Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 17 de 17
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Nature ; 588(7836): 118-123, 2020 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33177711

RESUMO

Wavelength is a physical measure of light, and the intricate understanding of its link to perceived colour enables the creation of perceptual entities such as metamers-non-overlapping spectral compositions that generate identical colour percepts1. By contrast, scientists have been unable to develop a physical measure linked to perceived smell, even one that merely reflects the extent of perceptual similarity between odorants2. Here, to generate such a measure, we collected perceptual similarity estimates of 49,788 pairwise odorants from 199 participants who smelled 242 different multicomponent odorants and used these data to refine a predictive model that links odorant structure to odorant perception3. The resulting measure combines 21 physicochemical features of the odorants into a single number-expressed in radians-that accurately predicts the extent of perceptual similarity between multicomponent odorant pairs. To assess the usefulness of this measure, we investigated whether we could use it to create olfactory metamers. To this end, we first identified a cut-off in the measure: pairs of multicomponent odorants that were within 0.05 radians of each other or less were very difficult to discriminate. Using this cut-off, we were able to design olfactory metamers-pairs of non-overlapping molecular compositions that generated identical odour percepts. The accurate predictions of perceptual similarity, and the ensuing creation of olfactory metamers, suggest that we have obtained a valid olfactory measure, one that may enable the digitization of smell.


Assuntos
Odorantes/análise , Percepção Olfatória/fisiologia , Olfato/fisiologia , Adulto , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Discriminação Psicológica , Feminino , Ferula , Humanos , Masculino , Rosa , Viola , Adulto Jovem
2.
Nat Hum Behav ; 3(5): 501-512, 2019 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31089297

RESUMO

Olfactory stimulus acquisition is perfectly synchronized with inhalation, which tunes neuronal ensembles for incoming information. Because olfaction is an ancient sensory system that provided a template for brain evolution, we hypothesized that this link persisted, and therefore nasal inhalations may also tune the brain for acquisition of non-olfactory information. To test this, we measured nasal airflow and electroencephalography during various non-olfactory cognitive tasks. We observed that participants spontaneously inhale at non-olfactory cognitive task onset and that such inhalations shift brain functional network architecture. Concentrating on visuospatial perception, we observed that nasal inhalation drove increased task-related brain activity in specific task-related brain regions and resulted in improved performance accuracy in the visuospatial task. Thus, mental processes with no link to olfaction are nevertheless phase-locked with nasal inhalation, consistent with the notion of an olfaction-based template in the evolution of human brain function.


Assuntos
Ondas Encefálicas/fisiologia , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Conectoma , Inalação/fisiologia , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Pensamento/fisiologia , Adulto , Expiração/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Cavidade Nasal/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
3.
Chem Senses ; 44(4): 267-278, 2019 04 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30873534

RESUMO

A common goal in olfaction research is modeling the link between odorant structure and odor perception. Such modeling efforts require large data sets on olfactory perception, yet only a few of these are publicly and freely available. Given that individual odor perception may be informative on personal makeup and interpersonal relationships, we hypothesized that people would gladly provide olfactory perceptual estimates in the context of an odor-based social network. We developed a web-based infrastructure for such a network we called SmellSpace and distributed 10 000 scratch-and-sniff registration booklets each containing a subset of 12 out of 35 microencapsulated odorants. Within ~100 days, we obtained data from ~1000 participants who rated the odorants along 13 verbal descriptors. To verify that these estimates are comparable to lab-collected estimates we tested 26 participants in a controlled lab setting using the same odorants and descriptors. We observed remarkably high overall group correlations between lab and SmellSpace data, implying that this method provides for credible group-representations of odorants. We further estimated the usability of the data by applying to it two previously published models that used odorant structure alone to predict either odorant pleasantness or pairwise odorant perceptual similarity. We observed statistically significant predictions in both cases, thus further implying that the current data may be helpful toward future efforts of modeling olfactory perception from structure. We conclude that an odor-based social network is a potentially useful instrument for collecting extensive data on olfactory perception and here post the complete raw data set from the first ~1000 participants.


