Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 83
Filtrar
1.
Doc Ophthalmol ; 149(1): 1-10, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38955959

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The electroretinogram (ERG) is the summed response from all levels of the retinal processing of light, and exhibits several profound nonlinearities in the underlying processing pathways. Accurate computational models of the ERG are important, both for understanding the multifold processes of light transduction to ecologically useful signals by the retina, and for their diagnostic capabilities for the identification and characterization of retinal disease mechanisms. There are, however, very few computational models of the ERG waveform, and none that account for the full extent of its features over time. METHODS: This study takes the neuroanalytic approach to modeling the ERG waveform, defined as a computational model based on the main features of the transmitter kinetics of the retinal neurons. RESULTS: The present neuroanalytic model of the human rod ERG is elaborated from the same general principles as that of Hood and Birch (Vis Neurosci 8(2):107-126, 1992), but incorporates the more recent understanding of the early nonlinear stages of ERG generation by Robson and Frishman (Prog Retinal Eye Res 39:1-22, 2014). As a result, it provides a substantially better match than previous models of rod responses in six different waveform features of the ERG flash intensity series on which the Hood and Birch model was based. CONCLUSION: The neuroanalytic approach extends previous models of the component waves of the ERG, and can be structured to provide an accurate characterization of the full timecourse of the ERG waveform. The approach thus holds promise for advancing the theoretical understanding of the retinal kinetics of the light response.


Assuntos
Simulação por Computador , Eletrorretinografia , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Bastonetes , Humanos , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Bastonetes/fisiologia , Cinética , Estimulação Luminosa
2.
Vision Res ; 221: 108422, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38718618

RESUMO

We used the psychophysical summation paradigm to reveal some spatial characteristics of the mechanism responsible for detecting a motion-defined visual target in central vision. There has been much previous work on spatial summation for motion detection and direction discrimination, but none has assessed it in terms of the velocity threshold or used velocity noise to provide a measure of the efficiency of the velocity processing mechanism. Motion-defined targets were centered within square fields of randomly selected gray levels. The motion was produced within the disk-shaped target region by shifting the pixels rightwards for 0.2 s. The uniform target motion was perturbed by Gaussian motion noise in horizontal strips of 16 pixels. Independent variables were field size, the diameter of the disk target, and the variance of an independent perturbation added to the (signed) velocity of each 16-pixel strip. The dependent variable was the threshold velocity for target detection. Velocity thresholds formed swoosh-shaped (descending, then ascending) functions of target diameter. Minimum values were obtained when targets subtended approximately 2 degrees of visual angle. The data were fit with a continuum of models, extending from the theoretically ideal observer through various inefficient and noisy refinements thereof. In particular, we introduce the concept of sparse sampling to account for the relative inefficiency of the velocity thresholds. The best fits were obtained from a model observer whose responses were determined by comparing the velocity profile of each stimulus with a limited set of sparsely sampled "DoG" templates, each of which is the product of a random binary array and the difference between two 2-D Gaussian density functions.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento , Estimulação Luminosa , Psicofísica , Limiar Sensorial , Humanos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto
3.
Vision Res ; 214: 108329, 2024 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37951053

RESUMO

Free-fusion stereograms are routinely used for demonstrating various stereoscopic effects. Yet, untrained observers find it challenging to perform this task. This study showed that only less than 1/3rd of sixty-one pre-presbyopic adults with normal binocular vision could successfully free-fuse random-dot image pairs and identify the stereoscopic shapes embedded in these patterns. Another one-third of participants performed the task with poor success rates, while the remaining could not perform the task. There was a clear dissociation of vergence and accommodative responses in participants who were successful with free-fusion, as recorded using a dynamic infrared eye tracker and photorefractor. Those in the unsuccessful cluster either showed strong vergence and accommodation or weak vergence and strong accommodation during the task. These response patterns, however, were specific to the free-fusion task because all these participants generated good convergence/accommodation to real-world targets and to conflicting vergence and accommodative demands stimulated with prisms or lenses. Task performance of the unsuccessful cluster also improved significantly following pharmacological paralysis of accommodation and reached the performance levels of the successful cluster. A minority of participants also appeared to progressively learn to dissociate one of the two directions of their vergence and accommodation crosslinks with repeated free-fusion trials. These results suggest that successful free-fusion might depend upon how well participants generate a combination of volitional and reflex vergence responses to large differences in disparity with conflicting static accommodative demands. Such responses would require that only one direction of the vergence-accommodation crosslinks be active at any given time. The sequence of near-responses could also be learnt through repeated trials to optimize task performance.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular , Convergência Ocular , Adulto , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Visão Binocular/fisiologia
4.
J Vis ; 23(7): 17, 2023 07 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37505916

