Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 8 de 8
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
R Soc Open Sci ; 7(5): 192240, 2020 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32537212

RESUMO

Through the mechanisms of observation, imitation and teaching, young children readily pick up the tool using behaviours of their culture. However, little is known about the baseline abilities of children's tool use: what they might be capable of inventing on their own in the absence of socially provided information. It has been shown that children can spontaneously invent 11 of 12 candidate tool using behaviours observed within the foraging behaviours of wild non-human apes (Reindl et al. 2016 Proc. R. Soc. B 283, 20152402. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.2402)). However, no investigations to date have examined how tool use invention in children might vary across cultural contexts. The current study investigated the levels of spontaneous tool use invention in 2- to 5-year-old children from San Bushmen communities in South Africa and children in a large city in Australia on the same 12 candidate problem-solving tasks. Children in both cultural contexts correctly invented all 12 candidate tool using behaviours, suggesting that these behaviours are within the general cognitive and physical capacities of human children and can be produced in the absence of direct social learning mechanisms such as teaching or observation. Children in both cultures were more likely to invent those tool behaviours more frequently observed in great ape populations than those less frequently observed, suggesting there is similarity in the level of difficulty of invention across these behaviours for all great ape species. However, children in the Australian sample invented tool behaviours and succeeded on the tasks more often than did the Bushmen children, highlighting that aspects of a child's social or cultural environment may influence the rates of their tool use invention on such task sets, even when direct social information is absent.

2.
Dev Psychol ; 55(4): 877-889, 2019 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30640502

RESUMO

Prior research suggests that human children lack an aptitude for tool innovation. However, children's tool making must be explored across a broader range of tasks and across diverse cultural contexts before we can conclude that they are genuinely poor tool innovators. To this end, we investigated children's ability to independently construct 3 new tools using distinct actions: adding, subtracting, and reshaping. We tested 422 children across a broad age range from 5 geographic locations across South Africa (N = 126), Vanuatu (N = 190), and Australia (N = 106), which varied in their levels of exposure to Westernized culture. Children were shown a horizontal, transparent tube that had a sticker in its middle. Children were sequentially given each incomplete tool, which when accurately constructed could be used to push the sticker out of the tube. As predicted, older children were better at performing the innovation tasks than younger children across all cultures and innovation actions. We also found evidence for cultural variation: While all non-Western groups performed similarly, the Western group of children innovated at higher rates. However, children who did not innovate often adopted alternate methods when using the tools that also led to success. This suggests that children's innovation levels are influenced by the cultural environment, and highlights the flexibility inherent in human children's tool use. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Criatividade , Comparação Transcultural , Difusão de Inovações , Resolução de Problemas , Fatores Etários , Austrália , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , África do Sul , Vanuatu
3.
Child Dev ; 90(1): 51-61, 2019 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29737036

RESUMO

This study examined future-oriented behavior in children (3-6 years; N = 193) from three diverse societies-one industrialized Western city and two small, geographically isolated communities. Children had the opportunity to prepare for two alternative versions of an immediate future event over six trials. Some 3-year-olds from all cultures demonstrated competence, and a majority of the oldest children from each culture prepared for both future possibilities on every trial. Although there were some cultural differences in the youngest age groups that approached ceiling performance, the overall results indicate that children across these communities become able to prepare for alternative futures during early childhood. This acquisition period is therefore not contingent on Western upbringing, and may instead indicate normal cognitive maturation.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Comparação Transcultural , Pensamento/fisiologia , Austrália/etnologia , Criança , Comportamento Infantil/etnologia , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
4.
Child Dev ; 87(3): 795-806, 2016 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27189406

RESUMO

This study explored how overimitation and collaboration interact in 3- to 6-year-old children in Westernized (N = 48 in Experiment 1; N = 26 in Experiment 2) and Indigenous Australian communities (N = 26 in Experiment 2). Whether working in pairs or on their own rates of overimitation did not differ. However, when the causal functions of modeled actions were unclear, the Indigenous Australian children collaborated at enhanced rates compared to the Western children. When the causal role of witnessed actions was identifiable, collaboration rates were correlated with production of causally unnecessary actions, but in the Indigenous Australian children only. This study highlights how children employ imitation and collaboration when acquiring new skills and how the latter can be influenced by task structure and cultural background.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil/etnologia , Comportamento Cooperativo , Comparação Transcultural , Comportamento Imitativo , Austrália/etnologia , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Havaiano Nativo ou Outro Ilhéu do Pacífico/etnologia
5.
Child Dev ; 85(6): 2169-84, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25040582

