Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 53
Filtrar
1.
J Neurosci Methods ; 403: 110033, 2024 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38056633

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Functional ultrasound imaging (fUS) is an emerging imaging technique that indirectly measures neural activity via changes in blood volume. Chronic fUS imaging during cognitive tasks in freely moving animals faces multiple exceptional challenges: performing large durable craniotomies with chronic implants, designing behavioral experiments matching the hemodynamic timescale, stabilizing the ultrasound probe during freely moving behavior, accurately assessing motion artifacts, and validating that the animal can perform cognitive tasks while tethered. NEW METHOD: We provide validated solutions for those technical challenges. In addition, we present standardized step-by-step reproducible protocols, procedures, and data processing pipelines. Finally, we present proof-of-concept analysis of brain dynamics during a decision making task. RESULTS: We obtain stable recordings from which we can robustly decode task variables from fUS data over multiple months. Moreover, we find that brain wide imaging through hemodynamic response is nonlinearly related to cognitive variables, such as task difficulty, as compared to sensory responses previously explored. COMPARISON WITH EXISTING METHODS: Computational pipelines in fUS are nascent and we present an initial development of a full processing pathway to correct and segment fUS data. CONCLUSIONS: Our methods provide stable imaging and analysis of behavior with fUS that will enable new experimental paradigms in understanding brain-wide dynamics in naturalistic behaviors.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Roedores , Animais , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Ultrassonografia , Movimento (Física) , Cognição
2.
bioRxiv ; 2023 Dec 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38106107

RESUMO

Active sensing is a behavioral strategy for exploring the environment. In this study, we show that contact vocal behaviors can be an active sensing mechanism that uses sampling to gain information about the social environment, in particular, the vocal behavior of others. With a focus on the realtime vocal interactions of marmoset monkeys, we contrast active sampling to a vocal accommodation framework in which vocalizations are adjusted simply to maximize responses. We conducted simulations of a vocal accommodation and an active sampling policy and compared them with real vocal exchange data. Our findings support active sampling as the best model for marmoset monkey vocal exchanges. In some cases, the active sampling model was even able to predict the distribution of vocal durations for individuals. These results suggest a new function for primate vocal interactions in which they are used by animals to seek information from social environments.

3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(39): e2201194119, 2022 09 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36122243

RESUMO

The brain continuously coordinates skeletomuscular movements with internal physiological states like arousal, but how is this coordination achieved? One possibility is that the brain simply reacts to changes in external and/or internal signals. Another possibility is that it is actively coordinating both external and internal activities. We used functional ultrasound imaging to capture a large medial section of the brain, including multiple cortical and subcortical areas, in marmoset monkeys while monitoring their spontaneous movements and cardiac activity. By analyzing the causal ordering of these different time series, we found that information flowing from the brain to movements and heart-rate fluctuations were significantly greater than in the opposite direction. The brain areas involved in this external versus internal coordination were spatially distinct, but also extensively interconnected. Temporally, the brain alternated between network states for this regulation. These findings suggest that the brain's dynamics actively and efficiently coordinate motor behavior with internal physiology.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Callithrix , Movimento , Animais , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Callithrix/fisiologia , Frequência Cardíaca , Movimento/fisiologia
4.
Elife ; 112022 07 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35880740

RESUMO

Human and non-human primates produce rhythmical sounds as soon as they are born. These early vocalizations are important for soliciting the attention of caregivers. How they develop remains a mystery. The orofacial movements necessary for producing these vocalizations have distinct spatiotemporal signatures. Therefore, their development could potentially be tracked over the course of prenatal life. We densely and longitudinally sampled fetal head and orofacial movements in marmoset monkeys using ultrasound imaging. We show that orofacial movements necessary for producing rhythmical vocalizations differentiate from a larger movement pattern that includes the entire head. We also show that signature features of marmoset infant contact calls emerge prenatally as a distinct pattern of orofacial movements. Our results establish that aspects of the sensorimotor development necessary for vocalizing occur prenatally, even before the production of sound.