Assuntos
Odorantes , Olfato , Rede Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Percepção Olfatória , Adulto Jovem
4.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 1602, 2019 02 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30733509

RESUMO

In many sensory systems, different sensory features are transmitted in parallel by several different types of output neurons. In the mouse olfactory bulb, there are only two output neuron types, the mitral and tufted cells (M/T), which receive similar odor inputs, but they are believed to transmit different odor characteristics. How these two neuron types deliver different odor information is unclear. Here, by combining electrophysiology and optogenetics, it is shown that distinct inhibitory networks modulate M/T cell responses differently. Overall strong lateral inhibition was scarce, with most neurons receiving lateral inhibition from a handful of unorganized surrounding glomeruli (~5% on average). However, there was a considerable variability between different neuron types in the strength and frequency of lateral inhibition. Strong lateral inhibition was mostly found in neurons locked to the first half of the respiration cycle. In contrast, weak inhibition arriving from many surrounding glomeruli was relatively more common in neurons locked to the late phase of the respiration cycle. Proximal neurons could receive different levels of inhibition. These results suggest that there is considerable diversity in the way M/T cells process odors so that even neurons that receive the same odor input transmit different odor information to the cortex.


Assuntos
Inibição Neural , Neurônios/citologia , Bulbo Olfatório/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Masculino , Camundongos
5.
J Biotechnol ; 267: 45-49, 2018 Feb 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29292129

RESUMO

Improved easy-to-use diagnostic tools for infections are in strong demand worldwide. Yet, despite dramatic advances in diagnostic technologies, the gold-standard remains culturing. Here we offer an alternative tool demonstrating that a bacterial biosensor can efficiently detect Pseudomonas aeruginosa infections in patients suffering from otitis externa. Detection was based on specific binding between the biosensor and 2-aminoacetophenone (2-AA), a volatile produced by P. aeruginosa in high amounts. We collected pus samples from ears of 26 subjects exhibiting symptoms of otitis externa. Detection of P. aeruginosa using the biosensor was compared to detection using gold-standard culturing assay and to gas-chromatograph-mass-spectrometry (GC-MS) analyses of 2-AA. The biosensor strain test matched the culture assay in 24 samples (92%) and the GC-MS analyses in 25 samples (96%). With this result in hand, we designed a device containing a whole-cell luminescent biosensor combined with a photo-multiplier tube. This device allowed detection of 2-AA at levels as low as 2 nmol, on par with detection level of GC-MS. The results of the described study demonstrate that the volatile 2-AA serves as an effective biomarker for P. aeruginosa in ear infections, and that activation of the biosensor strain by 2-AA provides a unique opportunity to design an easy-to-use device that can specifically detect P. aeruginosa infections.


Assuntos
Acetofenonas/isolamento & purificação , Técnicas Biossensoriais , Otite Externa/diagnóstico , Pseudomonas aeruginosa/isolamento & purificação , Acetofenonas/química , Cromatografia Gasosa-Espectrometria de Massas , Humanos , Otite Externa/microbiologia , Infecções por Pseudomonas/diagnóstico , Infecções por Pseudomonas/microbiologia , Pseudomonas aeruginosa/química
6.
PLoS One ; 11(10): e0162918, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27711189

RESUMO

Nasal airflow is greater in one nostril than in the other because of transient asymmetric nasal passage obstruction by erectile tissue. The extent of obstruction alternates across nostrils with periodicity referred to as the nasal cycle. The nasal cycle is related to autonomic arousal and is indicative of asymmetry in brain function. Moreover, alterations in nasal cycle periodicity have been linked to various diseases. There is therefore need for a tool allowing continuous accurate measurement and recording of airflow in each nostril separately. Here we provide detailed instructions for constructing such a tool at minimal cost and effort. We demonstrate application of the tool in 33 right-handed healthy subjects, and derive several statistical measures for nasal cycle characterization. Using these measures applied to 24-hour recordings we observed that: 1: subjects spent slightly longer in left over right nostril dominance (left = 2.63 ± 0.89 hours, right = 2.17 ± 0.89 hours, t(32) = 2.07, p < 0.05), 2: cycle duration was shorter in wake than in sleep (wake = 2.02 ± 1.7 hours, sleep = 4.5 ± 1.7 hours, (t(30) = 5.73, p < 0.0001). 3: slower breathing was associated with a more powerful cycle (the extent of difference across nostrils) (r = 0.4, p < 0.0001), and 4: the cycle was influenced by body posture such that lying on one side was associated with greater flow in the contralateral nostril (p < 0.002). Finally, we provide evidence for an airflow cycle in each nostril alone. These results provide characterization of an easily obtained measure that may have diagnostic implications for neurological disease and cognitive state.


