Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 11 de 11
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Hered ; 88(4): 294-304, 1997.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9262011

RESUMO

We present five case studies highlighting the effects of habitat fragmentation on the genetic structure of small mammal populations. The studies reflect different spatial scales and components of genetic variation. In marginal and central populations of Sigmodon hispidus we found less allozymic variation within the marginal population, whereas patterns of morphological variability were the converse. In the rice rat (Oryzomys spp.), nucleotide diversity in mtDNA was similar in an island population in the Florida Keys to mainland populations in the Everglades. This observation contrasts with insular vole populations (Microtus spp.), where isolation on islands results in genetic structuring. Temporal changes in abundance in mainland populations had no effects on genetic differentiation (FST values) because subpopulations did not experience bottlenecks. In an experimentally fragmented landscape, fragmentation influenced demographic processes but not genetic structure. We conclude that (1) with extreme fragmentation, small mammal populations become depauperate of genetic variation and differentiate genetically; (2) different components of genetic variation lead to different genetic structuring; (3) spatial and temporal scales should both be considered when examining genetic structure of populations; (4) demographic and ecological processes are more likely influenced by fragmentation than genetic structure; and (5) there is an interaction between demographic processes and genetic structure.


Assuntos
Variação Genética , Mamíferos/genética , Animais , Arvicolinae/genética , Ecologia , Isoenzimas/genética , Ratos , Sigmodontinae/genética
2.
Physiol Behav ; 59(1): 205-8, 1996 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8848484

RESUMO

To determine whether neighbor familiarity can affect reproduction, we studied the relationship between familiarity, odor preference, and plasma estradiol levels in the meadow vole, Microtus pennsylvanicus. Bedding was switched between pairs of female meadow voles for 2 wk to allow them to develop olfactory familiarity. When familiarization was complete animals were reexposed, after 24 h of no exposure to conspecific odors, to either the bedding of the familiar female or to the bedding of a new, unfamiliar female. Voles exposed to the bedding of unfamiliar females experienced a dramatic reversal in odor preference and failed to orient towards male odors. This behavioral change was accompanied by a significant decrease in plasma estradiol levels. These changes suggest that exposure to unfamiliar conspecifics may result in reproductive inhibition. Excessive contact between unfamiliar females in the field may be indicative of environmental conditions unfavorable to breeding.


Assuntos
Arvicolinae/fisiologia , Estradiol/sangue , Odorantes , Comportamento Sexual Animal/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Masculino , Radioimunoensaio
4.
Health Phys ; 49(6): 1217-28, 1985 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3908406

RESUMO

The retention of 14 radionuclides was examined in meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) maintained under laboratory conditions. Individuals were monitored by whole-body gamma spectroscopy for up to 62 days following intraperitoneal injection with single radionuclides. One- and two-compartment exponential retention curves were fit to the data using nonlinear least-squares regression techniques. Slow-component biological half-lives in meadow voles were determined to be 346.6 days (46Sc), 31.5 days (51Cr), 49.5 days (54Mn), 115.5 days (59Fe), 3.4 days (58Co), 3.5 days (60Co), 57.8 days (65Zn), 26.7 days (75Se), 33 days (85Sr), 6.4 days (86Rb), 231 days (88Y), 30.1 days (95Nb), 99 days (110mAg), and 1.9 days (125Sb). No significant relationship was found between biological half-life and weight, season or amount injected for any of the radionuclides. The considerable variation in half-life values for each radionuclide was thought to be due to a number of behavioral and physiological variables. Problems in comparing the results to those of other experiments, and in extending them to field conditions, are discussed.


Assuntos
Arvicolinae/metabolismo , Radioisótopos/metabolismo , Animais , Monitoramento Ambiental , Feminino , Meia-Vida , Masculino
10.
Science ; 179(4068): 35-41, 1973 Jan 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-4734149