Much like human babies, newborn monkeys cry and coo to get their caregiver's attention. They all produce these sounds in the same way. They push air from the lungs to vibrate the vocal cords, and adjust the movement of their jaws, lips, tongue and other muscles to create different kinds of sounds. Ultrasounds show that human fetuses begin making crying-like mouth movements during the last trimester of pregnancy. Yet the prenatal development of this crucial skill remains unclear, as most studies of early primate vocalization take place after birth. To explore this question, Narayanan et al. focused on a small species of monkeys known as marmosets. Regular ultrasounds were performed on four pregnant marmosets, starting on the first day the fetuses' faces became visible and ending the day before delivery. The developing marmosets acquired the ability to independently move their mouth from their head over time, a skill crucial for feeding and vocalizing. By the end of pregnancy, a subset of fetal mouth movements were nearly identical to those produced when baby marmosets call for their caregivers after birth. Human ultrasound studies are needed to confirm whether vocal development follows a similar trajectory in our species.This is likely given the developmental similarities between both species. If so, work in marmosets could be helpful to understand how conditions such as cerebral palsy interfere with this process, and to potentially develop early interventions.


Assuntos
Callithrix , Vocalização Animal , Animais , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Som
5.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 18(6): e1010173, 2022 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35696441

RESUMO

Evolution and development are typically characterized as the outcomes of gradual changes, but sometimes (states of equilibrium can be punctuated by sudden change. Here, we studied the early vocal development of three different mammals: common marmoset monkeys, Egyptian fruit bats, and humans. Consistent with the notion of punctuated equilibria, we found that all three species undergo at least one sudden transition in the acoustics of their developing vocalizations. To understand the mechanism, we modeled different developmental landscapes. We found that the transition was best described as a shift in the balance of two vocalization landscapes. We show that the natural dynamics of these two landscapes are consistent with the dynamics of energy expenditure and information transmission. By using them as constraints for each species, we predicted the differences in transition timing from immature to mature vocalizations. Using marmoset monkeys, we were able to manipulate both infant energy expenditure (vocalizing in an environment with lighter air) and information transmission (closed-loop contingent parental vocal playback). These experiments support the importance of energy and information in leading to punctuated equilibrium states of vocal development.


Assuntos
Quirópteros , Voz , Acústica , Animais , Callithrix , Humanos , Vocalização Animal
6.
Curr Biol ; 30(24): 5026-5032.e3, 2020 12 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33065007

RESUMO

The domestication syndrome refers to a set of traits that are the by-products of artificial selection for increased tolerance toward humans [1-3]. One hypothesis is that some species, like humans and bonobos, "self-domesticated" and have been under selection for that same suite of domesticated phenotypes [4-8]. However, the evidence for this has been largely circumstantial. Here, we provide evidence that, in marmoset monkeys, the size of a domestication phenotype-a white facial fur patch-is linked to their degree of affiliative vocal responding. During development, the amount of parental vocal feedback experienced influences the rate of growth of this facial white patch, and this suggests a mechanistic link between the two phenotypes, possibly via neural crest cells. Our study provides evidence for links between vocal behavior and the development of morphological phenotypes associated with domestication in a nonhuman primate.


Assuntos
Callithrix/fisiologia , Domesticação , Fenótipo , Vocalização Animal/fisiologia , Animais , Face/fisiologia , Feminino , Masculino , Pigmentação/fisiologia
7.
Nat Commun ; 11(1): 1096, 2020 Feb 24.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32094328

RESUMO

An amendment to this paper has been published and can be accessed via a link at the top of the paper.

8.
Nat Commun ; 10(1): 4592, 2019 10 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31597928

RESUMO

Across vertebrates, progressive changes in vocal behavior during postnatal development are typically attributed solely to developing neural circuits. How the changing body influences vocal development remains unknown. Here we show that state changes in the contact vocalizations of infant marmoset monkeys, which transition from noisy, low frequency cries to tonal, higher pitched vocalizations in adults, are caused partially by laryngeal development. Combining analyses of natural vocalizations, motorized excised larynx experiments, tensile material tests and high-speed imaging, we show that vocal state transition occurs via a sound source switch from vocal folds to apical vocal membranes, producing louder vocalizations with higher efficiency. We show with an empirically based model of descending motor control how neural circuits could interact with changing laryngeal dynamics, leading to adaptive vocal development. Our results emphasize the importance of embodied approaches to vocal development, where exploiting biomechanical consequences of changing material properties can simplify motor control, reducing the computational load on the developing brain.