Assuntos
Ar , Monitorização Fisiológica/métodos , Nariz , Periodicidade , Adulto , Artefatos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Obstrução Nasal/diagnóstico , Obstrução Nasal/fisiopatologia , Postura , Respiração , Sono/fisiologia , Vigília/fisiologia
7.
Cereb Cortex ; 26(11): 4180-4191, 2016 Oct 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27591145

RESUMO

Rules linking patterns of olfactory receptor neuron activation in the nose to activity patterns in the brain and ensuing odor perception remain poorly understood. Artificially stimulating olfactory neurons with electrical currents and measuring ensuing perception may uncover these rules. We therefore inserted an electrode into the nose of 50 human volunteers and applied various currents for about an hour in each case. This induced assorted non-olfactory sensations but never once the perception of odor. To validate contact with the olfactory path, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure resting-state brain activity in 18 subjects before and after un-sensed stimulation. We observed stimulation-induced neural decorrelation specifically in primary olfactory cortex, implying contact with the olfactory path. These results suggest that indiscriminate olfactory activation does not equate with odor perception. Moreover, this effort serendipitously uncovered a novel path for minimally invasive brain stimulation through the nose.

8.
PLoS One ; 11(2): e0147449, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26840072

RESUMO

We define a new measure of sensory stimuli which has the following properties: It is cross modal, performance based, robust, and well defined. We interpret this measure as the intricacy or complexity of the stimuli, yet its validity is independent of its interpretation. We tested the validity and cross modality of our measure using three olfactory and one visual experiment. In order to test the link between our measure and cognitive performance we also conducted an additional visual experiment. We found that our measure is correlated with the results of the well-established Rapid Serial Visual Presentation masking experiment. Specifically, ranking stimuli according to our measure was correlated at r = 0.75 (p < 0.002) with masking effectiveness. Thus, our novel measure of sensory stimuli provides a new quantitative tool for the study of sensory processing.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Modelos Teóricos , Odorantes , Estimulação Luminosa , Sensação , Algoritmos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Fatores Sexuais
9.
J Neurophysiol ; 115(5): 2294-302, 2016 05 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26888107

RESUMO

Most forms of suprathreshold sensory stimulation perturb sleep. In contrast, presentation of pure olfactory or mild trigeminal odorants does not lead to behavioral or physiological arousal. In fact, some odors promote objective and subjective measures of sleep quality in humans and rodents. The brain mechanisms underlying these sleep-protective properties of olfaction remain unclear. Slow oscillations in the electroencephalogram (EEG) are a marker of deep sleep, and K complexes (KCs) are an EEG marker of cortical response to sensory interference. We therefore hypothesized that odorants presented during sleep will increase power in slow EEG oscillations. Moreover, given that odorants do not drive sleep interruption, we hypothesized that unlike other sensory stimuli odorants would not drive KCs. To test these hypotheses we used polysomnography to measure sleep in 34 healthy subjects (19 women, 15 men; mean age 26.5 ± 2.5 yr) who were repeatedly presented with odor stimuli via a computer-controlled air-dilution olfactometer over the course of a single night. Each participant was exposed to one of four odorants, lavender oil (n = 13), vetiver oil (n = 5), vanillin (n = 12), or ammonium sulfide (n = 4), for durations of 5, 10, and 20 s every 9-15 min. Consistent with our hypotheses, we found that odor presentation during sleep enhanced the power of delta (0.5-4 Hz) and slow spindle (9-12 Hz) frequencies during non-rapid eye movement sleep. The increase was proportionate to odor duration. In addition, odor presentation did not modulate the occurrence of KCs. These findings imply a sleep-promoting olfactory mechanism that may deepen sleep through driving increased slow-frequency oscillations.


Assuntos
Ritmo Delta , Odorantes , Percepção Olfatória , Sono/fisiologia , Adulto , Nível de Alerta , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
10.
Curr Biol ; 25(14): 1904-10, 2015 Jul 20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26144969