RESUMO

To investigate the mechanisms underlying elongated spatial summation with a pattern-masking paradigm, we measured the contrast detection thresholds for elongated Gabor targets situated at 3° eccentricity to either the left or right of the fixation and elongated along an arc of the same radius to access homogeneous retinal sensitivity. The mask was a ring with a Gabor envelope of the same 3° center radius containing either a concentric (iso-orientation mask) or a radial (orthogonal mask) modulation. The task of the observer was to indicate whether the target in each trial was on the left or the right of the fixation. With orthogonal or low contrast iso-orientation masks, target thresholds first decreased with size with slope -1 on log-log coordinates until the target length reached 45' (specified as the half-height full-width of the Gabor envelope) and then further decreased according to a slope of -1/2, the latter being the signature of an ideal summation process. When the contrast of the iso-orientation mask was sufficiently high, however, the target thresholds, while still showing a -1 slope up to ∼10', asymptoted up to about 50' length, suggesting that the presence of the mask eliminated the ideal summation regime. Beyond about 50', the data approximated another -1 slope decrease in threshold, suggesting the existence of an extra-long channel that is not revealed by the conventional spatial summation paradigm. The full results could be explained by a divisive inhibition model, in which second-order filters sum responses across local oriented channels, combined with a single extra-long filter at least 300' in extent. In this model, the local filter response is given by the linear excitation of the local channels raised to a power, and scaled by divisive inhibition from all channels in the neighborhood. With the high-contrast iso-orientation masks, such divisive inhibition swamps the response to eliminate the ideal summation regime until the stimulus is long enough to activate the extra-long filter.


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Humanos , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica
5.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37256188

RESUMO

How is the cortical navigation network reorganized by the Likova Cognitive-Kinesthetic Navigation Training? We measured Granger-causal connectivity of the frontal-hippocampal-insular-retrosplenial-V1 network of cortical areas before and after this one-week training in the blind. Primarily top-down influences were seen during two tasks of drawing-from-memory (drawing complex maps and drawing the shortest path between designated map locations), with the dominant role being congruent influences from the egocentric insular to the allocentric spatial retrosplenial cortex and the amodal-spatial sketchpad of V1, with concomitant influences of the frontal cortex on these areas. After training, and during planning-from-memory of the best on-demand path, the hippocampus played a much stronger role, with the V1 sketchpad feeding information forward to the retrosplenial region. The inverse causal influences among these regions generally followed a recursive feedback model of the opposite pattern to a subset of congruent influences. Thus, this navigational network reorganized its pattern of causal influences with task demands and the navigation training, which produced marked enhancement of the navigational skills.

6.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 16: 875829, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35726280

RESUMO

This overview discusses the nature of perceptual illusions with particular reference to the theory that illusions represent the operation of a sensory code for which there is no meaningful ground truth against which the illusory percepts can be compared, and therefore there are no illusions as such. This view corresponds to the Bayesian theory that "illusions" reflect unusual aspects of the core strategies of adapting to the natural world, again implying that illusions are simply an information processing characteristic. Instead, it is argued that a more meaningful approach to the field that we call illusions is the Ebbinghaus approach of comparing the illusory percept with a ground truth that is directly verifiable as aberrant by the observer in the domain of the illusory phenomenology (as opposed to relying on the authority of other experts). This concept of direct verifiability not only provides an operational definition of "illusion"; it also makes their interactive observation more effective and informative as to the perceptual processes underlying the illusory appearance. An expanded version of Gregory's categorization of types of illusion is developed, and a range of classic and more recent illusions that illustrate the differences between these philosophical viewpoints is considered in detail. Such cases make it clear that the discrepancies from the measurable image structure cannot be simply regarded as idiosyncrasies of sensory coding, but are categorical exemplars of perceptual illusions. The widespread existence of such illusory percepts is indicative of the evolutionary limits of adaptive sensory coding.

7.
Curr Biol ; 32(11): 2556-2562.e2, 2022 06 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35508171

RESUMO

To camouflage themselves on the seafloor, European cuttlefish Sepia officinalis control the expression of about 30 pattern components to produce a range of body patterns.1 If each component were under independent control, cuttlefish could produce at least 230 patterns. To examine how cuttlefish deploy this vast potential, we recorded cuttlefish on seven experimental backgrounds, each designed to resemble a pattern component, and then compared their responses to predictions of two models of sensory control of component expression. The body pattern model proposes that cuttlefish integrate low-level sensory cues to categorize the background and co-ordinate component expression to produce a small number of overall body patterns.2-4 The feature matching model proposes that each component is expressed in response to one (or more) local visual features, and the overall pattern depends upon the combination of features in the background. Consistent with the feature matching model, six of the backgrounds elicited a specific set of one to four components, whereas the seventh elicited eleven components typical of a disruptive body pattern. This evidence suggests that both modes of control are important, and we suggest how they can be implemented by a recent hierarchical model of the cuttlefish motor system.5,6.