Assuntos
Callithrix/fisiologia , Laringe/fisiologia , Prega Vocal/fisiologia , Vocalização Animal/fisiologia , Algoritmos , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Callithrix/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Feminino , Laringe/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Ruído , Som , Prega Vocal/crescimento & desenvolvimento
9.
Elife ; 82019 07 16.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31310236

RESUMO

In adult animals, movement and vocalizations are coordinated, sometimes facilitating, and at other times inhibiting, each other. What is missing is how these different domains of motor control become coordinated over the course of development. We investigated how postural-locomotor behaviors may influence vocal development, and the role played by physiological arousal during their interactions. Using infant marmoset monkeys, we densely sampled vocal, postural and locomotor behaviors and estimated arousal fluctuations from electrocardiographic measures of heart rate. We found that vocalizations matured sooner than postural and locomotor skills, and that vocal-locomotor coordination improved with age and during elevated arousal levels. These results suggest that postural-locomotor maturity is not required for vocal development to occur, and that infants gradually improve coordination between vocalizations and body movement through a process that may be facilitated by arousal level changes.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Sistema Nervoso Autônomo/fisiologia , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Movimento , Desempenho Psicomotor , Vocalização Animal , Animais , Callithrix , Eletrocardiografia , Feminino , Frequência Cardíaca , Masculino
10.
Curr Biol ; 29(4): R125-R127, 2019 02 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30779900

RESUMO

Animals modulate their behavior by interacting with others. Nevertheless, popular theories of vocal learning frequently overlook the role of ongoing social interactions. New research suggests that a social feedback loop between young male zebra finches and adult females guides the process of song learning.


Assuntos
Tentilhões , Animais , Retroalimentação , Feminino , Aprendizagem , Masculino , Reforço Social , Vocalização Animal
11.
Curr Biol ; 28(22): R1298-R1300, 2018 11 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30458147

RESUMO

Coordinated behaviors are the hallmark of animal societies. General mechanisms for the emergence of social group dynamics are still unknown. New research suggests that a vocal feedback loop explains the appearance of coordinated vocal exchanges in large groups of meerkats.


Assuntos
Herpestidae , Comunicação Animal , Animais
12.
Curr Biol ; 27(12): 1844-1852.e6, 2017 Jun 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28552359

RESUMO

For over half a century now, primate vocalizations have been thought to undergo little or no experience-dependent acoustic changes during development [1]. If any changes are apparent, then they are routinely (and quite reasonably) attributed to the passive consequences of growth. Indeed, previous experiments on squirrel monkeys and macaque monkeys showed that social isolation [2, 3], deafness [2], cross-fostering [4] and parental absence [5] have little or no effect on vocal development. Here, we explicitly test in marmoset monkeys-a very vocal and cooperatively breeding species [6]-whether the transformation of immature into mature contact calls by infants is influenced by contingent parental vocal feedback. Using a closed-loop design, we experimentally provided more versus less contingent vocal feedback to twin infant marmoset monkeys over their first 2 months of life, the interval during which their contact calls transform from noisy, immature calls to tonal adult-like "phee" calls [7, 8]. Infants who received more contingent feedback had a faster rate of vocal development, producing mature-sounding contact calls earlier than the other twin. The differential rate of vocal development was not linked to genetics, perinatal experience, or body growth; nor did the amount of contingency influence the overall rate of spontaneous vocal production. Thus, we provide the first experimental evidence for production-related vocal learning during the development of a nonhuman primate.