RESUMO

Internal action models (IAMs) are brain templates for sensory-motor coordination underlying diverse behaviors. An emerging theory suggests that impaired IAMs are a common theme in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, whether impaired IAMs occur across sensory systems and how they relate to the major phenotype of ASD, namely impaired social communication, remains unclear. Olfaction relies on an IAM known as the sniff response, where sniff magnitude is automatically modulated to account for odor valence. To test the failed IAM theory in olfaction, we precisely measured the non-verbal non-task-dependent sniff response concurrent with pleasant and unpleasant odors in 36 children--18 with ASD and 18 matched typically developing (TD) controls. We found that whereas TD children generated a typical adult-like sniff response within 305 ms of odor onset, ASD children had a profoundly altered sniff response, sniffing equally regardless of odor valance. This difference persisted despite equal reported odor perception and allowed for 81% correct ASD classification based on the sniff response alone (binomial, p < 0.001). Moreover, increasingly aberrant sniffing was associated with increasingly severe ASD (r = -0.75, p < 0.001), specifically with social (r = -0.72, p < 0.001), but not motor (r < -0.38, p > 0.18), impairment. These results uncover a novel ASD marker implying a mechanistic link between the underpinnings of olfaction and ASD and directly linking an impaired IAM with impaired social abilities.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Espectro Autista/etiologia , Odorantes/análise , Olfato , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Israel , Masculino
11.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 112(28): 8750-5, 2015 Jul 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26100865

RESUMO

Each person expresses a potentially unique subset of ∼ 400 different olfactory receptor subtypes. Given that the receptors we express partially determine the odors we smell, it follows that each person may have a unique nose; to capture this, we devised a sensitive test of olfactory perception we termed the "olfactory fingerprint." Olfactory fingerprints relied on matrices of perceived odorant similarity derived from descriptors applied to the odorants. We initially fingerprinted 89 individuals using 28 odors and 54 descriptors. We found that each person had a unique olfactory fingerprint (P < 10(-10)), which was odor specific but descriptor independent. We could identify individuals from this pool using randomly selected sets of 7 odors and 11 descriptors alone. Extrapolating from this data, we determined that using 34 odors and 35 descriptors we could individually identify each of the 7 billion people on earth. Olfactory perception, however, fluctuates over time, calling into question our proposed perceptual readout of presumably stable genetic makeup. To test whether fingerprints remain informative despite this temporal fluctuation, building on the linkage between olfactory receptors and HLA, we hypothesized that olfactory perception may relate to HLA. We obtained olfactory fingerprints and HLA typing for 130 individuals, and found that olfactory fingerprint matching using only four odorants was significantly related to HLA matching (P < 10(-4)), such that olfactory fingerprints can save 32% of HLA tests in a population screen (P < 10(-6)). In conclusion, a precise measure of olfactory perception reveals meaningful nonolfactory genetic information.


Assuntos
Percepção Olfatória , Adulto , Feminino , Antígenos HLA/imunologia , Humanos , Masculino , Odorantes , Percepção Olfatória/genética , Adulto Jovem
12.
Curr Biol ; 24(7): R263-4, 2014 Mar 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24698370

RESUMO

Consciously undetected events are represented at the sensory-motor level and in the neurons of sensory-motor control, for example, consciously undetected visual targets drive eye movements [1] and neural activity [2]. Olfaction offers an opportunity to investigate processing of undetected stimuli through measurements of the sniff-response: odorant-specific modulations of nasal airflow [3-6]. Here, we report evidence that consciously undetected odorants modulate sniffing in a predicted manner. Moreover, in our study we observed that sniff-modulations recurred at least 10 seconds after the onset of an undetected odor, implying that information which was not consciously perceived was nevertheless maintained in memory, available for future decision making.


Assuntos
Odorantes , Olfato/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Humanos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Fatores de Tempo
13.
Chem Senses ; 39(4): 277-81, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24457159

RESUMO

Ample evidence suggests that social chemosignaling plays a significant role in human behavior. Processing of odors and chemosignals depends on sniffing. Given this, we hypothesized that humans may have evolved an automatic mechanism driving sniffs in response to conspecific sniffing. To test this, we measured sniffing behavior of human subjects watching the movie Perfume, which contains many olfactory sniffing events. Despite the total absence of odor, observers sniffed when characters in the movie sniffed. Moreover, this effect was most pronounced in scenes where subjects heard the sniff but did not see the sniffed-at object. We liken this response to the orienting towards conspecific gaze in vision and argue that its robustness further highlights the significance of olfactory information processing in human behavior.