Assuntos
Decapodiformes , Sepia , Animais , Sinais (Psicologia) , Decapodiformes/fisiologia , Sepia/fisiologia , Percepção Visual
8.
Doc Ophthalmol ; 144(3): 179-190, 2022 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35445376

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Concussion-induced light sensitivity, or traumatic photalgia, is a lifelong debilitating problem for upwards of 50% of mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) cases, though of unknown etiology. We employed spectral analysis of electroretinographic (ERG) responses to assess retinal changes in mTBI as a function of the degree of photalgia. METHODS: The design was a case-control study of the changes in the ERG waveform as a function of level of light sensitivity in individuals who had suffered incidents of mild traumatic brain injury. The mTBI participants were categorized into non-, mild-, and severe-photalgic groups based on their spectral nociophysical settings. Light-adapted ERG responses were recorded from each eye for 200 ms on-off stimulation of three spectral colors (R:red, G:green, and B:blue) and their sum (W:white) at the highest pain-free intensity level for each participant. The requirement of controls for testing hypersensitive individuals at lower light levels was addressed by recording a full light intensity series in the control group. RESULTS: Both the b-wave and the photopic negative response (PhNR) were significantly reduced in the non-photalgic mTBI group relative to controls. In the photalgic groups, the main b-wave peak shifted to the timing of the rod b-wave, with reduced amplitude at the timing of the cone response. CONCLUSION: These results suggest the interpretation that the primary etiology of the painful light sensitivity in mTBI is release of the rod pathway from cone-mediated inhibition at high light levels, causing overactivation of the rod pathway.


Assuntos
Concussão Encefálica , Lesões Encefálicas Traumáticas , Lesões Encefálicas Traumáticas/diagnóstico , Lesões Encefálicas Traumáticas/etiologia , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Adaptação à Escuridão , Eletrorretinografia , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Fotofobia/diagnóstico , Fotofobia/etiologia , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones/fisiologia
9.
Front Psychol ; 13: 810780, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35282214

RESUMO

This study provides evidence supporting the operation of a novel cognitive process of a somatic seat of attention, or ego-center, whose somatic location is under voluntary control and that provides access to differential emotional resources. Attention has typically been studied in terms of what it is directed toward, but it can also be associated with a localized representation in the body image that is experienced as the source or seat of attention-an aspect that has previously only been studied by subjective techniques. Published studies of this phenomenon under terms such as egocenter or self-location suggest that the seat of attention can be situated in various ways within the experienced body, resulting in what are here referred to as different attentional stances. These studies also provide evidence that changes in attentional stance are associated with differences in cognitive skill, emotional temperament, self-construal, and social and moral attitudes, as well as with access to certain states of consciousness. In the present study, EEG results from multiple trials of each of 11 specific attentional stances confirmed that patterns of neural activity associated with the voluntarily control of attentional stances can be reliably measured, providing evidence for a differential neural substrate underlying the subjective location of the seat of attention. Additionally, brain activation patterns for the attentional stances showed strong correlations with EEG signatures associated with specific positive emotional states and with arousal, confirming that differential locations of the seat of attention can be objectively associated with different emotion states, as implied in previous literature. The ability to directly manage the seat of attention into various attentional stances holds substantial potential for facilitating access to specific cognitive and emotional resources in a new way.

10.
J Percept Imaging ; Human Vision and Electronic Imaging 2021: 1561-15610, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34734166

RESUMO

The history of cartography has been marked by the endless search for the perfect form for the representation of the information on a spherical surface manifold into the flat planar format of the printed page or computer screen. Dozens of cartographic formats have been proposed over the centuries from ancient Greek times to the present. This is an issue not just for the mapping of the globe, but in all fields of science where spherical entities are found. The perceptual and representational advantages and drawbacks of many of these formats are considered, particularly in the tension between a unified representation, which is always distorted in some dimension, and a minimally distorted representation, which can only be obtained by segmentation into sectorial patches. The use of these same formats for the mapping of spherical manifolds are evaluated, from quantum physics through the mapping of the brain to the large-scale representation of the cosmos.