Assuntos
Callithrix/fisiologia , Callithrix/psicologia , Aprendizagem , Reforço Social , Vocalização Animal , Animais , Callithrix/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Feminino , Masculino
13.
Front Neurosci ; 11: 66, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28261045

RESUMO

One major problem in neuroscience is the comparison of functional brain networks of different populations, e.g., distinguishing the networks of controls and patients. Traditional algorithms are based on search for isomorphism between networks, assuming that they are deterministic. However, biological networks present randomness that cannot be well modeled by those algorithms. For instance, functional brain networks of distinct subjects of the same population can be different due to individual characteristics. Moreover, networks of subjects from different populations can be generated through the same stochastic process. Thus, a better hypothesis is that networks are generated by random processes. In this case, subjects from the same group are samples from the same random process, whereas subjects from different groups are generated by distinct processes. Using this idea, we developed a statistical test called ANOGVA to test whether two or more populations of graphs are generated by the same random graph model. Our simulations' results demonstrate that we can precisely control the rate of false positives and that the test is powerful to discriminate random graphs generated by different models and parameters. The method also showed to be robust for unbalanced data. As an example, we applied ANOGVA to an fMRI dataset composed of controls and patients diagnosed with autism or Asperger. ANOGVA identified the cerebellar functional sub-network as statistically different between controls and autism (p < 0.001).

14.
Front Neurosci ; 11: 16, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28174516

RESUMO

Understanding how brain activities cluster can help in the diagnosis of neuropsychological disorders. Thus, it is important to be able to identify alterations in the clustering structure of functional brain networks. Here, we provide an R implementation of Analysis of Cluster Variability (ANOCVA), which statistically tests (1) whether a set of brain regions of interest (ROI) are equally clustered between two or more populations and (2) whether the contribution of each ROI to the differences in clustering is significant. To illustrate the usefulness of our method and software, we apply the R package in a large functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) dataset composed of 896 individuals (529 controls and 285 diagnosed with ASD-autism spectrum disorder) collected by the ABIDE (The Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange) Consortium. Our analysis show that the clustering structure of controls and ASD subjects are different (p < 0.001) and that specific brain regions distributed in the frontotemporal, sensorimotor, visual, cerebellar, and brainstem systems significantly contributed (p < 0.05) to this differential clustering. These findings suggest an atypical organization of domain-specific function brain modules in ASD.

15.
Elife ; 62017 01 16.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28092262

RESUMO

Vocal development is the adaptive coordination of the vocal apparatus, muscles, the nervous system, and social interaction. Here, we use a quantitative framework based on optimal control theory and Waddington's landscape metaphor to provide an integrated view of this process. With a biomechanical model of the marmoset monkey vocal apparatus and behavioral developmental data, we show that only the combination of the developing vocal tract, vocal apparatus muscles and nervous system can fully account for the patterns of vocal development. Together, these elements influence the shape of the monkeys' vocal developmental landscape, tilting, rotating or shifting it in different ways. We can thus use this framework to make quantitative predictions regarding how interfering factors or experimental perturbations can change the landscape within a species, or to explain comparative differences in vocal development across species.


Assuntos
Callithrix/fisiologia , Vocalização Animal , Animais , Modelos Biológicos
16.
J Neurophysiol ; 116(2): 753-64, 2016 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27250909

RESUMO

Vocal production is the result of interacting cognitive and autonomic processes. Despite claims that changes in one interoceptive state (arousal) govern primate vocalizations, we know very little about how it influences their likelihood and timing. In this study we investigated the role of arousal during naturally occurring vocal production in marmoset monkeys. Throughout each session, naturally occurring contact calls are produced more quickly, and with greater probability, during higher levels of arousal, as measured by heart rate. On average, we observed a steady increase in heart rate 23 s before the production of a call. Following call production, there is a sharp and steep cardiac deceleration lasting ∼8 s. The dynamics of cardiac fluctuations around a vocalization cannot be completely predicted by the animal's respiration or movement. Moreover, the timing of vocal production was tightly correlated to the phase of a 0.1-Hz autonomic nervous system rhythm known as the Mayer wave. Finally, a compilation of the state space of arousal dynamics during vocalization illustrated that perturbations to the resting state space increase the likelihood of a call occurring. Together, these data suggest that arousal dynamics are critical for spontaneous primate vocal production, not only as a robust predictor of the likelihood of vocal onset but also as scaffolding on which behavior can unfold.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Callithrix/fisiologia , Dinâmica não Linear , Vocalização Animal/fisiologia , Animais , Eletromiografia , Potencial Evocado Motor/fisiologia , Feminino , Frequência Cardíaca/fisiologia , Masculino , Movimento/fisiologia , Probabilidade , Respiração , Análise Espectral
17.
IEEE Trans Biomed Eng ; 63(12): 2450-2460, 2016 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27076053