Assuntos
Comportamento/fisiologia , Olfato , Adulto , Discriminação Psicológica , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
14.
Curr Opin Neurobiol ; 25: 107-15, 2014 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24440370

RESUMO

Mammals have ∼1000 different olfactory receptor subtypes, each responding to a number of different odorants, and each odorant activating a number of different receptor subtypes. These molecular and anatomical underpinnings of olfaction imply a perceptual structure of very high dimensionality that relies on combinatorial coding. In contrast to this expectation, the study of olfactory perception reveals a structure of much lower dimensionality. Moreover, a low-dimensionality approach to olfaction enabled derivation of perception-based structural metrics for smell. These metrics provided meaningful predictions of odorant-induced neural activity and perception from odorant structure alone. Based on this low functional dimensionality, we speculate that olfaction likely does not functionally rely on 1000 different receptor subtypes, and their persistence in evolution may imply that they have additional roles in non-olfactory functions such as in guidance of embryogenesis and development.


Assuntos
Percepção Olfatória/fisiologia , Psicofísica/métodos , Olfato/fisiologia , Animais
15.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 22(11): 2491-502, 2010 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19925193

RESUMO

Human electrophysiological research is generally restricted to scalp EEG, magneto-encephalography, and intracranial electrophysiology. Here we examine a unique patient cohort that has undergone decompressive hemicraniectomy, a surgical procedure wherein a portion of the calvaria is removed for several months during which time the scalp overlies the brain without intervening bone. We quantify the differences in signals between electrodes over areas with no underlying skull and scalp EEG electrodes over the intact skull in the same subjects. Signals over the hemicraniectomy have enhanced amplitude and greater task-related power at higher frequencies (60-115 Hz) compared with signals over skull. We also provide evidence of a metric for trial-by-trial EMG/EEG coupling that is effective over the hemicraniectomy but not intact skull at frequencies >60 Hz. Taken together, these results provide evidence that the hemicraniectomy model provides a means for studying neural dynamics in humans with enhanced spatial and temporal resolution.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Piscadela/fisiologia , Lesões Encefálicas/fisiopatologia , Lesões Encefálicas/cirurgia , Córtex Cerebral/cirurgia , Craniectomia Descompressiva/métodos , Eletromiografia/métodos , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/fisiologia , Feminino , Análise de Fourier , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Pressão Intracraniana/fisiologia , Masculino , Movimento/fisiologia , Couro Cabeludo , Fatores de Tempo , Tomografia Computadorizada por Raios X/métodos
16.
J Neurosci ; 29(41): 12948-56, 2009 Oct 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19828809

RESUMO

A fundamental organizational principle of the primate motor system is cortical control of contralateral limb movements. Motor areas also appear to play a role in the control of ipsilateral limb movements. Several studies in monkeys have shown that individual neurons in primary motor cortex (M1) may represent, on average, the direction of movements of the ipsilateral arm. Given the increasing body of evidence demonstrating that neural ensembles can reliably represent information with a high temporal resolution, here we characterize the distributed neural representation of ipsilateral upper limb kinematics in both monkey and man. In two macaque monkeys trained to perform center-out reaching movements, we found that the ensemble spiking activity in M1 could continuously represent ipsilateral limb position. Interestingly, this representation was more correlated with joint angles than hand position. Using bilateral electromyography recordings, we excluded the possibility that postural or mirror movements could exclusively account for these findings. In addition, linear methods could decode limb position from cortical field potentials in both monkeys. We also found that M1 spiking activity could control a biomimetic brain-machine interface reflecting ipsilateral kinematics. Finally, we recorded cortical field potentials from three human subjects and also consistently found evidence of a neural representation for ipsilateral movement parameters. Together, our results demonstrate the presence of a high-fidelity neural representation for ipsilateral movement and illustrates that it can be successfully incorporated into a brain-machine interface.


Assuntos
Braço , Mapeamento Encefálico , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Córtex Motor/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Animais , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Eletromiografia/métodos , Potencial Evocado Motor/fisiologia , Humanos , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Interface Usuário-Computador , Adulto Jovem
17.
Opt Lett ; 31(19): 2849-51, 2006 Oct 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16969399

RESUMO

The g(44) grating is an electroholographic transmission grating in which the applied field is perpendicular to both the grating vector and the wave vector of the incident beam. It is argued that in this configuration the incident beam traverses through a periodically rotating index ellipsoid. It is shown that in the g(44) configuration the Bragg condition is fulfilled for a specific value of the applied field and for a diffracting beam polarization that is perpendicular to that of the incident beam. Consequently, the g(44) grating can be used as an electrically controlled filter. Tunability of 7 nm is demonstrated in a 2mm thick grating.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...