11.
Entropy (Basel) ; 23(11)2021 Oct 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34828141

RESUMO

This paper considers three classes of analyses of the nature of consciousness: abstract theories of the functional organization of consciousness, and concrete proposals as to the neural substrate of consciousness, while providing a rationale for contesting non-neural and transcendental conceptualizations of consciousness. It indicates that abstract theories of the dynamic core of consciousness have no force unless they are grounded in the physiology of the brain, since the organization of dynamic systems, such as the Sun, could equally well qualify as conscious under such theories. In reviewing the wealth of studies of human consciousness since the mid-20th century, it concludes that many proposals for the particular neural substrate of consciousness are insufficient in various respects, but that the results can be integrated into a novel scheme that consciousness extends through a subcortical network of interlaminar structures from the brainstem to the claustrum. This interstitial structure has both the specificity and the extended connectivity to account for the array of reportable conscious experiences.

12.
Vision Res ; 181: 38-46, 2021 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33556821

RESUMO

Luminance contrast is one of the key factors in the visibility of objects in the world around us. Previous work has shown that the perceived depth from binocular disparity depends profoundly on the luminance contrast of the image. This dependence cannot be explained by existing disparity models, such as the well-established disparity energy model, because they predict no effect of luminance contrast on depth perception. Here, we develop a model for disparity processing that incorporates contrast normalization of the neural response into the disparity energy model to account for the contrast dependence of perceived depth from disparity. Our model contains an array of disparity channels, each with a different disparity selectivity. The binocular images are first processed by the left- and right-eye receptive fields of each channel. The outputs of the two receptive fields are combined linearly as the excitatory disparity sensitivity and then fed into a nonlinear contrast gain control mechanism. The perceived depth is determined by the weighted average of all the disparity channels that respond to the binocular images. This model provides the first analytic account of how luminance contrast affects perceived depth from disparity.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade , Visão Binocular , Humanos , Disparidade Visual
13.
Perception ; 50(2): 165-169, 2021 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33459168

RESUMO

Selective attention is well known for 2D patterns and perceptual alternations are well established for 3D structures projected into 2D, such as the Necker cube. Here, these concepts are extended to the spatial fourth dimension in the form of the mathematical structure of the 4D hypercube. In orthographic projection, its 2D outline figure has multiple and highly dynamic percepts of up to 28 different 3D interpretations, which correspond to local 3D views of the 4D hypercube. Thus, the spontaneous operations of perceptual processing can provide direct insight into conceptual structure in the fourth dimension.


Assuntos
Atenção , Humanos
14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35359581

RESUMO

We describe the development of a multipurpose haptic stimulus delivery and spatiomotor recording system with tactile map-overlays for electronic processing This innovative multipurpose spatiomotor capture system will serve a wide range of functions in the training and behavioral assessment of spatial memory and precise motor control for blindness rehabilitation, both for STEM learning and for navigation training and map reading. Capacitive coupling through the map-overlays to the touch-tablet screen below them allows precise recording i) of hand movements during haptic exploration of tactile raised-line images on one tablet and ii) of line-drawing trajectories on the other, for analysis of navigational errors, speed, time elapsed, etc. Thus, this system will provide for the first time in an integrated and automated manner quantitative assessments of the whole 'perception-cognition-action' loop - from non-visual exploration strategies, spatial memory, precise spatiomotor control and coordination, drawing performance, and navigation capabilities, as well as of haptic and movement planning and control. The accuracy of memory encoding, in particular, can be assessed by the memory-drawing operation of the capture system. Importantly, this system allows for both remote and in-person operation. Although the focus is on visually impaired populations, the system is designed to equally serve training and assessments in the normally sighted as well.

15.
Perception ; 49(12): 1265-1282, 2020 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33302775

RESUMO

In the time of the Renaissance, a major aspect of vision science was understanding how spatial information projected to the viewpoint of the observer, that is, visual perspective, which is one of the primary cues to depth perception. Perspective representation was thus an early form of virtual reality. Although accurate perspective representation was developed earlier in the 15th century, the first analytic perspective scheme was developed by Piero della Francesca, whose chef d'oeuvre is in the Church of San Francesco, Arezzo, in which the present lecture took place. The focus of the lecture was to evaluate some of the contributions of Piero della Francesca and his 15th-century contemporaries to the visual science, art and symbolism of his era, and its significance for the perception of depth structure from two-dimensional images.