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To present a unified mathematical derivation of the frequency-dependent asymptotic behavior of the three main forms of directed transfer function (DTF). METHODS: A synthesis of the results (proved in an extended Appendix) is followed by a series of Monte Carlo simulations of representative examples. RESULTS: DTF estimators are asymptotically normal when the true values are different from zero. Under the null hypothesis H0: DTF=0, the estimator is distributed as a linear combination of independent χ21 variables. CONCLUSIONS: Null DTF rejection is shown to be achievable with identical performance irrespective of which DTF form is adopted. SIGNIFICANCE: Together with recent allied partial directed coherence results, this paper rounds up connectivity inference tools for a class of frequency-domain connectivity estimators.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Estatísticos , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Humanos , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador
18.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 371(1693)2016 May 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27069047

RESUMO

In humans, vocal turn-taking is a ubiquitous form of social interaction. It is a communication system that exhibits the properties of a dynamical system: two individuals become coupled to each other via acoustic exchanges and mutually affect each other. Human turn-taking develops during the first year of life. We investigated the development of vocal turn-taking in infant marmoset monkeys, a New World species whose adult vocal behaviour exhibits the same universal features of human turn-taking. We find that marmoset infants undergo the same trajectory of change for vocal turn-taking as humans, and do so during the same life-history stage. Our data show that turn-taking by marmoset infants depends on the development of self-monitoring, and that contingent parental calls elicit more mature-sounding calls from infants. As in humans, there was no evidence that parental feedback affects the rate of turn-taking maturation. We conclude that vocal turn-taking by marmoset monkeys and humans is an instance of convergent evolution, possibly as a result of pressures on both species to adopt a cooperative breeding strategy and increase volubility.


Assuntos
Callithrix/fisiologia , Poder Familiar , Vocalização Animal , Animais , Callithrix/psicologia , Feminino , Masculino
19.
J Neurophysiol ; 114(1): 274-83, 2015 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25925323

RESUMO

Humans adjust speech amplitude as a function of distance from a listener; we do so in a manner that would compensate for such distance. This ability is presumed to be the product of high-level sociocognitive skills. Nonhuman primates are thought to lack such socially related flexibility in vocal production. Using predictions from a simple arousal-based model whereby vocal feedback from a conspecific modulates the drive to produce a vocalization, we tested whether another primate exhibits this type of cooperative vocal control. We conducted a playback experiment with marmoset monkeys and simulated "far-away" and "nearby" conspecifics using contact calls that differed in sound intensity. We found that marmoset monkeys increased the amplitude of their contact calls and produced such calls with shorter response latencies toward more distant conspecifics. The same was not true in response to changing levels of background noise. To account for how simulated conspecific distance can change both the amplitude and timing of vocal responses, we developed a model that incorporates dynamic interactions between the auditory system and limbic "drive" systems. Overall, our data show that, like humans, marmoset monkeys cooperatively control the acoustics of their vocalizations according to changes in listener distance, increasing the likelihood that a conspecific will hear their call. However, we propose that such cooperative vocal control is a system property that does not necessitate any particularly advanced sociocognitive skill. At least in marmosets, this vocal control can be parsimoniously explained by the regulation of arousal states across two interacting individuals via vocal feedback.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Callithrix/psicologia , Retroalimentação Psicológica , Comportamento Social , Vocalização Animal , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Acústica , Animais , Retroalimentação Fisiológica , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Ruído , Testes Psicológicos , Localização de Som , Fatores de Tempo
20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26737509

RESUMO

In this work we show the asymptotic behavior of information partial directed coherence estimator via the Monte Carlo simulation of a particular toy model taken from the literature. We show that the control of false positive rate tends to the chosen significance level if detection decision is made at specific frequency values.


Assuntos
Simulação por Computador , Modelos Teóricos , Método de Monte Carlo
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...