RESUMO

We conclude that population fluctuations in Microtus in southern Indiana are produced by a syndrome of changes in birth and death rates similar to that found in other species of voles and lemmings. The mechanisms which cause the changes in birth and death rates are demolished by fencing the population so that no dispersal can occur. Dispersal thus seems critical for population regulation in Microtus. Because most dispersal occurs during the increase phase of the population cycle and there is little dispersal during the decline phase, dispersal is not directly related to population density. Hence the quality of dispersing animals must be important, and we have found one case of increased dispersal tendency by one genotype. The failure of population regulation of Microtus in enclosed areas requires an explanation by any hypothesis attempting to explain population cycles in small rodents. It might be suggested that the fence changed the predation pressure on the enclosed populations. However, the fence was only 2 feet (0.6 meter) high and did not stop the entrance of foxes, weasels, shrews, or avian predators. A striking feature was that the habitat in the enclosures quickly recovered from complete devastation by the start of the spring growing season. Obviously the habitat and food quality were sufficient to support Microtus populations of abnormally high densities, and recovery of the habitat was sufficiently quick that the introduction of new animals to these enclosed areas resulted in another population explosion. Finally, hypotheses of population regulation by social stress must account for the finding that Microtus can exist at densities several times greater than normal without "stress" taking an obvious toll. We hypothesize that the prevention of dispersal changes the quality of the populations in the enclosures in comparison to those outside the fence. Voles forced to remain in an overcrowded fenced population do not suffer high mortality rates and continue to reproduce at abnormally high densities until starvation overtakes them. The initial behavioral interactions associated with crowding do not seem sufficient to cause voles to die in situ. What happens to animals during the population decline? Our studies have not answered this question. The animals did not appear to disperse, but it is possible that the method we used to measure dispersal (movement into a vacant habitat) missed a large segment of dispersing voles which did not remain in the vacant area but kept on moving. Perhaps the dispersal during the increase phase of the population cycle is a colonization type of dispersal, and the animals taking part in it are likely to stay in a new habitat, while during the population decline dispersal is a pathological response to high density, and the animals are not attracted to settling even in a vacant habitat. The alternative to this suggestion is that animals are dying in situ during the decline because of physiological or genetically determined behavioral stress. Thus the fencing of a population prevents the change in rates of survival and reproduction, from high rates in the increase phase to low rates in the decline phase, and the fenced populations resemble "mouse plagues." A possible explanation is that the differential dispersal of animals during the phase of increase causes the quality of the voles remaining at peak densities in wild populations to be different from the quality of voles at much higher densities in enclosures. Increased sensitivity to density in Microtus could cause the decline of wild populations at densities lower than those reached by fenced populations in which selection through dispersal has been prevented. Fencing might also alter the social interactions among Microtus in other ways that are not understood. The analysis of colonizing species by MacArthur and Wilson (27) can be applied to our studies of dispersal in populations of Microtus. Groups of organisms with good dispersal and colonizing ability are called r strategists because they have high reproductive potential and are able to exploit a new environment rapidly. Dispersing voles seem to be r strategists. Young females in breeding condition were over-represented in dispersing female Microtus (17). The Tf(C)/Tf(E) females, which were more common among dispersers during the phase of population increase (Fig. 6), also have a slight reproductive advantage over the other Tf genotypes (19). Thus in Microtus populations the animals with the highest reproductive potential, the r strategists, are dispersing. The segment of the population which remains behind after the selection-via-dispersal are those individuals which are less influenced by increasing population densities. These are the individuals which maximize use of the habitat, the K strategists in MacArthur and Wilson's terminology, or voles selected for spacing behavior. Thus we can describe population cycles in Microtus in the same theoretical framework as colonizing species on islands. Our work on Microtus is consistent with the hypothesis of genetic and behavioral effects proposed by Chitty (6) (Fig. 7) in that it shows both behavioral differences in males during the phases of population fluctuation and periods of strong genetic selection. The greatest gaps in our knowledge are in the area of genetic-behavioral interactions which are most difficult to measure. We have no information on the heritability of aggressive behavior in voles. The pathways by which behavioral events are translated into physiological changes which affect reproduction and growth have been carefully analyzed by Christian and his associates (28) for rodents in laboratory situations, but the application of these findings to the complex field events described above remains to be done. Several experiments are suggested by our work. First, other populations of other rodent species should increase to abnormal densities if enclosed in a large fenced area (29). We need to find situations in which this prediction is not fulfilled. Island populations may be an important source of material for such an experiment (30). Second, if one-way exit doors were provided from a fenced area, normal population regulation through dispersal should occur. This experiment would provide another method by which dispersers could be identified. Third, if dispersal were prevented after a population reached peak densities, a normal decline phase should occur. This prediction is based on the assumption that dispersal during the increase phase is sufficient to ensure the decline phase 1 or 2 years later. All these experiments are concerned with the dispersal factor, and our work on Microtus can be summarized by the admonition: study dispersal.


Assuntos
Demografia , Genética Populacional , Roedores , Agressão , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Feminino , Frequência do Gene , Variação Genética , Humanos , Indiana , Masculino , Polimorfismo Genético , Reprodução , Estações do Ano , Seleção Genética
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...