RESUMO

Children often "overimitate," comprehensively copying others' actions despite manifest perceptual cues to their causal ineffectuality. The inflexibility of this behavior renders its adaptive significance difficult to apprehend. This study explored the boundaries of overimitation in 3- to 6-year-old children of three distinct cultures: Westernized, urban Australians (N = 64 in Experiment 1; N = 19 in Experiment 2) and remote communities of South African Bushmen (N = 64) and Australian Aborigines (N = 19). Children overimitated at high frequency in all communities and generalized what they had learned about techniques and object affordances from one object to another. Overimitation thus provides a powerful means of acquiring and flexibly deploying cultural knowledge. The potency of such social learning was also documented compared to opportunities for exploration and practice.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil/etnologia , Comportamento Imitativo , Grupos Populacionais/etnologia , Transferência de Experiência , Angola/etnologia , Austrália/etnologia , População Negra/etnologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Namíbia/etnologia , Havaiano Nativo ou Outro Ilhéu do Pacífico/etnologia , África do Sul/etnologia , População Branca/etnologia
6.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 126: 384-94, 2014 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25014272

RESUMO

A capacity for constructing new tools, or using old tools in new ways, to solve novel problems is a core feature of what it means to be human. Yet current evidence suggests that young children are surprisingly poor at innovating tools. However, all studies of tool innovation to date have been conducted with children from comparatively privileged Western backgrounds. This raises questions as to whether or not previously documented tool innovation failure is culturally and economically specific. In the current study, thus, we explored the innovation capacities of children from Westernized urban backgrounds and from remote communities of South African Bushmen. Consistent with past research, we found tool innovation to occur at extremely low rates and that cultural background had no bearing on this. The current study is the first to empirically test tool innovation in children from non-Western backgrounds, with our data being consistent with the view that despite its key role in human evolution, a capacity for innovation in tool making remains remarkably undeveloped during early childhood.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil/etnologia , Comparação Transcultural , Resolução de Problemas , População Negra/etnologia , População Negra/psicologia , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Grupos Populacionais , África do Sul , População Branca/etnologia , População Branca/psicologia
7.
Psychol Sci ; 21(5): 729-36, 2010 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20483853

RESUMO

Children are surrounded by objects that they must learn to use. One of the most efficient ways children do this is by imitation. Recent work has shown that, in contrast to nonhuman primates, human children focus more on reproducing the specific actions used than on achieving actual outcomes when learning by imitating. From 18 months of age, children will routinely copy even arbitrary and unnecessary actions. This puzzling behavior is called overimitation. By documenting similarities exhibited by children from a large, industrialized city and children from remote Bushman communities in southern Africa, we provide here the first indication that overimitation may be a universal human trait. We also show that overimitation is unaffected by the age of the child, differences in the testing environment, or familiarity with the demonstrating adult. Furthermore, we argue that, although seemingly maladaptive, overimitation reflects an evolutionary adaptation that is fundamental to the development and transmission of human culture.


Assuntos
Aculturação , Comparação Transcultural , Comportamento Imitativo , Grupos Populacionais/psicologia , População Rural , Meio Social , Botsuana , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Queensland , África do Sul , População Urbana
8.
Afr J AIDS Res ; 8(3): 311-20, 2009 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25864546

RESUMO

University students form an important constituency in interventions against HIV and AIDS. The majority of university students are between ages 18 and 30 years, which, according to recent surveys, is the age category at the highest risk of HIV infection. Even though there is currently no comprehensive statistical data on the HIV prevalence at South African institutions of higher learning, a number of studies have noted increasing AIDS-related deaths and sicknesses among students. This highlights the need for effective intervention against HIV infections within this community. Condom use remains the most effective intervention against HIV infection within sexually active populations. This paper examines perceptions of public-sector condoms and their impact on condom use among university students, based on the findings of research conducted at three universities in KwaZulu-Natal Province, South Africa. Study findings indicate that public-sector condoms are perceived as ineffective, smelly and even 'infectious' and are widely seen to be of lower status as compared to the commercial brands. These perceptions were found to influence condom use as some students preferred to engage in unprotected sex rather than use public-sector condoms. The paper highlights the need for communication programmes to demystify the misconceptions surrounding public-sector condoms and to provide reassurance of the quality of such condoms.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...