Assuntos
Arte/história , Sinais (Psicologia) , Ciência/história , Visão Ocular , História do Século XV , História do Século XVI , Humanos , Itália
16.
Front Psychol ; 11: 1144, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32670141

RESUMO

This article develops a view of consciousness in the context of a new philosophical approach that invokes the concept of emergence, through which the operative principles of each level of organization of physical energy flow are functionally dissociated from those of the levels below it, despite the continuity of the physical laws that govern them. The particular form of emergence that is the focus of the present analysis is the emergence of conscious mental processing from neural activity carried by the underlying biochemical principles of brain organization. Within this framework, a process model of consciousness is developed to account for many of the experienced aspects of consciousness, many that are rarely considered in the philosophical discourse. Each of these aspects is rigorously specified in terms of its definable properties. It is then analyzed in terms of specific empirical tests that can be used to determine its neural substrate and relevant data that implement such tests. The article concludes with an analysis of the evolutionary function of consciousness, and a critique of the Integrated Information Theory approach to defining its properties.

17.
JAMA Ophthalmol ; 137(11): 1332-1333, 2019 Nov 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31513246
18.
J Vis ; 19(9): 11, 2019 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31434107

RESUMO

To investigate the effect of background noise on visual summation, we measured the contrast detection thresholds for targets with or without a white noise mask in luminance contrast. The targets were Gabor patterns placed at 3° eccentricity to either the left or right of the fixation and elongated along an arc of the same radius to ensure equidistance from fixation for every point along the long axis. The task was a spatial two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) paradigm in which the observer had to indicate whether the target was on the left or the right of the fixation. The threshold was measured at 75% accuracy with a staircase procedure. The detection threshold decreased with target length with slope -1/2 on log-log coordinates for target lengths between 30' and 300' half-height full-width (HHFW), defining a range of ideal matched-filter summation extending up to about 200' (or about 16× the center width of the Gabor targets). The summation curves for different noise contrasts were shifted copies of each other. For the threshold versus mask contrast (TvN) functions, the target threshold was constant for noise levels up to about -22 dB, then increased with noise contrast to a linear asymptote on log-log coordinates. Since the "elbow" of the target threshold versus noise function is an index of the level of the equivalent noise experienced by the visual system during target detection, our results suggest that the signal-to-noise ratio was invariant with target length. We further show that a linear-nonlinear-linear gain-control model can fully account for these results with far fewer parameters than a matched-filter model.


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Adulto , Humanos , Modelos Biológicos , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia
19.
Behav Res Methods ; 51(5): 2139-2151, 2019 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31313136

RESUMO

Eye movements are an important index of the neural functions of visual information processing, decision making, visuomotor coordination, sports performance, and so forth. However, the available optical tracking methods are impractical in many situations, such as the wearing of eyeglasses or the presence of ophthalmic disorders, and this can be overcome by accurate recording of eye movements by electrooculography (EOG). In this study we recorded eye movements by EOG simultaneously with high-density electroencephalogram (EEG) recording using a 128-channel EGI electrode net at a 500-Hz sampling rate, including appropriate facial electrodes. The participants made eye movements over a calibration target consisting of a 5×5 grid of stimulus targets. The results showed that the EOG methodology allowed accurate analysis of the amplitude and direction of the fixation locations and saccadic dynamics with a temporal resolution of 500 Hz, under both cued and uncued analysis regimes. Blink responses could be identified separately and were shown to have a more complex source derivation than has previously been recognized. The results also showed that the EOG signals recorded through the EEG net can achieve results as accurate as typical optical eye-tracking devices, and also allow for simultaneous assessment of neural activity during all types of eye movements. Moreover, the EOG method effectively avoids the technical difficulties related to eye-tracker positioning and the synchronization between EEG and eye movements. We showed that simultaneous EOG/EEG recording is a convenient means of measuring eye movements, with an accuracy comparable to that of many specialized eye-tracking systems.


Assuntos
Movimentos Sacádicos , Adulto , Piscadela , Calibragem , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Eletroculografia/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
20.
PLoS One ; 14(7): e0218663, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31314778

RESUMO

Earlier work by one of us examined a historical corpus of portraits and found that artists often paint the subject such that one eye is centred horizontally. If due to psychological mechanisms constraining artistic composition, this eye-centring bias should be detectable also in portraits by non-professionals. However, this finding has been questioned both on theoretical and empirical grounds. Here we tested eye-centring in a larger (N ~ = 4000) and more representative set of selfies spontaneously posted on Instagram from six world cities. In contrast with previous selfie results, the distribution of the most-centred eye position peaked almost exactly at the horizontal centre of the image and was statistically different from predictions based on realistic Monte-Carlo predictions. In addition, we observed a small but statistically reliable pseudoneglect effect as well as a preference for centring the left-eye. An eye-centring tendency appears to exist in self-portraits by non-artists.


Assuntos
Olho , Fotografação , Mídias Sociais , Viés , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...