; 42(2): 517-24, 2010 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20479183

RESUMO

Laboratory animals should be provided with enrichment objects in their cages; however, it is first necessary to test whether the proposed enrichment objects provide benefits that increase the animals' welfare. The two main paradigms currently used to assess proposed enrichment objects are the choice test, which is limited to determining relative frequency of choice, and consumer demand studies, which can indicate the strength of a preference but are complex to design. Here, we propose a third methodology: a runway paradigm, which can be used to assess the strength of an animal's motivation for enrichment objects, is simpler to use than consumer demand studies, and is faster to complete than typical choice tests. Time spent with objects in a standard choice test was used to rank several enrichment objects in order to compare with the ranking found in our runway paradigm. The rats ran significantly more times, ran faster, and interacted longer with objects with which they had previously spent the most time. It was concluded that this simple methodology is suitable for measuring rats' motivation to reach enrichment objects. This can be used to assess the preference for different types of enrichment objects or to measure reward system processes.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Motivação , Psicologia Experimental/métodos , Corrida , Animais , Masculino , Ratos , Ratos Endogâmicos , Fatores de Tempo
19.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 30(2): 152-9, 2010 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20444119

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Vergence and accommodation studies often use adult participants with experience of vision science. Reports of infant and clinical responses are generally more variable and of lower gain, with the implication that differences lie in immaturity or sub-optimal clinical characteristics but expert/naïve differences are rarely considered or quantified. METHODS: Sixteen undergraduates, naïve to vision science, were individually matched by age, visual acuity, refractive error, heterophoria, stereoacuity and near point of accommodation to second- and third-year orthoptics and optometry undergraduates ('experts'). Accommodation and vergence responses were assessed to targets moving between 33 cm, 50 cm, 1 m and 2 m using a haploscopic device incorporating a PlusoptiX SO4 autorefractor. Disparity, blur and looming cues were separately available or minimised in all combinations. Instruction set was minimal. RESULTS: In all cases, vergence and accommodation response slopes (gain) were steeper and closer to 1.0 in the expert group (p = 0.001), with the largest expert/naïve differences for both vergence and accommodation being for near targets (p = 0.012). For vergence, the differences between expert and naïve response slopes increased with increasingly open-loop targets (linear trend p = 0.025). Although we predicted that proximal cues would drive additional response in the experts, the proximity-only cue was the only condition that showed no statistical effect of experience. CONCLUSIONS: Expert observers provide more accurate responses to near target demand than closely matched naïve observers. We suggest that attention, practice, voluntary and proprioceptive effects may enhance responses in experienced participants when compared to a more typical general population. Differences between adult reports and the developmental and clinical literature may partially reflect expert/naïve effects, as well as developmental change. If developmental and clinical studies are to be compared to adult normative data, uninstructed naïve adult data should be used.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Variações Dependentes do Observador , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa/instrumentação , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Estudantes de Medicina , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
20.
Optom Vis Sci ; 86(11): 1276-86, 2009 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19770814

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Accommodation can mask hyperopia and reduce the accuracy of non-cycloplegic refraction. It is, therefore, important to minimize accommodation to obtain a measure of hyperopia as accurate as possible. To characterize the parameters required to measure the maximally hyperopic error using photorefraction, we used different target types and distances to determine which target was most likely to maximally relax accommodation and thus more accurately detect hyperopia in an individual. METHODS: A PlusoptiX SO4 infra-red photorefractor was mounted in a remote haploscope which presented the targets. All participants were tested with targets at four fixation distances between 0.3 and 2 m containing all combinations of blur, disparity, and proximity/looming cues. Thirty-eight infants (6 to 44 weeks) were studied longitudinally, and 104 children [4 to 15 years (mean 6.4)] and 85 adults, with a range of refractive errors and binocular vision status, were tested once. Cycloplegic refraction data were available for a sub-set of 59 participants spread across the age range. RESULTS: The maximally hyperopic refraction (MHR) found at any time in the session was most frequently found when fixating the most distant targets and those containing disparity and dynamic proximity/looming cues. Presence or absence of blur was less significant, and targets in which only single cues to depth were present were also less likely to produce MHR. MHR correlated closely with cycloplegic refraction (r = 0.93, mean difference 0.07 D, p = n.s., 95% confidence interval +/-<0.25 D) after correction by a calibration factor. CONCLUSIONS: Maximum relaxation of accommodation occurred for binocular targets receding into the distance. Proximal and disparity cues aid relaxation of accommodation to a greater extent than blur, and thus non-cycloplegic refraction targets should incorporate these cues. This is especially important in screening contexts with a brief opportunity to test for significant hyperopia. MHR in our laboratory was found to be a reliable estimation of cycloplegic refraction.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular , Sinais (Psicologia) , Hiperopia/diagnóstico , Disparidade Visual , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Hiperopia/fisiopatologia , Lactente , Estudos Longitudinais , Midriáticos , Refração Ocular , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...